Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T21:30:21.312Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Letter to the editor: social influences on the relationship between dissociation and psychotic-like experience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2024

Hisao Toyoshima*
Affiliation:
Pharmacy Management Institute, Graduate School of Business, Japan University of Economics, Shibuya-ku, Tokyo, Japan
Tomohide Akase
Affiliation:
Pharmacy Management Institute, Graduate School of Business, Japan University of Economics, Shibuya-ku, Tokyo, Japan Graduate School of Business, Japan University of Economics, Shibuya-ku, Tokyo, Japan
*
Corresponding author: Hisao Toyoshima; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Letter to the Editor
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

We read with great interest the paper by Charles Heriot-Maitland, Til Wykes, and Emmanuelle Peters titled ‘Social influences on the relationship between dissociation and psychotic-like experiences’ (Psychological Medicine, 1–9, 2024). In this study, social factors were extracted from experiences such as feeling humiliated by others and feelings of safety and connection with others. However, we believe there are several limitations and issues that warrant attention. Here are some examples.

  1. 1. The conclusion that ‘two factors (shame and psychological safety) may be involved in psychotic-like experiences’ may be overly definitive. Reports indicate that the onset of psychotic-like experiences (PLE) involves factors beyond social influences such as shame and low psychological safety environments. These include a history of psychotic disorders (including medication use [Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013]), sleep (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Sheaves, Goodwin, Yu, Nickless, Harrison and Espie2017), genetic factors (Legge et al., Reference Legge, Jones, Kendall, Pardiñas, Menzies, Bracher-Smith and Walters2019), unemployment, leave of absence (Stilo et al., Reference Stilo, Di Forti, Mondelli, Falcone, Russo, O'Connor and Morgan2013), economic difficulties, and cultural factors (Zandi et al., Reference Zandi, Havenaar, Smits, Limburg-Okken, van der Velden, Kahn and van den Brink2010). Without conducting a survey that considers these factors, it is difficult to conclusively determine the involvement of only shame and psychological safety. Therefore, further examination is necessary, including the possibility that these factors may be confounding variables.

  2. 2. In the examination of models A, B, and C, the temporal order of shame's influence on dissociation is unclear. The study design does not allow for distinguishing whether shame preceded and influenced dissociation, or if shame resulted from dissociation. A more detailed explanation of this point is required.

  3. 3. The measurement periods for each score are not uniform, potentially affecting the quality and reliability of the data. PLE were measured over 1 week, the period for the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) is unclear, the Other as Shamer scale (OAS) over 2 weeks, and the Social Safeness and Pleasure Scale (SSPS) over 1 week. This variability in measurement periods may increase respondent bias, necessitating caution in generalizing results to all subjects (Stull, Leidy, Parasuraman, & Chassany, Reference Stull, Leidy, Parasuraman and Chassany2009).

  4. 4. While the study uses a 6-month follow-up for PLE in the longitudinal study, reviews suggest that at least 12 months of follow-up is necessary for proper evaluation (Alvarez-Jimenez et al., Reference Alvarez-Jimenez, Priede, Hetrick, Bendall, Killackey, Parker and Gleeson2012). Therefore, medium- and long-term examinations are needed.

  5. 5. While sampling from the general population is useful, attention needs to be paid to cultural sensitivity, cross-country comparisons, consideration of cultural contexts, and the use of mixed research methods (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013).

Considering these points, we suggest that the authors make the following additions to points 1–5:

For point 1, provide detailed explanations of the subjects’ history of psychotic disorders (including medication use), sleep status, genetic factors, employment status, economic situation, and cultural background. If these factors were excluded, present the results of their impact assessment.

For point 2, provide a detailed explanation of the temporal relationship between dissociation and shame in the model.

For point 3, acknowledge the implications of different time scales for each measurement scale and specify precautions for accurately assessing emotional states and their duration. Consider using scales that evaluate a 1-week period, such as the Experience of Shame Scale (ESS).

For point 4, extend the follow period to 12–24 months or longer and increase measurement points to more appropriately assess the long-term course of PLE and their association with social factors. Modify the conclusion to state, ‘The 6-month observation suggested a possible association between social factors and PLE reporting, but further comprehensive research is needed to elucidate long-term stability and causal relationships’, to appropriately convey the study's limitations and significance of findings. For point 5, consider using data from both general population and clinical samples, as it has been suggested that this approach allows for a more comprehensive understanding of the process of psychosis onset, thereby deepening the significance of the findings.

We believe that these responses improved the quality and validity of this study. Thank you for considering our manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the use of an AI language model (Claude, Anthropic) for English language editing assistance. The authors are entirely responsible for the content of the manuscript.

Author contributions

H. T. wrote the draft, and T. A. revised and approved the final version. The authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding statement

This study received no funding.

Competing interests

None.

References

Alvarez-Jimenez, M., Priede, A., Hetrick, S. E., Bendall, S., Killackey, E., Parker, A. G., … Gleeson, J. F. (2012). Risk factors for relapse following treatment for first episode psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Schizophrenia Research, 139(1–3), 116128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.05.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Sheaves, B., Goodwin, G. M., Yu, L. M., Nickless, A., Harrison, P. J., … Espie, C. A. (2017). The effects of improving sleep on mental health (OASIS): A randomised controlled trial with mediation analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(10), 749758. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30328-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legge, S. E., Jones, H. J., Kendall, K. M., Pardiñas, A. F., Menzies, G., Bracher-Smith, M., … Walters, J. T. R. (2019). Association of genetic liability to psychotic experiences with neuropsychotic disorders and traits. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(12), 12561265. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linscott, R. J., & van Os, J. (2013). An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: On the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Medicine, 43(6), 11331149. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712001626CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stilo, S. A., Di Forti, M., Mondelli, V., Falcone, A. M., Russo, M., O'Connor, J., … Morgan, C. (2013). Social disadvantage: Cause or consequence of impending psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(6), 12881295. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stull, D. E., Leidy, N. K., Parasuraman, B., & Chassany, O. (2009). Optimal recall periods for patient-reported outcomes: Challenges and potential solutions. Current Medical Research and Opinion, 25(4), 929942. https://doi.org/10.1185/03007990902774765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandi, T., Havenaar, J. W., Smits, W. A. F., Limburg-Okken, A. G., van der Velden, H., Kahn, W., & van den Brink, R. (2010). First contact incidence of psychotic disorders among native Dutch and Moroccan immigrants in the Netherlands: Influence of diagnostic bias. Schizophrenia Research, 119(1–3), 2733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.02.1059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed