Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:05:07.558Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are neurological and psychiatric disorders different?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Anthony S. David*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Timothy Nicholson
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Anthony S. David, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, DeCrespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

There have been recent calls to abandon the distinction between neurological and psychiatric disorders on philosophical and moral grounds. Crossley and colleagues, in this issue, meta-analyse published structural brain imaging data and prove that they are different after all – or do they?

Type
Editorials
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2015

Background

The study by Crossley and colleagues, Reference Crossley, Scott, Ellison-Wright and Mechelli1 in this issue of the BJPsych, is a rare and therefore welcome empirical contribution to the debate, recently revived, on whether the distinction between psychiatric and neurological disorders is real and/or should be abandoned. Reference White, Rickards and Zeman2 Psychiatric disorders are not just ‘mental’ but physical too.

The ‘reality’ of the psychiatry–neurology distinction, refers to the soundness of its theoretical basis. This might be best summarised by the following rule: if a disorder in question is reliably associated with a recognisable pathological process affecting the central nervous system (CNS), then it is neurological. But that of course just begs questions about what is meant by ‘reliably’? Does this mean ‘necessary and sufficient’ to cause the condition? What is ‘recognisable pathology’? Does this refer to the microscopic or macroscopic level? What then do we make of ‘quantitative change’ in the CNS in relation to some normative standard of a given magnitude? Is that ‘pathology’?

If we take schizophrenia, there is ample evidence of quantitative regional change in the CNS thanks to the widespread use of structural magnetic resonance imaging, but only insofar as this is detectable at the group level against a control group and according to some more or less arbitrary statistical threshold. Reference Wright, Rabe-Hesketh, Woodruff, David, Murray and Bullmore3 The same can be said of affective disorder but at a rather lower statistical threshold. So are these neurological disorders? Perhaps, yet we do not see much interest among our neurology colleagues to start seeing these patients or engaging in research on them. What then of Alzheimer's disease? Clearly a neurological disorder and of interest to a few neurologists, but ‘core business’ for the old age psychiatrist and a shared topic of interest for research. Hence, the discussion moves rapidly from philosophy (is the distinction real?), through biology (is there evidence of a pathological process?), to sociology, culture and prejudice (whose job is it? who gets the credit/blame?).

Crossley et al's meta-analysis

The paper by Crossley and colleagues takes a novel approach. Reference Crossley, Scott, Ellison-Wright and Mechelli1 It is a meta-analysis of studies using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to measure changes in grey matter in neurological and psychiatric diagnostic groups. So there are studies of diseases as diverse as multiple sclerosis and Huntington's disease, Asperger syndrome, anorexia and Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia and panic disorder, each comparing a patient group with controls and reporting statistical differences in, strictly speaking grey matter density used as a proxy for volume change, usually with, areas of reduced grey matter mapped onto a brain template. Importantly, these methods look at the whole brain and are not influenced by prior assumptions of where the abnormality might be. The authors applied some basic quality controls over the included studies and applied weightings according to the numbers of participants but were interested in the broad pattern that such large data-sets might reveal.

Now, you might question the whole enterprise. Is it not like going into a general hospital, picking patients from the cardiac ward, the renal unit and the orthopaedic department and sticking them in a scanner? Little surprise then if the results showed that abnormalities tended to centre on (although not being confined to) the heart, the kidney and the skeleton, respectively? But it is not quite the same since both psychiatry and neurology are laying claim to the same organ, the brain. Going along with the idea one might then hypothesise that neurological patients show the bulk of abnormalities in the areas of the brain associated with more ‘basic’ functions: movement, sensation and, with respect to dementia, memory, whereas psychiatry patients show differences in ‘higher’ brain regions associated with self-consciousness and identity (the frontal lobes) or emotion (the ‘limbic system’).

The results were more nuanced. First and foremost, there was a clear and statistically robust difference between the two classes of disorder. Second, the pattern was to some extent in line with predictions but with surprises. Summarising the data both quantitatively and qualitatively, the basal ganglia and insula clearly fell on the neurological side of the divide as did primary sensory and motor networks. However, so did the dorsal prefrontal ‘executive’ region, which might not have been envisaged. On the other hand the area that came out as clearly ‘psychiatric’ was the medial prefrontal region, an area with a growing reputation for functions concerning self-reflection and social cognition. Reference David, Bedford, Wiffen and Gilleen4 This region is part of the so-called ‘default mode network’, thought to be involved in awareness and stimulus-independent thought, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford5 which was generally less implicated in studies of neurology patients and includes attentional systems reaching back to the posterior cingulate cortex. An unexpected and frankly odd finding was that regions of the visual association cortex were significantly more allied with psychiatric disorders. These included the lingual gyrus, which may after all be genuinely important given its role in face perception. The temporal cortex was, perhaps predictably, the truly contested area – containing both the hippocampus (reflecting the arbitrariness of regarding dementia as neurological or psychiatric) and components of the limbic system, as well as being a key area of epilepsy pathology, such as mesial temporal sclerosis.

Context

This is not the first global look at grey matter across diagnoses. A recent meta-analysis of VBM studies with similar methods has just been published by Goodkind and colleagues, this time focusing just on psychiatric (Axis I) diagnoses, but including all published data rather than just representative samples. Reference Goodkind, Eickhoff, Oathes, Jiang, Chang and Jones-Hagata6 This study found reduced grey matter in both anterior cingulate and insula cortices to be common across all disorders, concluding these areas could represent a ‘shared neural substrate’ for mental illness. It is interesting to note that the insula in this study was also found to be a key psychiatric region, whereas it came out as associated with neurological disease in Crossley et al's study. Again, this ambiguity is perhaps not surprising given the large body of evidence linking the insula with emotion processing, motor function and – a true interface between mind and body – introception. Reference Craig7

There are a few limitations to get out of the way. The studies do not take into account medication. There is controversial evidence that antipsychotic medication can alter brain structure, for example increasing volume loss. Reference Fusar-Poli, Smieskova, Kempton, Ho, Andreasen and Borgwardt8 Somewhat less controversially, traditional antipsychotics have been found to cause increases in basal ganglia volumes. Reference Keshavan, Bagwell, Haas, Sweeney, Schooler and Pettegrew9 Hence, what we may be seeing here is the interaction between diagnosis and medication. Also, neither study was confined to grey matter and as psychiatric neuroimaging techniques advance and spread (for example encompassing more diffusion tensor imaging) we would expect more studies on white matter and connectivity to be published and to contribute to this debate. Reference Filippi, van den Heuvel, Fornito, He, Hulshoff Pol and Agosta10

Conclusions

The results may be taken as a slap in the face to the distinction-abolitionists, yet such individuals might take heart in them in that there is no implicit hierarchy in what emerges as the brain-based hallmark of neurological versus psychiatric conditions; they both involve the functionally interesting parts of the brain, it is just that they are, quite subtly, different.

Perhaps one lesson that we can all take from this novel and ingenious work is that we need to continue to use every ounce of our brains to answer the question of what makes a psychiatric disorder. As McHugh & Slavney Reference McHugh and Slavney11 wrote in their thoughtful The Perspectives of Psychiatry:

‘In the everyday world of the clinic, psychiatrists are distinguished from other medical specialists not because they are concerned with “minds” rather than “bodies”, but because they focus on complaints appearing in people's thoughts, perceptions, moods, and behaviours rather than their skins, bones, muscles and viscera … The diagnostic process may be difficult, but causal explanations are always complex and depend on the physician's capacity to evaluate issues ranging from intermediary metabolism (a “body” issue) to interpersonal misunderstanding (a “mind” issue). Psychiatric concerns thus extend from the ultrastructure of the body to the relationship of groups of minds within a social context.’

Footnotes

See pp. 429–434, this issue.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Crossley, NA, Scott, J, Ellison-Wright, I, Mechelli, A. Neuroimaging distinction between neurological and psychiatric disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2015; 207: 429–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 White, PD, Rickards, H, Zeman, AZ. Time to end the distinction between mental and neurological illnesses. BMJ 2012; 344: e3454.Google Scholar
3 Wright, IC, Rabe-Hesketh, S, Woodruff, PW, David, AS, Murray, RM, Bullmore, ET. Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157: 1625.Google Scholar
4 David, AS, Bedford, N, Wiffen, B, Gilleen, J. Failures of metacognition and lack of insight in neuropsychiatric disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2012; 367: 1379–90.Google Scholar
5 Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, Ford, JM. Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2012; 8: 4976.Google Scholar
6 Goodkind, M, Eickhoff, SB, Oathes, DJ, Jiang, Y, Chang, A, Jones-Hagata, LB, et al. Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry 2015; 72: 305–15.Google Scholar
7 Craig, AD. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 5970.Google Scholar
8 Fusar-Poli, P, Smieskova, R, Kempton, MJ, Ho, BC, Andreasen, NC, Borgwardt, S. Progressive brain changes in schizophrenia related to antipsychotic treatment? A meta-analysis of longitudinal MRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37: 1680–91.Google Scholar
9 Keshavan, MS, Bagwell, WW, Haas, GL, Sweeney, JA, Schooler, NR, Pettegrew, JW. Changes in caudate volume with neuroleptic treatment. Lancet 1994; 344: 1434.Google Scholar
10 Filippi, M, van den Heuvel, MP, Fornito, A, He, Y, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Agosta, F, et al. Assessment of system dysfunction in the brain through MRI-based connectomics. Lancet Neurol 2013; 12: 1189–99.Google Scholar
11 McHugh, PR, Slavney, PR. The Perspectives of Psychiatry. Johns Hopkins University Press, 1983.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.