Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:38:32.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sex differences in Alzheimer's disease risk: are we looking at the wrong hormones?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 August 2014

Cynthia A. Munro*
Affiliation:
Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Baltimore, MD, USA Email: [email protected]

Extract

Two-thirds of individuals with Alzheimer's disease (AD) are women, owing largely to the fact that women outlive men (https://www.alz.org/downloads/facts_figures_2012.pdf). Women's increased longevity, however, is not sufficient to explain the fact that women are 1.5 times more likely than men to develop the disease (Gao et al., 1998). After age 80, the incidence of AD is much higher in women than in men, such that the proportion of women with AD is almost twice the proportion of men with the disease (e.g., Zandi et al., 2002; Plassman et al., 2007). Moreover, once diagnosed with AD, women decline more rapidly, both cognitively and functionally, compared to men (Ito et al., 2011; Tschanz et al., 2011).

Type
Guest Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2014 

Two-thirds of individuals with Alzheimer's disease (AD) are women, owing largely to the fact that women outlive men (https://www.alz.org/downloads/facts_figures_2012.pdf). Women's increased longevity, however, is not sufficient to explain the fact that women are 1.5 times more likely than men to develop the disease (Gao et al., Reference Gao, Hendrie, Hall and Hui1998). After age 80, the incidence of AD is much higher in women than in men, such that the proportion of women with AD is almost twice the proportion of men with the disease (e.g., Zandi et al., Reference Zandi2002; Plassman et al., Reference Plassman2007). Moreover, once diagnosed with AD, women decline more rapidly, both cognitively and functionally, compared to men (Ito et al., Reference Ito2011; Tschanz et al., Reference Tschanz2011).

To explain women's increased risk for AD, and faster progression after onset, sex hormones—estrogens in particular—are often invoked. Numerous studies have established that age-related depletion of sex hormones increases the risk of AD, prompting researchers to hypothesize protective roles of these hormones against AD (see Vest & Pike, Reference Vest and Pike2013, for a review). Further support for the sex-hormone hypothesis came from a series of studies on the relation between fertility and AD. Based on the hypothesis that pregnancy-induced changes in estrogen levels would increase AD risk, this line of work has revealed that women with a greater number of pregnancies have a higher risk of developing AD and/or a younger age of onset (Sobow and Kloszewska, Reference Sobow and Kloszewska2004; Colucci et al., Reference Colucci2006). Even more persuasive is that having children increases the likelihood of developing AD in women but not in men (Colucci et al., Reference Colucci2006), and is positively correlated with AD neuropathology in women but not in men (Beeri, Reference Beeri2009). Furthermore, the association between parity and age of AD onset appears confined to women without the APOE4 allele, as it was not observed in women with the APOE4 allele in one study (Corbo et al., Reference Corbo2007), suggesting fertility is an independent risk factor for AD in women. Taken together, these findings provide perhaps the most compelling evidence for the sex hormone hypothesis of sex differences in risk for AD.

The notion that brief periods of altered sex hormone levels lead to the development of AD pathology a half century later, however, is not altogether convincing for several reasons. First, the notion that pregnancy induces long-term decreases in basal estrogen has been suggested to explain the delayed temporal association between pregnancy and AD. This explanation does not account for similar associations among parenthood, sex hormone levels, and risk of AD in men. Specifically, just as low estrogen levels increase the risk of AD in women, low testosterone is associated with an increased risk of AD in men. Moreover, men who become fathers evince a steeper decline in testosterone levels over time compared to men who remain childless (Gettler et al., Reference Gettler, McDade, Feranil and Kuzawa2011). Thus, if both motherhood and fatherhood are associated with decreased sex hormone levels, then fertility would be expected to increase the risk for AD in both sexes. But it does not. Second, the results of hormone replacement studies suggest strongly that hormone replacement increases AD risk, the opposite of what was predicted (e.g., Shumaker et al., Reference Shumaker2003). Most importantly, the line of work based on the sex hormone hypothesis has not led to treatments for AD, suggesting a need to consider alternative hypotheses linking female sex and AD risk. Findings from several lines of research implicate sex differences in the stress response as a promising candidate.

The stress response

In response to threat, a complex interaction among glands, hormones, and parts of the mid-brain ensues. In humans, stress triggers the release of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) from the para-ventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. CRH stimulates the pituitary gland to release adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH), which in turn causes the adrenal glands to release cortisol. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is thus a key component of the stress response system, aberrations of which are associated with various stress-related illnesses.

Prolonged stress-induced release of glucocorticoids leads to alterations in the hippocampus (Sapolsky, Reference Sapolsky1996; Gould et al., Reference Gould, Tanapat, McEwen, Flugge and Fuchs1998), including remodeling of dendrites (Gourley et al., Reference Gourley, Swanson and Koleske2013), reductions in long-term potentiation (Tadavarty et al., Reference Tadavarty, Kaan and Sastry2009; Kamal et al., Reference Kamal, Ramakers, Altinbilek and Kas2014) or brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; Bath et al., Reference Bath, Schilit and Lee2013), increases in markers of oxidative stress (as reviewed by Rothman and Mattson, Reference Rothman and Mattson2010), and reduced volume (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien1998). Stress also suppresses neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus (Gould et al., Reference Gould, McEwen, Tanapat, Galea and Fuchs1997, Reference Gould, Tanapat, McEwen, Flugge and Fuchs1998), an effect that increases with advancing age (Simon et al., Reference Simon, Czéh and Fuchs2005). Additionally, stress alters dendritic morphology of prefrontal cortical neurons (Radley et al., Reference Radley2004; Liston et al., Reference Liston2006). The functional consequences of these changes include cognitive impairment, particularly in memory (see Lupien et al., Reference Lupien2005, for a review), and also in executive functioning (Plessow et al., Reference Plessow, Kiesel and Kirschbaum2012).

Stress and AD

In rodents, stress provokes misprocessing of the amyloid precursor protein, leading to increased levels of Aβ40 and Aβ42 in the hippocampi (Martisova et al., Reference Martisova, Aisa, Guereñu and Ramírez2013), increases tau phosphorylation in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (Yang et al., Reference Yang2014), and accelerates cognitive impairment (Cuadrado-Tejedor et al., Reference Cuadrado-Tejedor, Ricobaraza, Frechilla, Franco, Pérez-Mediavilla and Garcia-Osta2012). Interestingly, these effects are observed in only “stress-sensitive” (rather than “stress-resistant”) animals (Briones et al., Reference Briones2012), suggesting that the apparent AD-inducing effects of stress are not inevitable, but require a particular vulnerability to the effects of stress.

In patients with AD, both plasma and cerebrospinal fluid contain increased cortisol, the level of which is positively correlated with the degree of cognitive impairment (see, e.g., Dong and Csernansky, Reference Dong and Csernansky2009), but unrelated to symptoms of depression (Hoogendijk et al., Reference Hoogendijk, Meynen, Endert, Hofman and Swaab2006). Longitudinal studies have found that experiencing major stressful life events is associated with younger age of onset in familial AD (Mejia et al., Reference Mejía, Giraldo, Pineda, Ardila and Lopera2003). Furthermore, death of a spouse more than doubles the risk of AD in those who never remarry, a risk that is further increased in individuals who carry at least one APOE4 allele (Håkansson, Reference Håkansson2009).

Sex differences in the stress response

Cortisol

Both preclinical and human studies show sex differences in the cortisol response to stress. In rodents, stress induces a greater cortisol response in males compared to females (Beck and Luine, Reference Beck and Luine2002; Luine, Reference Luine2002; Bowman et al., Reference Bowman, Beck and Luine2003). Findings in humans are less consistent, as they are more dependent on factors such as type of stressor, age of subjects, and the timing of hormone measures (see Kudielka and Kirschbaum, Reference Kudielka and Kirschbaum2005, for a review).

In studies examining the effects of stress on cognition, the degree of cortisol response to stress, rather than the mere experience of it, better predicts the cognitive effects of stress (Wolf et al., Reference Wolf, Schommer, Hellhammer, McEwen and Kirschbaum2001; Takahashi et al., Reference Takahashi2004). Whereas findings in young adults do not consistently favor men or women, advancing age appears to place women at a disadvantage. In a meta-analysis of 45 studies, Otte and colleagues (Reference Otte, Hart, Neylan, Marmar, Yaffe and Mohr2005) found that the effect of age on the cortisol response to a pharmacological or psychological stressor was almost three times higher in women than in men. Importantly, the effect sizes of studies that controlled for sex hormone variations in women (e.g., standardizing menstrual cycles, excluding women on oral contraceptives or hormone replacement therapy) did not differ from those that did not, suggesting that sex hormones do not alter the effect of aging on the stress response in women. In line with this finding, studies examining the effect of stress on cognition in older men and women find that an acute psychosocial stressor causes memory impairment in women only (Wolf et al., Reference Wolf, Kudielka, Hellhammer, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum1998; Almela et al., Reference Almela2011).

BDNF

In addition to cortisol, the association between stress and BDNF is another mechanism by which women may be more vulnerable than men to AD. In mice, stress reduces hippocampal BDNF in females but not in males (Yamaura et al., Reference Yamaura, Ishiwatari, Oishi, Fukata and Ueno2013). In rats exposed to stress, females show greater stress hormone (corticosterone) response, less cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus, and lower levels of hippocampal BDNF than males (Malheiros et al., Reference Malheiros2014). In non-human primates, stress alters plasma BDNF in females but not males (Cirulli et al., Reference Cirulli2009).

In humans, a cross-continent meta-analysis found that the Met66 polymorphism of the BDNF gene, shown to reduce the transport of BDNF, conferred an increased risk of AD in women but not in men (Fukumoto et al., Reference Fukumoto2010). While the role of estrogen in BDNF expression was postulated to underlie this finding, it is notable that BDNF is correlated with cortisol (Begluiomini et al., Reference Begliuomini2008). In a study of young adults, women with the Met66 (compared to the Val/Val) BDNF polymorphism had an increased cortisol response to a social stressor, whereas the same polymorphism was associated with a decreased cortisol response in men (Shalev et al., Reference Shalev2009). Taken together, these findings underscore the potential importance of stress hormones in explaining women's increased risk of AD.

Sex differences in stress vulnerability

In a 35-year longitudinal study of women, Johansson and colleagues (Reference Johansson2010) found that reports of “frequent/chronic” stress during mid-life increased the risk of AD at follow-up. Similarly, a recent meta-analysis of five prospective studies (Terraccino et al., Reference Terracciano2014) found that individuals in the top quartile of distress proneness (high scores on neuroticism) had a three-fold risk of AD. Sex differences in neuroticism are consistently found across the world, with women scoring higher than men (Lynn and Martin, Reference Lynn and Martin1997). The magnitude of the sex difference, slightly greater than one-half of a standard deviation, is almost identical in young and older adults (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Terracciano and McCrae2001; Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Duberstein, Sörensen and Lyness2007). These findings correspond to those from animal studies, and suggest that “stress-sensitive” individuals are at increased risk for AD, and also that women are more vulnerable than men to this particular risk.

Sex differences in major stressful life events

Given the links between stress and risk of AD, it is notable that one of life's most stressful events—death of a spouse—is much more common in women than in men. Not only do women marry men who average two years their senior (Copen et al., Reference Copen, Daniels, Vespa and Mosher2012), they also live longer than men (81 vs. 76 years; Population Reference Bureau 2013 World Population Data Sheet). For these reasons, the sex difference in the proportion of individuals experiencing widowhood increases with age. From ages 75–84, fewer than 20% of men, but over half of women (54%) have lost a spouse to death. After age 85, 35% of men and 72% of women have been widowed (U.S. Census Bureau, 2006). If stress can induce the development of AD pathology or hasten its clinical manifestation in both sexes, then women would be at greater risk for AD simply by virtue of being much more likely than men to experience death of a spouse, even if there were no sex differences in the stress response. In this sense, the higher incidence of AD in women after age 80 could, indeed, be explained by the fact that they outlive men.

Pregnancy and stress hormones

Considering the studies reviewed above, it is intriguing to consider whether findings from the fertility studies might lend themselves to re-interpretation in the context of stress, rather than sex, hormones. During pregnancy, the adrenal glands become hypertrophic, and basal cortisol levels rise. By the third trimester, cortisol increases to two to three times higher than in the non-gestational period (Dörr et al., Reference Dörr1989), similar to the levels seen in Cushing's syndrome (Magiakou et al., Reference Magiakou, Mastorakos, Webster and Chrousos1997). At the same time, women's cortisol response to acute stress is attenuated (Kammerer et al., Reference Kammerer, Adams, von Castelberg and Glover2002). That repeated states of pregnancy-induced alterations in the stress response might lead to enduring perturbations of the HPA axis in a manner that increases the risk for AD is a tempting hypothesis. Indeed, multiparity reveals alterations in both diurnal cortisol and cortisol response to stress that are not evident in primiparous mothers (Tu et al., Reference Tu, Lupien and Walker2006a; Reference Tu, Lupien and Walker2006b).

Conclusion

Efforts to elucidate the mechanisms underlying sex differences in risk of AD have been dominated by research on sex hormones. While this work has done much to increase our knowledge of hormonal risk factors for AD, it has not led to effective treatments. As depicted in the Figure, the preponderance of the evidence reviewed herein convincingly argues that stress hormones should be afforded their rightful place in the lineup of suspects underlying women's increased vulnerability to AD.

Figure 1. Influence of stress on the development of AD. Factors for which women show greater vulnerability than men are shaded in red. *Increased vulnerability in women considers that pregnancy is a stressful event.

Conflict of interest

None

References

Almela, M. et al. (2011). Salivary alpha-amylase response to acute psychosocial stress: the impact of age. Biological Psychology, 87, 421429. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.008. https://www.alz.org/downloads/facts_figures_2012.pdf.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bath, K. G., Schilit, A. and Lee, F. S. (2013). Stress effects on BDNF expression: effects of age, sex, and form of stress. Journal of Neuroscience, 239, 149156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, K. D. and Luine, V. N. (2002). Sex differences in behavioral and neurochemical profiles after chronic stress: role of housing conditions. Physiology and Behavior, 75, 661673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beeri, M. S. (2009). Number of children is associated with neuropathology of Alzheimer's disease in women. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 11841191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begliuomini, S. et al. (2008). Plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor daily variations in men: correlation with cortisol circadian rhythm. Journal of Endocrinology, 197, 429435. doi: 10.1677/JOE-07-0376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, R. E., Beck, K. D. and Luine, V. N. (2003). Chronic stress effects on memory. Sex differences in performance and monoamines. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 4859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briones, A. et al. (2012). Stress-induced anhedonia is associated with an increase in Alzheimer's disease-related markers. British Journal of Pharmacology, 165, 897907. doi: 10.1111/j.1476–5381.2011.01602.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, B. P., Duberstein, P. R., Sörensen, S. and Lyness, J. M. (2007). Gender differences in five factor model personality traits in an elderly cohort. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 15941603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cirulli, F. et al. (2009). Changes in plasma levels of BDNF and NGF reveal a gender-selective vulnerability to early adversity in rhesus macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 172180. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colucci, M. et al. (2006). The number of pregnancies is a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease. European Journal of Neurology, 13, 1374–1347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copen, C. E., Daniels, K., Vespa, J. and Mosher, W. D. (2012). First marriages in the United States: data from the 2006–2010 National Survey of Family Growth. National Health Statistics Reports; no 49. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.Google Scholar
Corbo, R. M. et al. (2007). Combined effect of apolipoprotein e genotype and past fertility on age at onset of Alzheimer's disease in women. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 24, 8285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P., Terracciano, A. and McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuadrado-Tejedor, M., Ricobaraza, A., Frechilla, D., Franco, R., Pérez-Mediavilla, A. and Garcia-Osta, A. (2012). Chronic mild stress accelerates the onset and progression of the Alzheimer's disease phenotype in Tg2576 mice. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 28:567578. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, H. and Csernansky, J. G. (2009). Chronic mild stress accelerates the onset and progression of the Alzheimer's disease phenotype in Tg2576 mice. Effects of stress and stress hormones on amyloid-β protein and plaque deposition. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 18, 459469. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1152 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörr, H. G. et al. (1989). Longitudinal study of progestins, mineralocorticoids, and glucocorticoids throughout human pregnancy. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 68, 863868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukumoto, N. et al. (2010). Sexually dimorphic effect of the Val66Met polymorphism of BDNF on susceptibility to Alzheimer's disease: new data and meta-analysis. American Journal of Medical Genetics B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 153B:235242. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, S., Hendrie, H. C., Hall, K. S. and Hui, S. (1998). The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 809815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Feranil, A. B. and Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 108, 1619416199. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105403108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, E., McEwen, B. S., Tanapat, P., Galea, L. A. M. and Fuchs, E. (1997). Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress and NMDA receptor activation. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 24922498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, E., Tanapat, P., McEwen, B. S., Flugge, G. and Fuchs, E. (1998) Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 3168–3171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gourley, S. L., Swanson, A. M. and Koleske, A. J. (2013). Corticosteroid-induced neural remodeling predicts behavioral vulnerability and resilience. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 31073112. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2138-12.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Håkansson, K. et al. (2009). Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: population based cohort study. BMJ, 339, b2462. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoogendijk, W. J., Meynen, G., Endert, E., Hofman, M. A. and Swaab, D. F. (2006). Increased cerebrospinal fluid cortisol level in Alzheimer's disease is not related to depression. Neurobiology of Aging, 27, 780.e1780.e2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ito, K. et al. (2011). Disease progression model for cognitive deterioration from Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative database. Alzheimer's and Dementia, 7, 151160. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.03.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, L. et al. (2010). Midlife psychological stress and risk of dementia: a 35-year longitudinal population study. Brain, 133, 22172224. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamal, A., Ramakers, G. M., Altinbilek, B. and Kas, M. J. (2014). Social isolation stress reduces hippocampal long-term potentiation: effect of animal strain and involvement of glucocorticoid receptors. Journal of Neuroscience, 256, 262270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.10.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kammerer, M., Adams, D., von Castelberg, B. and Glover, V. (2002). Pregnant women become insensitive to cold stress. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 2, 8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudielka, B. M. and Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review. Biological Psychology, 69, 113132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liston, C. et al. (2006). Stress-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology predict selective impairments in perceptual attentional set-shifting. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 78707874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luine, V. N. (2002). Sex differences in chronic stress effects on memory in rats. Stress, 5, 205216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J. et al. (1998). Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 6973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J. et al. (2005). Stress hormones and human memory function across the lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 225242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynn, R. and Martin, T. (1997). Gender differences in extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism in 37 nations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 369373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magiakou, M. A., Mastorakos, G., Webster, E. and Chrousos, G. P. (1997). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the female reproductive system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 816, 4256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malheiros, J. M. et al. (2014). Repetitive noxious neonatal stimuli increases dentate gyrus cell proliferation and hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels. Hippocampus, 24, 415423. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martisova, E., Aisa, B., Guereñu, G. and Ramírez, M. J. (2013). Effects of early maternal separation on biobehavioral and neuropathological markers of Alzheimer's disease in adult male rats. Current Alzheimer Research, 10, 420432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mejía, S., Giraldo, M., Pineda, D., Ardila, A. and Lopera, F. (2003). Nongenetic factors as modifiers of the age of onset of familial Alzheimer's disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 15, 337349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otte, C., Hart, S., Neylan, T. C., Marmar, C. R., Yaffe, K. and Mohr, D. C. (2005). A meta-analysis of cortisol response to challenge in human aging: importance of gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 8091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plassman, B. L. et al. (2007). Prevalence of dementia in the United States: the aging, demographics, and memory study. Neuroepidemiology, 29, 125132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plessow, F., Kiesel, A. and Kirschbaum, C. (2012). The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals. Experimental Brain Research, 216, 397408. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2943-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Population Reference Bureau 2013 World Population Data Sheet, www.prb.org.Google Scholar
Radley, J. J. et al. (2004). Chronic behavioral stress induces apical dendritic reorganization in pyramidal neurons of the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience, 125, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothman, S. M. and Mattson, M. P. (2010). Adverse stress, hippocampal networks, and Alzheimer's disease. Neuromolecular Medicine, 12, 5670. doi: 10.1007/s12017-009-8107-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M. (1996). Stress, glucocorticoids, and damage to the nervous system: the current state of confusion. Stress, 1, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shalev, I. et al. (2009). BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with HPA axis reactivity to psychological stress characterized by genotype and gender interactions. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 382388. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shumaker, S. A. et al. (2003). Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women. The Women's Health Initiative Memory Study: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 289, 26512662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, M., Czéh, B. and Fuchs, E. (2005). Age-dependent susceptibility of adult hippocampal cell proliferation to chronic psychosocial stress. Brain Research, 1049, 244248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobow, T. and Kloszewska, I. (2004). Parity, number of pregnancies, and the age of onset of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 16, 120121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tadavarty, R., Kaan, T. K. and Sastry, B. R. (2009). Long-term depression of excitatory synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal CA1 neurons following sleep-deprivation. Experimental Neurology, 216, 239242. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.11.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takahashi, T. et al. (2004). Social stress-induced cortisol elevation acutely impairs social memory in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 363:125130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terracciano, A. et al. (2014). Personality and risk of Alzheimer's disease: new data and meta-analysis. Alzheimer's and Dementia, 10, 179186. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.03.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tschanz, J. T. et al. (2011). Progression of cognitive, functional, and neuropsychiatric symptom domains in a population cohort with Alzheimer dementia: the cache county dementia progression study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19, 532542. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181faec23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tu, M. T., Lupien, S. J. and Walker, C. D. (2006a). Multiparity reveals the blunting effect of breastfeeding on physiological reactivity to psychological stress. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 18, 494503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tu, M. T., Lupien, S. J. and Walker, C. D. (2006b). Diurnal salivary cortisol levels in postpartum mothers as a function of infant feeding choice and parity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 812824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vest, R. S. and Pike, C. J. (2013). Gender, sex steroid hormones, and Alzheimer's disease. Hormones and Behavior, 63, 301307. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.04.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, G. et al. (2014). Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor and the risk for dementia: the Framingham Heart Study. JAMA Neurology, 71, 5561. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, O. T., Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., Hellhammer, J. and Kirschbaum, C. (1998). Opposing effects of DHEA replacement in elderly subjects on declarative memory and attention after exposure to a laboratory stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 617629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, O. T., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., McEwen, B. S. and Kirschbaum, C. (2001). The relationship between stress-induced cortisol levels and memory differs between men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26, 711720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaura, K., Ishiwatari, Y. B. M., Oishi, N., Fukata, H. and Ueno, K. (2013). Sex differences in stress reactivity of hippocampal BDNF in mice are associated with the female preponderance of decreased locomotor activity in response to restraint stress. Zoological Science, 30, 10191024. doi: 10.2108/zsj.30.1019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. et al. (2014). Changes in tau phosphorylation levels in the hippocampus and frontal cortex following chronic stress. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 47, 237244. doi: 10.1590/1414-431X20133275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandi, P. P. et al. (2002). Hormone replacement therapy and incidence of Alzheimer disease in older women: the Cache County Study. JAMA, 288, 21232139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Influence of stress on the development of AD. Factors for which women show greater vulnerability than men are shaded in red. *Increased vulnerability in women considers that pregnancy is a stressful event.