Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T05:04:49.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Causal pathways to social and occupational functioning in the first episode of schizophrenia: uncovering unmet treatment needs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2021

Kathleen Miley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA School of Nursing, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Piper Meyer-Kalos
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Sisi Ma
Affiliation:
Institute for Health Informatics, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
David J. Bond
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Erich Kummerfeld
Affiliation:
Institute for Health Informatics, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Sophia Vinogradov
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Kathleen Miley, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

We aimed to identify unmet treatment needs for improving social and occupational functioning in early schizophrenia using a data-driven causal discovery analysis.

Methods

Demographic, clinical, and psychosocial measures were obtained for 276 participants from the Recovery After an Initial Schizophrenia Episode Early Treatment Program (RAISE-ETP) trial at baseline and 6-months, along with measures of social and occupational functioning from the Quality of Life Scale. The Greedy Fast Causal Inference algorithm was used to learn a partial ancestral graph modeling causal relationships across baseline variables and 6-month functioning. Effect sizes were estimated using a structural equation model. Results were validated in an independent dataset (N = 187).

Results

In the data-generated model, greater baseline socio-affective capacity was a cause of greater baseline motivation [Effect size (ES) = 0.77], and motivation was a cause of greater baseline social and occupational functioning (ES = 1.5 and 0.96, respectively), which in turn were causes of their own 6-month outcomes. Six-month motivation was also identified as a cause of occupational functioning (ES = 0.92). Cognitive impairment and duration of untreated psychosis were not direct causes of functioning at either timepoint. The graph for the validation dataset was less determinate, but otherwise supported the findings.

Conclusions

In our data-generated model, baseline socio-affective capacity and motivation are the most direct causes of occupational and social functioning 6 months after entering treatment in early schizophrenia. These findings indicate that socio-affective abilities and motivation are specific high-impact treatment needs that must be addressed in order to promote optimal social and occupational recovery.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders are characterized by severe disability in social and occupational functioning (Wiersma et al., Reference Wiersma, Wanderling, Dragomirecka, Ganev, Harrison, Der Heiden and Walsh2000), reducing quality of life and causing significant societal burden (Cloutier et al., Reference Cloutier, Sanon Aigbogun, Guerin, Nitulescu, Ramanakumar, Kamat and Wu2016). Though these impairments are evident early, specialized early intervention services have failed to drastically alter long-term functional outcomes (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Anderson, MacDougall, Manchanda, Harricharan, Subramanian and Northcott2018; Secher et al., Reference Secher, Hjorthøj, Austin, Thorup, Jeppesen, Mors and Nordentoft2015). Indeed, the greatest improvements in social and occupational functioning (when present) are observed during the first 6 months of treatment, with little significant gain thereafter (Humensky, Essock, & Dixon, Reference Humensky, Essock and Dixon2017; Phahladira et al., Reference Phahladira, Luckhoff, Asmal, Kilian, Scheffler, Du Plessis and Emsley2020). While numerous predictors of functional outcome have been identified [including positive and negative symptoms, cognitive impairment, and duration of untreated psychosis (DUP), to name a few] (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011; Milev, Ho, Arndt, & Andreasen, Reference Milev, Ho, Arndt and Andreasen2005; Nakagami, Hoe, & Brekke, Reference Nakagami, Hoe and Brekke2010; Penttilä, Jaä̈skel̈ainen, Hirvonen, Isohanni, & Miettunen, Reference Penttilä, Jaä̈skel̈ainen, Hirvonen, Isohanni and Miettunen2014; Santesteban-Echarri et al., Reference Santesteban-Echarri, Paino, Rice, González-Blanch, McGorry, Gleeson and Alvarez-Jimenez2017), some of them, such as positive symptoms, do not robustly affect functional outcomes even when they are altered by treatment (Swartz et al., Reference Swartz, Perkins, Stroup, Davis, Capuano and Rosenheck2007; Wunderink, Nieboer, Wiersma, Sytema, & Nienhuis, Reference Wunderink, Nieboer, Wiersma, Sytema and Nienhuis2013). This suggests that many clinical predictors of functional outcomes may not necessarily cause functional outcomes.

Previous research identifying predictors of functional outcomes have been limited by statistical methods that are not informed by causal theory. Identifying treatment needs with the greatest potential impact requires identifying which predictors are most plausibly causes of functional outcomes. Causal modeling algorithms are novel methods which combine graph theory, statistics, and machine learning to produce hypothetical causal models (Spirtes, Glymour, & Scheines, Reference Spirtes, Glymour and Scheines2000). Such models have been demonstrated to outperform predictive statistical approaches, such as structural equation models (SEMs) and other regression-based methods in the identification of treatment targets (Maathuis, Kalisch, & Bühlmann, Reference Maathuis, Kalisch and Bühlmann2009; Shen, Ma, Vemuri, & Simon, Reference Shen, Ma, Vemuri and Simon2020; Stekhoven et al., Reference Stekhoven, Moraes, Sveinbjörnsson, Hennig, Maathuis and Bühlmann2012; Taruttis, Spang, & Engelmann, Reference Taruttis, Spang and Engelmann2015). SEM analysis, for example, relies on a priori model selection to test a specific causal structure, and the effect estimate resulting from SEM is accurate only when the hypothesized causal structure is accurate, a condition hard to guarantee. SEM also requires specifying the causal structure of all included variables, which is rarely known in a complex causal system containing many variables. In contrast, the causal discovery analysis employed here uses a data-driven approach to search the space of possible SEMs to identify the causal network structure that best fits the data, including identification of causal effects of latent (unmeasured) variables (Ogarrio, Spirtes, & Ramsey, Reference Ogarrio, Spirtes and Ramsey2016). Thus, in addition to generating statistically defensible models of underlying causal structures (Shen et al., Reference Shen, Ma, Vemuri and Simon2020), causal discovery offers the advantage of modeling a more comprehensive set of treatment target candidates.

In order to identify plausible causes of functional outcomes during the 6-month critical window for early intervention, we applied a novel data-driven causal modeling method to data from the Recovery After an Initial Schizophrenia Episode Early Treatment Program (RAISE-ETP) trial. Our aim was to identify variables that are modeled to cause, rather than only predict or associate with, social and occupational functioning, and thus represent high impact unmet treatment needs for promoting functional recovery in early schizophrenia. We validated our results in an independent dataset drawn from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) study.

Methods

Data acquisition

De-identified data were obtained from the National Institute of Mental Health National Database for Clinical Trials (https://data-archive.nimh.nih.gov/). The University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board approved this research as exempt from human subjects research.

Participants

Participants for study 1 were drawn from the RAISE-ETP randomized controlled trial (Kane et al., Reference Kane, Robinson, Schooler, Mueser, Penn, Rosenheck and Heinssen2016). RAISE-ETP enrolled 404 participants across 21 states. Inclusion criteria were: ages 15–40; diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, schizophreniform disorder, brief psychotic disorder, or psychosis not otherwise specified; only one lifetime episode of psychosis; and duration of antipsychotic use less than 6 months. RAISE-ETP participants who completed baseline and 6-month follow up assessments were included in this analysis (N = 276). Participants from both arms of the RAISE-ETP trial were included because we sought to identify variables that causally impact functional outcome regardless of treatment program; the treatment arm was included in the model to assess any effect on the estimated causal network structure.

Participants for study 2 were drawn from the CATIE trial (Stroup et al., Reference Stroup, Mcevoy, Swartz, Byerly, Qlick, Canive and Lieberman2003). CATIE enrolled 1600 participants across 50 sites; inclusion criteria were: ages 18–65 years and a schizophrenia diagnosis. Exclusion criteria were psychotic symptoms for less than 3 years and antipsychotic use less than 1 year. We studied CATIE participants who were 40 years of age and younger, had antipsychotic duration of less than 5 years, and who completed baseline and 6-month assessments of our primary outcome measures; our goal was to attain similarity with the RAISE-ETP participants while maximizing the sample size (N = 187).

Study 1: Variables and measures

The outcome variables for our analysis included social and occupational functioning derived from the interviewer-rated Heinrichs–Carpenter Quality of Life Scale (QLS) (Heinrichs, Hanlon, & Carpenter, Reference Heinrichs, Hanlon and Carpenter1984), which was the primary outcome measure in the RAISE-ETP trial. Consistent with RAISE-ETP, we used the traditional subscale constructs for the Interpersonal Relations (items 1–8, assessing relationships and level of social activity) and Instrumental Role (items 9–12, assessing level of occupational role functioning, accomplishment, underemployment, and satisfaction with occupational functioning) subscales to derive our variables for Social and Occupational Functioning, respectively.

Primary variables

Primary variables were selected based on previous findings that motivation, social cognition, cognition, negative symptoms and DUP are consistent predictors of functional outcome in schizophrenia (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011; Milev et al., Reference Milev, Ho, Arndt and Andreasen2005; Nakagami et al., Reference Nakagami, Hoe and Brekke2010; Penttilä et al., Reference Penttilä, Jaä̈skel̈ainen, Hirvonen, Isohanni and Miettunen2014; Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Prestia, Twamley, Patterson, Bowie and Harvey2014; Santesteban-Echarri et al., Reference Santesteban-Echarri, Paino, Rice, González-Blanch, McGorry, Gleeson and Alvarez-Jimenez2017).

Motivation was derived from three items from the Intrapsychic Foundations subscale of the QLS: item 13, ‘sense of purpose,’ item 14, ‘degree of motivation,’ and item 15, ‘curiosity.’ These items were originally derived as a measure of intrinsic motivation (Nakagami, Xie, Hoe, & Brekke, Reference Nakagami, Xie, Hoe and Brekke2008) and may also reflect a general trait-like motivation (Choi, Choi, Felice Reddy, & Fiszdon, Reference Choi, Choi, Felice Reddy and Fiszdon2014). They have been used extensively as a measure of motivation in schizophrenia with demonstrated convergent validity with interviewer-rated measures of motivation and laboratory measures of effort-based decision making (Fervaha, Foussias, Takeuchi, Agid, & Remington, Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Takeuchi, Agid and Remington2015; Horan et al., Reference Horan, Reddy, Barch, Buchanan, Dunayevich, Gold and Green2015). A recent factor analysis found that these items loaded most strongly on the Intrapsychic Foundations/Motivation subscale (Mueser et al., Reference Mueser, Kim, Addingon, Mcgurk, Pratt and Addington2017), and they predict cross-sectional and longitudinal functional outcomes in early and chronic schizophrenia (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Choi, Felice Reddy and Fiszdon2014; Fervaha, Foussias, Agid, & Remington, Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington2015; Saperstein, Fiszdon, & Bell, Reference Saperstein, Fiszdon and Bell2011).

Socio-affective capacity. As there are no measures of social cognition in RAISE-ETP, we derived the variable ‘socio-affective capacity’ from the QLS as a proxy measure for higher order social cognitive abilities, such as social perception, theory of mind, and empathy. This measure consisted of item 20 ‘capacity for empathy’ and item 21, ‘capacity for engagement and emotional with the interviewer’ from the Intrapsychic Foundations subscale; they assess an individual's ability to perceive and respond to another person's perspective and affective state. Social perception, theory of mind, and empathy rely on partially shared neural circuitry and depend on intact functioning of the social brain (Green, Horan, & Lee, Reference Green, Horan and Lee2015; Shamay-Tsoory, Reference Shamay-Tsoory2011; Sparks, Mcdonald, Lino, O'donnell, & Green, Reference Sparks, Mcdonald, Lino, O'donnell and Green2010).

Cognition. A cognitive composite standardized Z-score was created from the Brief Assessment of Cognition using the average score of the six assessed cognitive domains (Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004).

Negative symptoms were derived from the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale traditional three syndrome model composite score (Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1987).

DUP, defined as time between onset of psychosis and initiation of antipsychotic medication, was measured in days at the time of study entry, and was log transformed due to skewness.

Secondary variables

Our analysis included psychopathology, substance use, psychological well-being (Browne et al., Reference Browne, Penn, Meyer-Kalos, Mueser, Estroff, Brunette and Kane2016), attitudes toward medications, age (log transformed) and treatment arm (Table 1) as additional variables which could influence functional outcomes and to identify more determinate edges among the primary variables (Ogarrio et al., Reference Ogarrio, Spirtes and Ramsey2016). Variable descriptions are included in the Supplemental Methods. Like the primary RAISE-ETP study, we did not include prescribed chlorpromazine equivalents in this analysis.

Table 1. Study sample characteristics and variable descriptive statistics

a Age p = <0.001, t = −8.0; QLS occupational functioning p = <0.001, t = 4.0.

b Neurocognitive tests utilized differed between groups and means were not compared. Study 1 utilized the Brief Assessment of Cognition and study 2 utilized a customized panel of neurocognitive tests. PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, QLS = Quality of Life Scale; Blank cells indicate variable was not available in dataset.<

Study 2 variables and measures

Study 2 used the same variables as study 1 for social functioning, occupational functioning, motivation, and socio-affective capacity and several of the same secondary variables, as detailed in Table 1.

Statistical analysis

Independent sample t tests for continuous variables and Pearson's chi-square tests for categorical variables tested for differences in demographic and clinical variables between (1) RAISE-ETP participants included and excluded from study 1, and (2) participants in study 1 and study 2.

We used the Greedy Fast Causal Inference (GFCI) algorithm to estimate the causal relationships among baseline and 6-month variables. GFCI searches the space of all possible partial ancestral graphs (PAGs) using a combination of goodness of fit statistics and conditional independence tests to identify the PAG that best models the causal process from which the data were sampled (Chickering, Reference Chickering2002; Ogarrio et al., Reference Ogarrio, Spirtes and Ramsey2016; Ramsey, Reference Ramsey2015). In its first step, GFCI temporarily assumes that there are no unmeasured common causes of the observed variables and searches the space of all possible causal models to find the model which has the best penalized likelihood score. In its second step, it drops the assumption that there are no unmeasured common causes, and refines the graph produced by the first step by testing for all possible statistical inconsistencies that could have been induced by latent common causes. These inconsistencies are identified using conditional independence tests, and the graph is modified appropriately (Chickering, Reference Chickering2002; Ogarrio et al., Reference Ogarrio, Spirtes and Ramsey2016; Ramsey, Reference Ramsey2015). In the resulting PAG, variables are represented as nodes in the graph. The type and orientation of an edge connecting two nodes specifies the nature of the modeled causal relationship (Fig. 1). See the Supplementary Methods and citations (Chickering, Reference Chickering2002; Ogarrio et al., Reference Ogarrio, Spirtes and Ramsey2016; Ramsey, Reference Ramsey2015) for more detailed information on GFCI, including assumptions and proof of correctness. This process has been mathematically proven to be asymptotically correct, and has outperformed similar methods in simulations, where adjacencies and edge orientations in the output PAG have been shown to have high precision and recall on sample sizes comparable to the datasets studied here (Ogarrio et al., Reference Ogarrio, Spirtes and Ramsey2016).

Fig. 1. Greedy fast causal inference edge types and meaning. *Note: When viewed in Tetrad 6.6.0, this edge type is represented as a thin green edge. We have used a blue edge here to distinguish the edge more easily from the thick green directed edge above.

The Tetrad software package version 6.6 was used for the GFCI analysis (https://cmu-phil.github.io/tetrad/manual/). Background knowledge included that the 6-month variables could not cause baseline variables, and no variable could cause age. Model parameters include a Bayesian Information Criterion score, set to the standard penalty discount value of 1, and Fisher Z test, set to the default value of 0.01.

Effect size estimation and model fit statistics

Raw and standardized effect sizes (ES) of the model-identified causal relationships were estimated by fitting a linear SEM to the PAG. Edges connecting to a possible latent variable were modeled as indirect covariations for the purposes of the SEM. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) were inspected to assess model fit. The R package Lavaan 0.6.3 was used for this analysis (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). Due to the prevalence of multiple possible latent variables in the CATIE validation study, model parameters were not estimated to avoid producing a SEM with low interpretability.

Graph stability

The stability of the study 1 and study 2 PAGs were evaluated by running GFCI on 1000 jackknifed datasets containing 90% of the original dataset and 1000 bootstrapped datasets. The percentage of edges in the PAG that were confirmed in re-sampled graphs was calculated. Additional SEM comparison and sensitivity analyses were performed to assess for effects of measurement or construct overlap (see Supplemental Methods).

Results

Participants

After excluding 128 participants for missing data, study 1 included 276 participants from RAISE-ETP (Table 1). Excluded participants had higher scores on the Beliefs About Medications scale (t = −2.16, p = 0.03). There were no statistically significant differences on any other baseline variables between participants included and excluded from the analysis (online Supplementary Table S1).

Study 2 included 187 participants from CATIE (Table 1). Descriptive statistics were not compared between included and excluded CATIE participants because the included subset was selected to best match the RAISE-ETP cohort. Study 1 participants were significantly younger than study 2 participants (t = −8.0, p < 0.001) and had better 6-month occupational functioning (t = 4.0, p < 0.001) (Table 1).

Study 1: Primary analysis

The full PAG for the RAISE-ETP cohort is shown in Fig. 2a, and a functional outcome subgraph is shown in Fig. 2b. The strength and sign of modeled causal relationships in the functional outcome subgraph are provided in Fig. 3 and all effect sizes (raw and standardized) are provided in online Supplementary Table S2. The raw effect sizes are reported here. Fit statistics indicated an appropriate fit of the model to the data (CFI = 0.884; RMSEA = 0.066).

Fig. 2. Study 1 complete PAG and functional outcome subgraph. All variables represent baseline unless specified as 6M (6 month). Alcohol use = days of alcohol use in previous month; Cannabis use = days of cannabis use in the past month; CGI = Clinical Global Impressions-Severity score; Depression = Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia; DUP = duration of untreated psychosis (log transformed); Global Cognition = Brief Assessment of Cognition composite Z score; Medication Beliefs = Brief evaluation of medication influences and beliefs total score; MH Recovery = Mental Health Recovery Measure total score; PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptom Scale; SaC = Socio-affective capacity; Stigma = Stigma scale total score; Treatment Group = randomized into coordinated specialty care v. Treatment as Usual. Wellbeing = Well-being scale total score.

Fig. 3. Study 1 augmented rolled graph. *Causal effect size for baseline motivation on baseline occupational functioning. The causal effect size from 6-month motivation to 6-month occupational functioning is 0.92. ES = effect size. + 6 indicates a 6-month time cycle over which the effect occurred.

Causal pathways to 6-month social functioning

Our model contains one causal pathway leading to 6-month social functioning: baseline socio-affective capacity is a cause of motivation (ES = 0.77), which is then a cause of baseline social functioning (ES = 1.5), which is then a cause of 6-month social functioning (ES = 0.6).

Causal pathways to 6-month occupational functioning

Our model contains two causal pathways leading to 6-month occupational functioning. Path 1: baseline motivation is a cause of baseline occupational functioning (ES = 0.96), which then is a cause of 6-month occupational functioning (ES = 0.36). Path 2: 6-month social functioning is a cause of motivation (ES = 0.21), which in turn is a cause of occupational functioning (ES = 0.92).

Causal effects of cognition, negative symptoms, and DUP

Cognition is not included in a causal pathway to social or occupational functioning. Negative symptoms are a direct cause of socio-affective capacity at baseline (ES = −0.27), but their effect on social and occupational functioning is mediated by socio-affective capacity and motivation. DUP is upstream of the causal pathway to functional outcomes, and its effect is mediated by several variables including socio-affective capacity and motivation.

Six-month causal cycles

Our model contains several causal cycles which unfold over a period of 6 months, as shown in the augmented rolled graph (Fig. 3). For example, socio-affective capacity causes motivation at baseline (ES = 0.76), and at 6 months, motivation causes socio-affective capacity (ES = 0.24). Similarly, motivation causes social functioning at baseline (ES = 1.4), and at 6 months, social functioning causes motivation (ES = 0.22). Thus, those with better socio-affective capacity at baseline have better motivation, and sustained improvements in motivation at 6 months lead to improved socio-affective capacity: a virtuous cycle. Motivation and social functioning also form a virtuous cycle: those with better motivation at baseline consequently enjoy better social functioning, which feeds forward to better motivation at 6 months.

Study 1: Graph stability

The jackknife had 100% concordance with all graph features (i.e. edge presence, edge absence and edge orientation) in the functional outcome subgraph and the full PAG. The bootstrap had 100% concordance with all features in the functional outcomes subgraph (online Supplementary Table S3). We also tested a model in which the motivation and socio-affective capacity variables covary instead of being separate variables with the causal relationships detailed above, however our original model had better fit (p < 0.0001, see Supplemental Methods). This is expected because GFCI is capable of modeling unmeasured common causes, therefore if motivation and socio-affective capacity are better represented by a single factor GFCI should indicate the possibility of a latent variable.

Study 2: Primary analyses

The overall structure of the PAG for the functional outcome subgraph in study 2 is similar to study 1 (Fig. 4). Relationships between baseline and 6-month socio-affective capacity, motivation, and social and occupational functioning variables are maintained in the study 2 PAG, supporting the findings from study 1. Some edge orientations in this graph are less determinate, which permits but does not confirm the corresponding orientations found in study 1.

Fig. 4. Study 2: validation complete PAG and functional outcome subgraph. All variables represent baseline unless specified as 6M (6-month). Antipsychotic years = years of antipsychotic use; CGI = Clinical Global Impressions-Severity score; Depression = Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia; Facial Affect Recognition = Face emotion discrimination task correct responses; Global Cognition = Cognition composite Z score; PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, SaC = Socio-affective capacity.

Study 2: Graph stability

The jackknife graph had 100% concordance with all graph features for the functional outcome subgraph. The bootstrap graph resulted in two dropped edges (connecting baseline to 6-month socio-affective capacity and baseline motivation to 6-month socio-affective capacity), and had more determinate edge orientations for two edges (connecting baseline to 6-month occupational functioning and baseline to 6-month social functioning) when compared to the functional outcome subgraph in the primary PAG (online Supplementary Table S3).

Discussion

Summary of causal pathway findings

We used causal discovery modeling on a large sample of patients with first episode schizophrenia, to model which variables plausibly cause social and occupational functioning 6 months after entering treatment. We discovered a primary causal pathway in our model with large effect sizes from baseline socio-affective capacity to motivation, and from motivation to both social and occupational functioning at 6 months. Interestingly, cognition did not play a causal role; and DUP and negative symptoms indirectly influenced social and occupational functioning. These results may indicate the importance of vigorously and specifically targeting socio-affective capacity and motivation as soon as possible after a young individual enters care. Further, the discovered causal 6-month cycle between socio-affective capacity and motivation suggests that early gains in these areas will be self-sustaining through a positive feedback loop. To the best of our knowledge, most evidence-based treatment programs do not explicitly identify these as critical targets of intervention nor directly assess them as part of patient outcome and program evaluation.

We also uncovered a modeled causal cycle between social functioning and motivation. At baseline, motivation was a cause of social functioning, and at 6 months social functioning was a cause of motivation. Higher motivation then led to higher occupational functioning. A recent SEM study using RAISE data found that social functioning, but not occupational functioning, predicted later motivation (Fulford et al., Reference Fulford, Piskulic, Addington, Kane, Schooler and Mueser2017). Directly improving social functioning may be a third critical early treatment target for maintaining motivation and ultimately improving occupational functioning (Fulford et al., Reference Fulford, Piskulic, Addington, Kane, Schooler and Mueser2017).

Possible mechanisms for the causal pathways and cycles

Socio-affective capacity and motivation

Socio-affective capacity as measured in this study may capture aspects of social perception, theory of mind, and empathy during the QLS interview. As such, this measure may reflect social cognition abilities and the capacity to engage interpersonally. Shared neural mechanisms underlie social cognition and intact reward processing (necessary for adaptive motivated behavior), including important hubs in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate (Fareri & Delgado, Reference Fareri and Delgado2014); moreover, social stimuli are primary reinforcers for reward networks and activate frontal−striatal circuits (Fareri & Delgado, Reference Fareri and Delgado2014). Deficits in perceiving and processing social stimuli and aberrant functioning of medial prefrontal circuitry can likely affect aspects of reward processing and motivated behavior. Consistently, we have found that social cognition training drives both adaptive changes in motivated behavior (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Nahum, Howard, Rowlands, Brandrett, Kermott and Vinogradov2017; Miley et al., Reference Miley, Fisher, Nahum, Howard, Rowlands, Brandrett and Vinogradov2020) and medial prefrontal cortex neural activity (Subramaniam et al., Reference Subramaniam, Luks, Fisher, Simpson, Nagarajan and Vinogradov2012).

Motivation and social functioning

Individuals with psychotic disorders exhibit not only impaired social cognition (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011), but reductions in social drive (Cornblatt et al., Reference Cornblatt, Carrion, Addington, Seidman, Walker, Cannon and Lencz2012; Tarbox & Pogue-Geile, Reference Tarbox and Pogue-Geile2008), and specific deficits in the valuation of social reward (Catalano, Heerey, & Gold, Reference Catalano, Heerey and Gold2018). Poor motivation for social rewards may therefore drive impairments in social functioning. At the same time, social inclusion and adaptive social functioning are critical developmental accomplishments during the adolescent and young adult years (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008; Filia, Jackson, Cotton, & Killackey, Reference Filia, Jackson, Cotton and Killackey2019; Filia, Jackson, Cotton, Gardner, & Killackey, Reference Filia, Jackson, Cotton, Gardner and Killackey2018; Gardner, Filia, Killackey, & Cotton, Reference Gardner, Filia, Killackey and Cotton2019), coinciding with the typical age of onset of psychosis. Social exclusion during this period may lead to the development of socially defeatist beliefs, further impeding motivated behavior for social participation (Campellone, Sanchez, & Kring, Reference Campellone, Sanchez and Kring2016). Thus, directly targeting social functioning may lead to enriched social environments that reinforce social rewards and improve the valuation of these rewards from continued exposure to positive social experiences, resulting in improved motivated behavior. Improved motivation may in turn promote engagement in goal setting and pursuit of occupational endeavors, and feedback to social cognitive functioning via the modulation of shared neural networks which underlie social cognitive and reward processes.

Causal roles of negative symptoms, cognitive impairment, and DUP

The negative symptoms of schizophrenia are strongly associated with poor functional outcomes (Milev et al., Reference Milev, Ho, Arndt and Andreasen2005; Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, Prestia, Twamley, Patterson, Bowie and Harvey2014). While negative symptoms were in the causal pathway to functional outcome in our model, their effect was at least partially mediated by socio-affective capacity and motivation. Socio-affective capacity and motivation share some, but not full, conceptual overlap with negative symptoms as measured by the PANSS subscale, which also includes measures of blunted affect and alogia. That the effect of negative symptoms in our model was at least partially mediated by socio-affective processing and motivation may suggest that specifically targeting the asociality and avolition aspects of negative symptoms – which rely on intact socio-affective and motivation processes − may be most likely to drive improvements in both social and occupational functioning.

Unexpectedly, baseline cognitive impairment was not found to be in the causal pathway to functional outcomes, despite the large body of evidence linking cognition to functional outcomes (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011; Santesteban-Echarri et al., Reference Santesteban-Echarri, Paino, Rice, González-Blanch, McGorry, Gleeson and Alvarez-Jimenez2017). It is possible that significantly impaired cognition causes poor functional outcomes, but that this relationship does not hold with only mildly or moderately impaired cognition, leading to unstable or missed relationships in causal discovery analyses. It is also possible that the relationship between cognition and functional outcomes is mediated by variables not measured in this study, such as more specific measures of social cognition, reward processing, or other neural/neurocognitive operations (Bhagyavathi et al., Reference Bhagyavathi, Mehta, Thirthalli, Kumar, Kumar, Subbakrishna and Gangadhar2015; Gard, Fisher, Garrett, Genevsky, & Vinogradov, Reference Gard, Fisher, Garrett, Genevsky and Vinogradov2009; Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee, & Wynn, Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012). In our validation study drawn from the CATIE trial, cognition was possibly on a causal pathway to functional outcomes, however the direction of these relationships cannot be determined. RAISE-ETP used a less comprehensive cognitive assessment battery compared to CATIE, and it is possible that this resulted in the observed differences between the two datasets.

The association of DUP with functional outcomes in schizophrenia has also been strongly supported by previous research (Penttilä et al., Reference Penttilä, Jaä̈skel̈ainen, Hirvonen, Isohanni and Miettunen2014); however, DUP was upstream in the causal pathways to functional outcomes in our model. It remains largely unknown whether shortening DUP will lead to improved long-term outcomes (Penttilä et al., Reference Penttilä, Jaä̈skel̈ainen, Hirvonen, Isohanni and Miettunen2014), and the specific mechanisms through which DUP impacts functional outcomes is unknown. Our model suggests that shortening DUP could impact functional recovery via its direct and downstream effects on several other clinical features, including depression, global disease severity, negative symptoms, motivation, and socio-affective capacity.

Unmet treatment needs in early schizophrenia

Our results point to critical unmet therapeutic needs in current coordinated specialty care (CSC) early intervention services, which are now the gold standard treatment for first episode schizophrenia. While CSC programs indirectly aim to improve social functioning, they currently lack explicit and consistently delivered interventions which robustly target social cognition or motivation as primary outcomes. Yet, deficits in social cognition and motivation are malleable to targeted interventions in early schizophrenia, such as social cognitive training, social skills groups, recovery focused goal-setting and digitally delivered motivational coaching (Eack et al., Reference Eack, Greenwald, Hogarty, Cooley, DiBarry, Montrose and Keshavan2009; Fernandez-Gonzalo et al., Reference Fernandez-Gonzalo, Turon, Jodar, Pousa, Hernandez Rambla, García and Palao2015; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Nahum, Howard, Rowlands, Brandrett, Kermott and Vinogradov2017; Fulford, Piper Meyer-Kalos, & Kim Mueser, Reference Fulford, Piper Meyer-Kalos and Kim Mueser2020; Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Combs, Willoughby, Mintz, Gibson, Rupp and Penn2014; Schlosser et al., Reference Schlosser, Campellone, Truong, Etter, Vergani, Komaiko and Vinogradov2018). Our results highlight the need for research on the integration of existing and novel social cognition and motivation interventions into CSC programs. Further, they suggest that specifically targeting social cognitive processes when individuals first enter treatment may exploit a positive feedback relationship between social cognition and motivation, thereby driving improved social and occupational functioning. Interventions specifically targeting social recovery have been shown to elicit improvements in social engagement when added to early intervention services (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Hodgekins, French, Marshall, Freemantle, Mccrone and Birchwood2018) and should continue to be evaluated as critical treatment components.

Validation and limitations

Our validation study from the CATIE dataset did not contradict the results of our primary analysis and supported many of the findings. The overall structure of the functional outcome subgraph was very similar to the RAISE-ETP model even though many edge orientations in the graph were less definitive. The less definitive relationships could be due to differences in sample size, which can influence the ability of the GFCI algorithm to rule out alternative causal models. RAISE-ETP participants were younger and included individuals aged 15–17 who may have different supports and functional expectations than adult patients. CATIE participants had greater illness chronicity, longer duration of exposure to medications, and an established diagnosis of schizophrenia (v. one lifetime episode of psychosis), which could lead to more heterogeneity in symptoms and functioning. Temporally, enrollment dates for RAISE-ETP and CATIE were approximately a decade apart during a movement toward recovery-oriented care. It is likely that these differences could also impact results pertinent to longitudinal functional outcomes, including the directionality and/or strength of causal relationships among our primary variables. These differences may lend support to the value of our validation study, suggesting that our results could generalize beyond illness phase and contextual factors and are not simply attributed to specific features of the population studied.

Our study has several limitations. First, because RAISE-ETP did not include laboratory measures of social cognition, we relied on interviewer-rated proxy measures of socio-affective capacity and engagement with the interviewer, the construct validity of which is unknown. Until validated by future research, our interpretation of the relationship between socio-affective capacity, motivation and functional outcomes should be considered in the context of this limitation. While our motivation measure has been validated in previous research, it is similarly an interviewer-rated item. Future prospective research must build on our analyses and include well-validated and robust social cognition and motivation measures to confirm the causal influence of these domains on functional outcomes. We focused our analysis on clinical predictors (i.e. symptoms, cognition) that could be treatment targets, and did not include variables reflecting treatment engagement or prescribed antipsychotic dose equivalents, which could also impact short-term functional outcomes. Some variables had non-Gaussian distributions which could affect model performance. The primary method of analysis, GFCI, was recently developed, and although some benchmarking has been done, its finite-sample performance has not been completely characterized. GFCI generates a model of the plausible causal relationships from which the data were sampled, and its correctness relies on some assumptions that cannot be directly tested, but are plausible for the studied data. Statistical models can be wrong, however they are highly useful if accurate. GFCI also cannot identify the presence of synchronous causal cycles, if there are any.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to model direct causal pathways between socio-affective abilities, motivation, and social and occupational functioning in an early schizophrenia sample, using a data-driven causal discovery analysis. These findings have high clinical relevance and underscore the importance of specifically and vigorously targeting social cognitive processes and motivation as early as possible in the course of treatment to enhance social and occupational recovery. In addition, our findings suggest that promoting social functioning may have continued downstream effects on motivation, socio-affective capacities, and occupational functioning, and should also be a specific target of early intervention services.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003780.

Acknowledgements

Data and/or research tools used in the preparation of this manuscript were obtained from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Data Archive (NDA). NDA is a collaborative informatics system created by the National Institutes of Health to provide a national resource to support and accelerate research in mental health. Dataset identifier(s): doi 10.15154/1520935]. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or of the Submitters submitting original data to NDA.

Financial support

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants 1R03MH117254-01A1, P50MH119569, and 1F31MH124278. Additionally, this research was supported by the National Institutes of Health' National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, Grants TL1R002493 and UL1TR002494. EK received support for this work from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health Award Number UL1TR000114. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of interest

Drs. Ma and Kummerfeld and Ms. Miley report no financial relationships with commercial interests. Dr Meyer-Kalos has received consulting fees from the NAVIGATE program for first episode psychosis and Happify.com. Dr Bond has received consulting fees and/or research grants from Alkermes PLC, Myriad Genetics, and NuBiyota. Dr Vinogradov serves on the Scientific Advisory Boards for the following organizations: Mindstrong, Inc., Alkermes, Inc., and Psyberguide. She also has scientific collaborations with scientists at PositScience, Inc.

Footnotes

*

Drs. Kummerfeld and Vinogradov contributed equally as senior authors to this manuscript.

References

Bhagyavathi, H. D., Mehta, U. M., Thirthalli, J., Kumar, C. N., Kumar, J. K., Subbakrishna, D. K., & Gangadhar, B. N. (2015). Cascading and combined effects of cognitive deficits and residual symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia - A path-analytical approach. Psychiatry Research, 229(1–2), 264271. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 267277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browne, J., Penn, D. L., Meyer-Kalos, P. S., Mueser, K. T., Estroff, S. E., Brunette, M. F., … Kane, J. M. (2016). Psychological well-being and mental health recovery in the NIMH RAISE early treatment program. Schizophrenia Research, 185, 167–162. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.11.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campellone, T. R., Sanchez, A. H., & Kring, A. M. (2016). Defeatist performance beliefs, negative symptoms, and functional outcome in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(6), 13431352. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalano, L. T., Heerey, E. A., & Gold, J. M. (2018). The valuation of social rewards in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(6), 602611. doi: 10.1037/abn0000366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chickering, D. M. (2002). Optimal structure identification with greedy search. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 3, 507554.Google Scholar
Choi, J., Choi, K.-H., Felice Reddy, L., & Fiszdon, J. M. (2014). Measuring motivation in schizophrenia: Is a general state of motivation necessary for task-specific motivation? Schizophrenia Research, 153(1–3), 209213. doi: 10.1016/J.SCHRES.2014.01.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloutier, M., Sanon Aigbogun, M., Guerin, A., Nitulescu, R., Ramanakumar, A. V., Kamat, S. A., … Wu, E. (2016). The economic burden of schizophrenia in the United States in 2013. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(6), 764771. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m10278CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornblatt, B. A., Carrion, R. E., Addington, J., Seidman, L., Walker, E. F., Cannon, T. D., … Lencz, T. (2012). Risk factors for psychosis: Impaired social and role functioning. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(6), 12471257. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eack, S. M., Greenwald, D. P., Hogarty, S. S., Cooley, S. J., DiBarry, A. L., Montrose, D. M., & Keshavan, M. S. (2009). Cognitive enhancement therapy for early-course schizophrenia: Effects of a two-year randomized controlled trial. Psychiatric Services, 60(11), 14681476. doi: 10.1176/ps.2009.60.11.1468CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fareri, D., & Delgado, M. (2014). Social rewards and social networks in the human brain. The Neuroscientist, 20(4), 387402. doi: 10.1177/1073858414521869CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Gonzalo, S., Turon, M., Jodar, M., Pousa, E., Hernandez Rambla, C., García, R., & Palao, D. (2015). A new computerized cognitive and social cognition training specifically designed for patients with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder in early stages of illness: A pilot study. Psychiatry Research, 228(3), 501509. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fervaha, G., Foussias, G., Agid, O., & Remington, G. (2015). Motivational deficits in early schizophrenia: Prevalent, persistent, and key determinants of functional outcome. Schizophrenia Research, 166(1), 916. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.04.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fervaha, G., Foussias, G., Takeuchi, H., Agid, O., & Remington, G. (2015). Measuring motivation in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 169(1–3), 423426. doi: 10.1016/J.SCHRES.2015.09.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A. K. J., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M. D. G., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filia, K., Jackson, H., Cotton, S., & Killackey, E. (2019). Understanding what it means to be socially included for people with a lived experience of mental illness. The International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 65(5), 413424. doi: 10.1177/0020764019852657CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filia, K. M., Jackson, H. J., Cotton, S. M., Gardner, A., & Killackey, E. J. (2018). What is social inclusion? A thematic analysis of professional opinion. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 41(3), 183195. doi: 10.1037/prj0000304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, M., Nahum, M., Howard, E., Rowlands, A., Brandrett, B., Kermott, A., … Vinogradov, S. (2017). Supplementing intensive targeted computerized cognitive training with social cognitive exercises for people with schizophrenia: An interim report. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 40(1), 2132. doi: 10.1037/prj0000244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, D., Hodgekins, J., French, P., Marshall, M., Freemantle, N., Mccrone, P., … Birchwood, M. (2018). Social recovery therapy in combination with early intervention services for enhancement of social recovery in patients with first-episode psychosis (SUPEREDEN3): A single-blind, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(1), 4150. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30476-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fulford, D., Piper Meyer-Kalos, S., & Kim Mueser, T. (2020). Focusing on recovery goals improves motivation in first-episode psychosis. 55, 16291637. doi: 10.1007/s00127-020-01877-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulford, D., Piskulic, D., Addington, J., Kane, J. M., Schooler, N. R., & Mueser, K. T. (2017). Prospective relationships between motivation and functioning in recovery after a first episode of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(2), 369377. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx096CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gard, D. E., Fisher, M., Garrett, C., Genevsky, A., & Vinogradov, S. (2009). Motivation and its relationship to neurocognition, social cognition, and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 115(1), 7481. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.08.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, A., Filia, K., Killackey, E., & Cotton, S. (2019). The social inclusion of young people with serious mental illness: A narrative review of the literature and suggested future directions. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 53(1), 1526. doi: 10.1177/0004867418804065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Hellemann, G., Horan, W. P., Lee, J., & Wynn, J. K. (2012). From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: Modeling the role of ability and motivation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(12), 12161224. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Horan, W. P., & Lee, J. (2015). Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(10), 620631. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, D. W., Hanlon, T. E., & Carpenter, W. T. (1984). The quality of life scale: An instrument for rating the schizophrenic deficit syndrome. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10(3), 388398. doi: 10.1093/schbul/10.3.388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horan, W. P., Reddy, L. F., Barch, D. M., Buchanan, R. W., Dunayevich, E., Gold, J. M., … Green, M. F. (2015). Effort-Based decision-making paradigms for clinical trials in schizophrenia: Part 2—external validity and correlates. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(5), 10551065. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv090CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humensky, J. L., Essock, S. M., & Dixon, L. B. (2017). Characteristics associated with the pursuit of work and school among participants in a treatment program for first episode of psychosis. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 40(1), 108112. doi: 10.1037/prj0000256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, J. M., Robinson, D. G., Schooler, N. R., Mueser, K. T., Penn, D. L., Rosenheck, R. A., … Heinssen, R. K. (2016). Comprehensive versus usual community care for first-episode psychosis: 2-year outcomes from the NIMH RAISE early treatment program. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(4), 362372. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15050632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, S., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 261276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Gold, J. M., Poe, M. P., & Coughenour, L. (2004). The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2–3), 283297. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maathuis, M. H., Kalisch, M., & Bühlmann, P. (2009). Estimating high-dimensional intervention effects from observational data. The Annals of Statistics, 37(6A), 31333164. doi: 10.1214/09-AOS685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milev, P., Ho, B.-C., Arndt, S., & Andreasen, N. C. (2005). Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: A longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(3), 495506. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miley, K., Fisher, M., Nahum, M., Howard, E., Rowlands, A., Brandrett, B., … Vinogradov, S. (2020). Six month durability of targeted cognitive training supplemented with social cognition exercises in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 20, 100171. doi: 10.1016/J.SCOG.2019.100171Google ScholarPubMed
Mueser, K. T., Kim, M., Addingon, J., Mcgurk, S., Pratt, S., & Addington, D. (2017). Confirmatory factor analysis of the quality of life scale and new proposed factor structure for the quality of life scale-revised. Schizophrenia Research, 181, 117123. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.10.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagami, E., Hoe, M., & Brekke, J. S. (2010). The prospective relationships among intrinsic motivation, neurocognition, and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(5), 935948. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagami, E., Xie, B., Hoe, M., & Brekke, J. S. (2008). Intrinsic motivation, neurocognition and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia: Testing mediator and moderator effects. Schizophrenia Research, 105(1–3), 95104. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.06.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, R. M. G., Anderson, K. K., MacDougall, A., Manchanda, R., Harricharan, R., Subramanian, P., … Northcott, S. (2018). Stability of outcomes after 5 years of treatment in an early intervention programme. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 12(4), 720725. doi: 10.1111/eip.12450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogarrio, J. M., Spirtes, P., & Ramsey, J. (2016). A hybrid causal search algorithm for latent variable models. JMLR Workshop and Conference Proceedings, 52, 368379.Google ScholarPubMed
Penttilä, M., Jaä̈skel̈ainen, E., Hirvonen, N., Isohanni, M., & Miettunen, J. (2014). Duration of untreated psychosis as predictor of long-term outcome in schizophrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 205(2), 88–84. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.127753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phahladira, L., Luckhoff, H. K., Asmal, L., Kilian, S., Scheffler, F., Du Plessis, S., … Emsley, R. (2020). Early recovery in the first 24 months of treatment in first-episode schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Npj Schizophrenia, 6(1), 18. doi: 10.1038/s41537-019-0091-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, J. D. (2015). Scaling up Greedy Causal Search for Continuous Variables. ArXiv Preprint, arXiv:1507.Google Scholar
Roberts, D. L., Combs, D. R., Willoughby, M., Mintz, J., Gibson, C., Rupp, B., & Penn, D. L. (2014). A randomized, controlled trial of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 281298. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, B. R., Prestia, D., Twamley, E. W., Patterson, T. L., Bowie, C. R., & Harvey, P. D. (2014). Social competence versus negative symptoms as predictors of real world social functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 160(1–3), 136141. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santesteban-Echarri, O., Paino, M., Rice, S., González-Blanch, C., McGorry, P., Gleeson, J., & Alvarez-Jimenez, M. (2017). Predictors of functional recovery in first-episode psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 58, 5975. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saperstein, A. M., Fiszdon, J. M., & Bell, M. D. (2011). Intrinsic motivation as a predictor of work outcome after vocational rehabilitation in schizophrenia. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199(9), 672677. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318229d0ebCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlosser, D. A., Campellone, T. R., Truong, B., Etter, K., Vergani, S., Komaiko, K., & Vinogradov, S. (2018). Efficacy of PRIME, a mobile app intervention designed to improve motivation in young people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(5), 10101020. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Secher, R. G., Hjorthøj, C. R., Austin, S. F., Thorup, A., Jeppesen, P., Mors, O., & Nordentoft, M. (2015). Ten-year follow-up of the OPUS specialized early intervention trial for patients with a first episode of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(3), 617626. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. The Neuroscientist, 17(1), 1824. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shen, X., Ma, S., Vemuri, P., & Simon, G. (2020). Challenges and opportunities with causal discovery algorithms: Application to Alzheimer's pathophysiology. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 2975. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59669-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparks, A., Mcdonald, S., Lino, B., O'donnell, M., & Green, M. J. (2010). Social cognition, empathy and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 122, 172178. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spirtes, P., Glymour, C. N., & Scheines, R. (2000). Causation, prediction, and search. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stekhoven, D. J., Moraes, I., Sveinbjörnsson, G., Hennig, L., Maathuis, M. H., & Bühlmann, P. (2012). Causal stability ranking. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England), 28(21), 28192823. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts523Google ScholarPubMed
Stroup, T. S., Mcevoy, J. P., Swartz, M. S., Byerly, M. J., Qlick, I. D., Canive, J. M., … Lieberman, J. A. (2003). The National Institute of Mental Health clinical antipsychotic trials of intervention effectiveness (CATIE) project: Schizophrenia trial design and protocol development. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(1), 1531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subramaniam, K., Luks, T. L., Fisher, M., Simpson, G. V., Nagarajan, S., & Vinogradov, S. (2012). Computerized cognitive training restores neural activity within the reality monitoring network in schizophrenia. Neuron, 73(4), 842853. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swartz, M. S., Perkins, D. O., Stroup, T. S., Davis, S. M., Capuano, G., Rosenheck, & R. A., … CATIE Investigators. (2007). Effects of antipsychotic medications on psychosocial functioning in patients with chronic schizophrenia: Findings from the NIMH CATIE study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(3), 428436. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarbox, S. I., & Pogue-Geile, M. F. (2008). Development of social functioning in preschizophrenia children and adolescents: A systematic review. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 561583. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.34.4.561CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taruttis, F., Spang, R., & Engelmann, J. C. (2015). A statistical approach to virtual cellular experiments: Improved causal discovery using accumulation IDA (aIDA). Bioinformatics (Oxford, England), 31(23), 38073814. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btv461Google ScholarPubMed
Wiersma, D., Wanderling, J., Dragomirecka, E., Ganev, K., Harrison, G., Der Heiden, W. A., … Walsh, D. (2000). Social disability in schizophrenia: Its development and prediction over 15 years in incidence cohorts in six European centres. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1155. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799002627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wunderink, L., Nieboer, R. M., Wiersma, D., Sytema, S., & Nienhuis, F. J. (2013). Recovery in remitted first-episode psychosis at 7 years of follow-up of an early dose reduction/discontinuation or maintenance treatment strategy. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(9), 913. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Study sample characteristics and variable descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Greedy fast causal inference edge types and meaning. *Note: When viewed in Tetrad 6.6.0, this edge type is represented as a thin green edge. We have used a blue edge here to distinguish the edge more easily from the thick green directed edge above.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Study 1 complete PAG and functional outcome subgraph. All variables represent baseline unless specified as 6M (6 month). Alcohol use = days of alcohol use in previous month; Cannabis use = days of cannabis use in the past month; CGI = Clinical Global Impressions-Severity score; Depression = Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia; DUP = duration of untreated psychosis (log transformed); Global Cognition = Brief Assessment of Cognition composite Z score; Medication Beliefs = Brief evaluation of medication influences and beliefs total score; MH Recovery = Mental Health Recovery Measure total score; PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptom Scale; SaC = Socio-affective capacity; Stigma = Stigma scale total score; Treatment Group = randomized into coordinated specialty care v. Treatment as Usual. Wellbeing = Well-being scale total score.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Study 1 augmented rolled graph. *Causal effect size for baseline motivation on baseline occupational functioning. The causal effect size from 6-month motivation to 6-month occupational functioning is 0.92. ES = effect size. + 6 indicates a 6-month time cycle over which the effect occurred.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Study 2: validation complete PAG and functional outcome subgraph. All variables represent baseline unless specified as 6M (6-month). Antipsychotic years = years of antipsychotic use; CGI = Clinical Global Impressions-Severity score; Depression = Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia; Facial Affect Recognition = Face emotion discrimination task correct responses; Global Cognition = Cognition composite Z score; PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, SaC = Socio-affective capacity.

Supplementary material: File

Miley et al. supplementary material

Miley et al. supplementary material

Download Miley et al. supplementary material(File)
File 168.1 KB