It has been known for over two decades that the life expectancy of many groups of people with mental illness is at least 20% less than for the population as a whole in high-income countries. Reference Newman and Bland1 Is this state of neglect improving? The remarkable paper by Wahlbeck and colleagues in this issue brings pessimistic news. Reference Wahlbeck, Westman, Nordentoft, Gissler and Munk Laursen2 Even in three Scandinavian countries that provide among the best-quality and most equitably distributed healthcare in the world, this mortality gap has narrowed only by a modest extent over the past two decades and remains stubbornly wide. Why is this the case and what needs to be done?
These extraordinarily higher death rates reflect a combination of: (a) a higher occurrence of risk factors for many chronic diseases and some types of cancer; (b) the iatrogenic effects of some psychiatric medications; (c) higher rates of suicide, accidental and violent death; and (d) poorer access to physical healthcare than for the population as a whole.
Several lifestyle factors adversely affect the physical health of people with mental illness, for example relatively low rates of exercise along with higher rates of obesity. These combine with relatively high rates of smoking and worse diet Reference McCreadie3 to contribute further to higher rates of hypertension, high plasma cholesterol and triglycerides, diabetes and obesity. Reference Leucht, Burkard, Henderson, Maj and Sartorius4 Yet the ‘ecological fallacy’ needs to be acknowledged, as the associations so clearly demonstrated in the paper by Wahlbeck and colleagues do not necessarily have aetiological importance, although they do generate clear hypotheses to be tested in future intervention studies (where the ecological fallacy means that the individual members of a group are incorrectly assumed to have the average characteristics of the group at large).
In addition, rates of ‘unnatural deaths’ are unnaturally high. Only 80% of people with schizophrenia die from natural causes, for example, compared with 97% of the general population. The higher rates of these deaths are largely attributable to accidents and suicide, which tends to occur more often in early than late adulthood, and the excess mortality rates identified among younger adults merit particular attention. Reference Inskip, Harris and Barraclough5,Reference Tiihonen, Lonnqvist, Wahlbeck, Klaukka, Niskanen and Tanskanen6
People with mental illness are also less likely to receive effective screening for cancer and have higher case-fatality rates. This is partly due to the particular challenges when treating these patients including medical comorbidity, drug interactions, lack of capacity and difficulties in coping with the treatment as a result of psychiatric symptoms. Reference Howard, Barley, Davies, Rigg, Lempp and Rose7 But more generally there is now strong evidence that people with mental illness receive worse treatment for physical disorders (‘diagnostic overshadowing’). This takes place because general healthcare staff are poorly informed or mis-attribute physical symptoms to a mental disorder. For example, after adjusting for other risk factors, such as cardiovascular risk factors and socioeconomic status, depression in men was found to be associated with an increase in cardiovascular-related mortality. Reference Desai, Rosenheck, Druss and Perlin8
It seems clear, therefore, that medical staff, guided by negative stereotypes, tend to systematically treat the physical illnesses of people with mental illness less thoroughly and less effectively. For example, people with comorbid mental illness and diabetes who presented to an emergency department, were less likely to be admitted to hospital for diabetic complications than those with no mental illness. Reference Sullivan, Han, Moore and Kotrla9 It is clear that such consistent patterns of less access to effective physical healthcare can be considered as a form of structural discrimination. Reference Thornicroft, Brohan, Rose and Sartorius10
If such a disparity in mortality rates were to affect a large segment of the population with a less stigmatised characteristic, then we would witness an outcry against a socially unacceptable decimation of this group. The fact that life expectancy remains about 20 years less for men with mental illness, and 15 years less for women with mental illness denotes a cynical disregard for these lost lives, and shows, in stark terms, by just how much people with mental illness are categorically valued less than others in our society. This can justifiably be seen as a violation of the ‘right to health’ as set out in Article 12 ‘The right to the highest attainable standard of health’ of the International Covenant on Economic, Social and Cultural Rights. 11 Further, in 2006, the United Nations General Assembly adopted the Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities that explicitly applies to people with mental health problems as well as people with intellectual disabilities. The Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities defines the protections and entitlements of the 650 million people with disabilities worldwide. In relation to the current violations of these legitimate expectations to equivalent years of life, Wahlbeck et al are correct to conclude that their results imply a ‘failure of social policy and health promotion, illness prevention and care provision’.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.