Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T16:26:12.988Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurobiological and epigenetic perspectives on hedonism, altruism and conscience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2022

Prasanna N. De Silva*
Affiliation:
Old age psychiatrist with Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Trust, based at Monkwearmouth Hospital, Sunderland, UK. He has previously published on the dopamine reward system and on the pathophysiology of synaptic pruning.
*
Correspondence Dr Prasanna N. De Silva. Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

This article examines notions of hedonism, altruism and conscience in relation to the activity of four neurotransmitter pathways: the dopamine reward, noradrenaline fight or flight, serotonin calming and glutamine learning pathways. Associated brain areas that modulate behaviour are highlighted: the prefrontal cortex (activity planning, risk mitigation), the hippocampus (memory retrieval) and the insular cortex (integration of information to decide on action). Putative epigenetic changes influencing adult behaviours after childhood privation are discussed. Pharmacological and psychological means of mitigating harmful behaviours are summarised, alongside the ethics of epigenetic screening to predict future addictive and violent tendencies.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

LEARNING OBJECTIVES

After reading this article you will be able to:

  • understand the neurobiology of conscience and how hedonism and altruism are mediated through the dopamine reward pathway

  • recognise the impact of childhood privation on adult behaviour through epigenetic modifications

  • understand current approaches to mitigate damaging hedonistic impulses, including lessons learned from prolonged dopamine agonist use.

Hedonistic behaviours potentially causing harm to self and to others have been described in various religious and secular texts, such as the Ten Commandments (Skeel Reference Skeel and Longman2011), the Buddhist path to enlightenment (Rahula Reference Rahula1959) and contemporary secular versions pertaining to academic life (Franke Reference Franke2013). These include suppressing impulses to kill, steal, speak untruths, use intoxicating substances and become involved in sexual misdemeanours (Table 1).

TABLE 1 Summary of three ethical codes, with possible contemporary applications

The notion of an inborn tendency to hedonism (‘original sin’ in Christian theology) suggests potential for these behaviours from childhood. The restraining influence, one's ‘conscience’, is also considered to be innate, albeit seen as a gradually maturing facility (Timpe Reference Timpe and Zalta2021).

The dopamine reward pathway

The brain's reward pathway is based on the neurotransmitter dopamine, which is produced at the substantia nigra (Koob Reference Koob and Volkow2010) (Fig. 1). On receipt of sensory stimuli via the thalamus, dopamine is transported to the prefrontal areas of the brain to motivate activity. Dopamine is also directed at the mesolimbic system in the mid-brain, comprising the ventral tegmental area and the nucleus accumbens. This joint structure experiences reward as a ‘thrill’, with the hippocampus recording the stimuli and response for future use.

Repetitive thrill-seeking behaviour can lead to both tolerance – needing more intense stimuli – and dependence – needing escalating frequency of activity, with features of withdrawal if discontinued (Miller Reference Miller, Dackis and Gold1987). There is increasing evidence that humans can move from one reward-inducing behaviour to another if the original activity is thwarted: cross-sensitisation between sweetened drinks, alcoholic beverages and stimulants, for example (de Silva Reference de Silva2019).

Altruistic thoughts and actions also produce a dopamine-induced reward experience (Sonne Reference Sonne and Gash2018), leading to repetition of such behaviours. Swapping between different altruistic acts can also occur, with the possibility of addiction to them, as can happen in blood donation, for example (Piliavin Reference Piliavin, Callero and Evans1982).

The adrenergic ‘fight, freeze or flight’ system

The main neurochemical pathway associated with ‘fight, freeze or flight’ is the adrenergic pathway (McCorry Reference McCorry2007). The source chemical noradrenaline is produced in the locus ceruleus in the brain-stem and supplied to the motor areas (frontal and parietal cortices) as well as to the amygdala, which is situated in front of the hippocampus.

Amygdala activation causes a sensation of fear, with the prefrontal cortex assisting the insula to decide whether to fight, freeze or escape. The fight response can secondarily release dopamine and be perceived as a thrill, habituating aggressive behaviours.

The serotonergic ‘calming’ system

The serotonergic (5-HT) system can downregulate both dopamine and noradrenaline (Lowry Reference Lowry, Johnson and Hay-Schmit2005). Over 80% of 5-HT is produced by the gut microbiota, although a local supply is produced by the raphe nucleus of the brain-stem, and it is distributed to prefrontal and limbic areas. The vagus nerve, which links the gut, heart and brain-stem, also modulates the adrenergic and dopaminergic supply.

Learning via glutamate and synaptic pruning

Glutamate is available from embryonic life onwards, prior to the formation of dopamine and noradrenaline pathways. Unlike dopamine, noradrenaline and 5-HT, glutamate is locally sourced through the central nervous system, mainly in astrocytes which surround and support neurons (Meldrum Reference Meldrum2000). It reinforces synapses devoted to recalling behaviour appropriate to a stimulus (Riedel Reference Riedel, Platt and Micheau2003). Repeated practice of behaviours is also accommodated via glutamate to ensure efficiency and lack of errors in application.

Assisting glutamate to hardwire learning, synaptic pruning is carried out by microglia phagocytosing unused synapses (Faust Reference Faust, Gunner and Schafer2021). Synaptic pruning typically occurs at particular periods: a whole brain prune between the ages of 8 and 11 (learning to avoid physical injury) and a selective prefrontal prune between the ages of 18 and 25 (learning to live and work collaboratively).

In keeping with the presence of glutamate from embryonic life, it appears that ‘caring behaviours’ are learned and hardwired during the first 2 years of life in reaction to parental nurturing (Upshaw Reference Upshaw, Kaiser and Sommerville2015), with neglect associated with more harmful behaviours in later life (Bland Reference Bland and Lambie2018).

Behavioural epigenetics

Evidence of early childhood privation leading to hedonistic and antisocial behaviours in adult life was raised over 30 years ago (Farringdon Reference Farringdon1993). Animal studies have shown that epigenetic mechanisms, specifically gene silencing via DNA methylation, provide a link between childhood neglect and later life behaviours: silencing at the glucocorticoid receptor gene appears to alter the cortisol responsiveness to stress in later life (Weaver Reference Weaver, Cervoni and Champagne2004).

The other area of epigenetic interest is intergenerational transmission of addictive behaviours (Vassoler Reference Vassoler and Sadri-Vakili2014), with the potential for altering the epigenome during early childhood (for example using micro-RNA or gene editing) to mitigate transmission of such behaviours.

Brain regions advising choice of behaviour

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is responsible for planning and sequencing activity (St Onge Reference St Onge and Floresco2010), as well as assisting the cerebellum and basal ganglia to smoothen motor and emotional activity. Subcomponents of the PFC have additional roles. The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) is involved in predicting and mitigating risks associated with proposed actions. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is responsible for recognising others' motivations, ensuring success of one's actions.

Functional imaging has revealed a reverberating circuit that perpetuates behaviour (Lipton Reference Lipton, Gonzales and Citri2019). This consists of the VMPFC, thalamus, anterior cingulate and hippocampus. Various compulsive behaviours (self-harm, ritualistic cleaning, bulimia and addictions) appear to share this circuit.

The insula

The site responsible for integrating and ultimately making choices on behaviour is buried under the surface of the frontotemporal cortex and is known as the insula (Gogolla Reference Gogolla2017). This site creates a three-dimensional representation of the self in relation to the environment, to assist selection of action. However, the likelihood of a particular response is influenced by previous episodes and consequences (information supplied by the hippocampus) as well as areas of the brain associated with conscience.

Conscience – the intrinsic modulator of hedonism

The main restraint on hedonistic impulses is one's ‘conscience’, an instinctive awareness that certain behaviours are unhelpful to others and, given time, to oneself (Schalkwijk Reference Schalkwijk2018). The experience of being protected and nurtured by adults during early childhood, alongside moral teaching and modelling of other humans, should develop one's conscience (Kochanska Reference Kochanska, Koenig and Barry2010). DLPFC-associated learning about how others experience one's behaviour, the so-called ‘theory of mind’ (Astington Reference Astington and Jenkins1995), would also assist maturation of conscience.

Neurochemically, conscience can be seen as glutaminergic suppression of excess dopaminergic/adrenergic drives, with further kindling achieved by gaining religious insights or utilising psychological techniques such as mindfulness (Schuman-Olivier Reference Schuman-Olivier, Trombka and Lovas2020). Alternatively, mitigation of hedonistic activity could potentially be induced via prayer to a ‘higher power’ (Arnaud Reference Arnaud, Kanyeredzi and Lawrence2015).

One interesting aspect of conscience is the presence of ‘guilt’ or ‘shame’ before and after a hedonistic act is contemplated. Two pilot studies (Takahashi Reference Takahashi, Yahata and Koeda2004; Michl Reference Michl, Meindl and Meister2014) involving functional imaging suggest that guilt and shame share similar sites of activation involving prefrontal, temporal and limbic areas, with specific activation of the amygdala and insula when experiencing guilt and activation of the anterior cingulate and parahippocampal areas during the experience of shame. It is possible that guilt and shame activate the reverberating circuit described above, resulting in compulsive behaviours.

Potential extrinsic modulators of hedonistic behaviours

Optimal functioning of the gut biome (Mohajeri Reference Mohajeri, La Fata and Steinert2018) and subsequent vagal nerve input to the brain-stem can have a modulating influence on fear-mediated behaviour (Breit Reference Breit, Kupferberg and Rogler2018). Indeed, improvements in the gut biome involving dietary fibre (prebiotics) and bacterial species supplementation (probiotics), alongside faecal transplantation, are being considered for behaviour modification (Meyyappan Reference Meyyappan, Forth and Wallace2020).

Another approach to gaining insight is by using psychedelics such as psilocybin, which can produce enhanced awareness of mental activity (Lowe Reference Lowe, Toyang and Steele2021). However, these can result in ‘bad trips’ involving frightening hallucinations. Repurposed drugs such as ketamine can increase expression of glutamate at the frontal cortex (Abdallah Reference Abdallah, de Feyter and Averill2018), thereby modulating dopamine release. Ketamine is being considered for treatment of addictive disorders (Jones Reference Jones, Mateus and Malcolm2018).

The main psychological approach used to modulate behaviour is a combination of progressive muscular relaxation coupled with guided imagery for slowing thought processes, which leads to improved awareness of one's thoughts, emotions and impulses (Toussaint Reference Toussaint, Nguyen and Roettger2021). This allows the person to gain insight about triggers and motivations for behaviour.

Forced hedonism: effects of dopamine agonist use

Three clinical syndromes are associated with prolonged use of dopamine agonists (such as pramipexole, cabergoline, ropinirole). These drugs are used for symptomatic treatment of Parkinsonism, restless legs syndrome and, of late, treatment-resistant depression.

Impulse control disorder

Up to 15% of patients on dopamine agonists experience hedonistic impulses (O'Sullivan Reference O'Sullivan, Evans and Lees2009), for example to shop or gamble excessively, become hypersexualised or access proscribed internet sites. These behaviours can lead to financial losses or criminal prosecution. Previous illicit drug use or a family history of addictions increase the risk of impulse control disorder.

Dopamine dysregulation syndrome

This associated condition is characterised by escalating dopamine agonist consumption (including levodopa for Parkinson's disease), psychomotor agitation, euphoria, drug-related dyskinesias and resistance to dose reduction (Lawrence Reference Lawrence, Evans and Lees2003). Discontinuation of these drugs can result in dopamine agonist withdrawal syndrome, characterised by agitation, panic attacks, generalised pain, orthostatic hypotension, insomnia and, at times, suicidality, with symptoms lasting months (Nirenberg Reference Nirenberg2013) even with gradual tapering. Patients who have experienced impulse control disorder are particularly at risk.

Punding behaviours

Punding is a stereotyped behaviour characterised by an intense fascination with a complex, repetitive activity without necessity (Fasano Reference Fasano and Petrovic2010). Examples include repetitive cleaning, dismantling electrical equipment and, arguably, constantly trawling social media. Punding has mainly been studied in people with Parkinson's disease prescribed dopamine agonists long term and among people addicted to cocaine or amphetamine. Drugs interacting with dopamine type 1 and 2 receptors are especially implicated. Individuals are aware of their behaviour, but reluctant to discontinue, as they experience punding as calming.

Religious approaches to modulating behaviour

The Abrahamic religions (Judaism, Christianity and Islam) regard hedonistic behaviour as being detrimental to faith in, and respect for, God the creator of humans, and therefore consider it ‘sinful’ (Weldon Reference Weldon, Leeming, Madden and Marlan2010). The expectation is to seek repentance for one's hedonistic impulses, minimising accessibility of environmental cues and being watchful of potential relapses. This approach of avoidance and vigilance is also used by secular organisations such as Alcoholics Anonymous, with similar faith in a ‘higher power’ based on an understanding that addictive behaviour cannot be controlled by one's will.

These religions hold contradictory opinions on altruistic behaviours: Christianity decries ‘carrying out good works’, believing this is not the path to communing with God, whereas the other two do believe in maximising altruistic behaviour. Perhaps the idea that altruistic behaviours are potentially as addictive as hedonistic tendencies owing to the shared dopamine reward pathway might be relevant in this debate.

Conclusions

Neuroscience provides an understanding of the physiological processes involved in reward-driven behaviour, both of an altruistic and harmful nature, via the dopamine reward pathway. Current neuroscience findings also suggest that the insula is the probable site of coordination and, in effect, the final decision maker. Current functional imaging suggests that the mind and brain ‘co-produce’ hedonistic impulses.

A ‘sensitive period’ during the first 2 years of life for experiencing privation (abuse, domestic disharmony or neglect) suggests an epigenetically mediated predisposition to harmful behaviour in later life. However, nurturing parental care can lead to more altruistic behaviours in adulthood, perhaps a more nuanced interpretation of ‘original sin’.

The possibility that compulsive or addictive behaviours could be modified by intervening in epigenetic processes in early childhood is intriguing but raises ethical problems. Furthermore, epigenetic signatures suggesting a predisposition to addiction or violence (for example to alcoholism or domestic violence) raises issues of ‘pre-crime’ described in the film Minority Report.

Clinically, pragmatic research is needed to compare the effectiveness of various religious and secular approaches to combat harmful compulsive hedonistic behaviours and to develop the conscience. Design of future studies should include the value of synergism in combining approaches and the influence of personal preferences.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

None.

MCQs

Select the single best option for each question stem

  1. 1 The dopamine ‘reward’ supply:

    1. a originates in the locus ceruleus

    2. b preferentially supplies the mesolimbic system

    3. c does not lead to tolerance

    4. d can produce cross-sensitisation between behaviours

    5. e does not involve altruistic behaviours.

  2. 2 The serotonergic pathway:

    1. a is experienced as a ‘thrill’ sensation

    2. b originates at the raphe nucleus

    3. c is inhibited by vagal nerve input

    4. d is not influenced by the gut biome

    5. e activates the dopamine pathway.

  3. 3 Glutamate in the brain:

    1. a is the main ‘learning from experience’ system

    2. b is expressed from childhood onwards

    3. c inhibits synaptic pruning periods

    4. d activates microglia

    5. e is predominantly produced in the brain-stem.

  4. 4 Behavioural epigenetics:

    1. a involves DNA cleavage

    2. b is mediated through demethylation

    3. c can result in better maternal care

    4. d can influence cortisol response to stress

    5. e has no effect on adult behaviour.

  5. 5 Impulse control disorder:

    1. a is associated with dopamine antagonists

    2. b is seen in 5% of dopamine agonist users

    3. c is more likely when a family history of addiction is present

    4. d does not include excess shopping

    5. e is not related to dopamine agonist withdrawal syndrome.

MCQ answers

1 d 2 b 3 a 4 d 5 c

References

Abdallah, CG, de Feyter, HM, Averill, LA, et al. (2018) The effects of ketamine on prefrontal glutamate neurotransmission in healthy and depressed subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43: 2154–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnaud, Y, Kanyeredzi, A, Lawrence, J (2015) AA members understandings of the Higher Power (HP) a qualitative study. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy, 6: 233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, JW, Jenkins, JM (1995) Theory of mind development and social understanding. Cognition and Emotion, 9: 151–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bland, VJ, Lambie, I (2018) Does childhood neglect contribute to violent behaviour in adulthood? A review of possible links. Clinical Psychology Review, 60: 126–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breit, S, Kupferberg, A, Rogler, G, et al. (2018) Vagus nerve as modulator of the brain–gut axis in psychiatric and inflammatory disorders front. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9: 44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Silva, PN (2019) Are sweetened drinks a gateway to alcohol addiction? Review of shared morbidity, neurocircuitry and therapy. MOJ Addiction Medicine and Therapy, 6: 14.Google Scholar
Farringdon, DP (1993) Childhood origins of teenage antisocial behaviour and adult social dysfunction. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 86: 13–7.Google Scholar
Fasano, A, Petrovic, I (2010) Insights into pathophysiology of punding reveal possible treatment strategies. Molecular Psychiatry, 15: 560–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faust, TE, Gunner, G, Schafer, DP (2021) Mechanisms governing activity-dependent synaptic pruning in the developing mammalian CNS. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 22: 657–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franke, A (2013) Temptation restrained: enhancing an ethical culture in the Academy (or the seven deadly sins updated for higher education). Proceedings of the 2013 Educational Leadership Conference 'Ethics in Education' (Washington, DC 28 Sept–2 Oct). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Gogolla, N (2017) The insular cortex. Current Biology, 27(12): R580–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, JL, Mateus, CF, Malcolm, RJ, et al. (2018) Efficacy of ketamine in the treatment of substance use disorders: a systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9: 277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G, Koenig, JL, Barry, RA, et al. (2010) Children's conscience during toddler and preschool years, moral self, and a competent, adaptive developmental trajectory. Developmental Psychology, 46: 1320–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koob, GF, Volkow, ND (2010) Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35: 217223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, AD, Evans, AH, Lees, AJ (2003) Compulsive use of dopamine replacement therapy in Parkinson's disease: reward systems gone awry? Lancet Neurology, 2: 595604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipton, DM, Gonzales, BJ, Citri, A (2019) Dorsal striatal circuits for habits, compulsions and addictions. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13: 28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, H, Toyang, N, Steele, B, et al. (2021) The therapeutic potential of psilocybin. Molecules, 26(10): 2948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowry, CA, Johnson, PL, Hay-Schmit, A, et al. (2005) Modulation of anxiety circuits by serotonergic systems. Stress, 8: 233–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCorry, LK (2007) Physiology of the autonomic nervous system. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 71(4): 78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meldrum, BS (2000) Glutamate as a neurotransmitter in the brain: review of physiology and pathology. Journal of Nutrition, 130: 1007S–15S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyyappan, AC, Forth, E, Wallace, FE, et al. (2020) Effect of faecal microbiota transplant on symptoms of psychiatric disorders: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 20: 299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michl, P, Meindl, T, Meister, F, et al. (2014) Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: a pilot fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9: 150157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, NS, Dackis, CA, Gold, MS. 1987 The relationship of addiction, tolerance, and dependence to alcohol and drugs: a neurochemical approach. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 4: 197207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohajeri, MH, La Fata, G, Steinert, RE, et al. (2018) Relationship between the gut microbiome and brain function. Nutrition Reviews, 76: 481–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nirenberg, MJ (2013) Dopamine agonist withdrawal syndrome: implications for patient care. Drugs & Aging, 30: 587–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Sullivan, SS, Evans, AH, Lees, AJ, et al. (2009) Dopamine dysregulation syndrome: an overview of its epidemiology, mechanisms and management. CNS Drugs, 23: 157–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piliavin, JA, Callero, PL, Evans, DE (1982) Addiction to altruism? Opponent-process theory and habitual blood donation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43: 1200–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rahula, W (1959) What the Buddha Taught. Reprinted (1974), Grove Press.Google Scholar
Riedel, G, Platt, B, Micheau, J (2003) Glutamate receptor function in learning and memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 140: 147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schalkwijk, F (2018) A new conceptualisation of conscience. Frontiers in Psychology, 9: 1863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuman-Olivier, Z, Trombka, M, Lovas, DA, et al. (2020) Mindfulness and behaviour change. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 28: 371–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skeel, DA, Longman, T (2011) The mosaic law in Christian perspective (public law research paper No. 11–25). University of Pennsylvania Law School. Available from: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1903928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonne, JW, Gash, DM (2018) Psychopathy to altruism: neurobiology of the selfish–selfless spectrum. Frontiers in Psychology, 9: 575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St Onge, JR, Floresco, SB (2010) Prefrontal cortical contribution to risk-based decision making. Cerebral Cortex, 20: 1816–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takahashi, H, Yahata, N, Koeda, M, et al. (2004) Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23: 967–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timpe, K (2021) Sin in Christian thought. In The Stanford Encyclopaedia of Philosophy (Summer 2021 Edition) (ed Zalta, EN). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University (https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2021/entries/sin-christian/).Google Scholar
Toussaint, L, Nguyen, QA, Roettger, C, et al. (2021) Effectiveness of progressive muscle relaxation, deep breathing, and guided imagery in promoting psychological and physiological states of relaxation. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 2021: 5924040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Upshaw, MB, Kaiser, CR, Sommerville, JA (2015) Parents’ empathic perspective taking, and altruistic behavior predicts infants’ arousal to others’ emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vassoler, FM, Sadri-Vakili, G (2014) Mechanisms of transgenerational inheritance of addictive-like behaviors. Neuroscience, 264: 198206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, IC, Cervoni, N, Champagne, FA, et al. (2004) Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8): 847–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weldon, C (2010) Original sin. In Encyclopaedia of Psychology and Religion (eds Leeming, DA, Madden, K, Marlan, S). Springer. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-71802-6_480.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Summary of three ethical codes, with possible contemporary applications

Figure 1
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.