Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:45:39.030Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The psychological impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns: a review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies and natural experiments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 January 2021

Gabriele Prati*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Italy, Piazza Aldo Moro, 90, 47521Cesena (FC), Italy
Anthony D. Mancini
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Pace University, Marks Hall, Rm 33, 861 Bedford Road, Pleasantville, NY10570, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Gabriele Prati, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Lockdowns to control the spread of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) have had profound effects on everyday life worldwide, but their effect on mental health remains unclear because available meta-analyses and reviews rely mostly on cross-sectional studies. We conducted a rapid review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies and natural experiments investigating the relationship between COVID-19 lockdowns and mental health. A total of 25 studies involving 72 004 participants and 58 effect sizes were analyzed. Using a random effects model, we found that lockdowns had small effects on mental health symptoms, g = 0.17, s.e. = 0.05, 95% CI (0.06–0.24), p = 0.001, but the effects on positive psychological functioning, g = −0.12, s.e. = 0.11, 95% CI (−0.33 to 0.09), p = 0.27, were not significant. Multivariate analysis of effect sizes revealed significant and relatively small effect sizes for anxiety and depression, while those for social support, loneliness, general distress, negative affect, and suicide risk were not significant. The results indicated substantial heterogeneity among studies, but meta-regression analyses found no significant moderation effects for mean age, gender, continent, COVID-19 death rate, days of lockdown, publication status or study design. The psychological impact of COVID-19 lockdowns is small in magnitude and highly heterogeneous, suggesting that lockdowns do not have uniformly detrimental effects on mental health and that most people are psychologically resilient to their effects.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

The spread of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) has resulted in an unprecedented series of lockdowns worldwide. Although these lockdowns have varied in stringency between and within countries, they have substantially altered people's daily lives globally, affecting their work, leisure activities, livelihood, and capacity for in-person social interaction. It is difficult to draw definitive conclusions regarding the impact of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) lockdowns from prior research, because the COVID-19 lockdowns have marked qualitative differences from those of previous pandemics (e.g. the substantial socio-economic impact, greater degree of stringency, and the variable nature of enforcement). Although one prior systematic review that suggests that general coronavirus infections have minimal effects on symptoms of mental illness (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Chesney, Oliver, Pollak, McGuire, Fusar-Poli and David2020), this prior review does not address the broader psychological impact of the pandemic lockdowns on general population samples.

In the present review and meta-analysis, we sought to focus on the emerging literature on COVID-19 lockdowns to investigate the psychological impact of lockdown on the general population. Specifically, we reviewed and meta-analyzed studies that included between-group or within-group controls, allowing for clearer inferences regarding the impact of lockdown on mental health. Following previous research (e.g. Haider et al., Reference Haider, Osman, Gadzekpo, Akipede, Asogun, Ansumana and McCoy2020), we defined lockdown as an emergency and temporary measure imposed by governmental authorities that (1) applies to a city, region, or nation to prevent the spread of the COVID-19 virus; (2) is mandatory and applied indiscriminately to a general population; and (3) requires citizens to stay at home and refrain from or limit social and economic activities outside the home.

Three recent systematic reviews investigated the broader psychological impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the general public worldwide. Luo, Guo, Yu, Jiang, and Wang (Reference Luo, Guo, Yu, Jiang and Wang2020) revealed a pooled prevalence of anxiety and depression of 32 and 27%, respectively, among the general public. Similar results emerged in Salari et al. (Reference Salari, Hosseinian-Far, Jalali, Vaisi-Raygani, Rasoulpoor, Mohammadi and Khaledi-Paveh2020), who found the pooled prevalence of stress, anxiety, and depression, is 29.6, 31.9, and 33.7% respectively. Vindegaard and Benros (Reference Vindegaard and Benros2020) also found increased levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms along with general mental health symptoms. Based on their findings, Luo and colleagues concluded that ‘the COVID-19 pandemic has caused heavy psychological impact among medical workers and the general public’ (Luo et al., Reference Luo, Guo, Yu, Jiang and Wang2020, p. 7). Similarly, Salari and colleagues indicate that the COVID-19 pandemic ‘has not only raised concerns over general public health, but has also caused a number of psychological and mental disorders’ (Salari et al., Reference Salari, Hosseinian-Far, Jalali, Vaisi-Raygani, Rasoulpoor, Mohammadi and Khaledi-Paveh2020, pp. 8–11). Finally, Vindegaard and Benros suggest that ‘currently data is scarce, but indicates that mental health is affected in the general public’ (Vindegaard & Benros, Reference Vindegaard and Benros2020, p. 10).

These prior reviews raise important concerns regarding the mental health impact of COVID-19 lockdowns, but they are based on studies with significant methodological limitations, including cross-sectional designs and absence of control groups. As Meda and Slongo (Reference Meda and Slongo2020) note, caution should be used when reporting conclusions on the psychological impact of the COVID-19 pandemic from cross-sectional studies lacking a proper control group. Given that prior reviews relied largely on cross-sectional studies, a review of more compelling evidence for the mental health effects of lockdown is needed. A stronger evidence base would have critical implications for policymaking and clinical interventions around the world.

The degree of media attention devoted to the psychological impact of COVID-19 may also create expectancy effects, consistent with the self-fulfilling prophecy (Merton, Reference Merton1948) and the Pygmalion effect (Rosenthal & Jacobson, Reference Rosenthal and Jacobson1968). The capacity for resilience in response to the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e. a relatively stable trajectory of healthy psychological functioning) has sometimes been discounted, though it is the modal response to widely varying forms of acute adversity (e.g. Bonanno, Westphal, & Mancini, Reference Bonanno, Westphal and Mancini2011; see also: Mancini, Reference Mancini2020). In sum, on the one hand, previous systematic reviews of cross-sectional research suggest that national lockdowns may have a heavy psychological impact. On the other hand, a resilience perspective suggests that the psychological impact of national lockdowns may be relatively small.

Purpose of the present study

In the present study, we took advantage of a growing evidence base using more sophisticated methodologies. When experimental randomized trials are neither feasible nor ethical, both longitudinal within-person designs (with at least one data collection point before and one during the lockdown) and natural experiments involving a control group provide a methodologically more rigorous test of the effect of the COVID-19 lockdown. Because many lockdowns happened at a national level, natural experiments involving an appropriate control group were difficult to undertake. Nevertheless, longitudinal studies that examine within-person change before and after lockdowns provide important information on the psychological impact of lockdowns. The number of longitudinal studies and natural experiments assessing the effect of the COVID-19 lockdowns on mental health among the general population has multiplied during the last months. To provide a more rigorous assessment of the influence of the COVID-19 lockdowns, we conducted a review and meta-analysis of this evidence base to determine the psychological impact of COVID-19 lockdowns on the general population. We focused on broad dimensions of psychological functioning, including mental health symptoms, such as anxiety and depression, and positive psychological functioning such as well-being and life-satisfaction, consistent with the idea that these dimensions are separable but related constructs (Keyes, Reference Keyes2005). In addition, given the effect of lockdowns on in-person social interaction, we also examined feelings of loneliness and social support as ancillary outcomes.

Method

Knowledge synthesis

To synthesize the evidence in a timely manner, we chose a rapid review approach (e.g. Arksey & O'Malley, Reference Arksey and O'Malley2005; Khangura, Konnyu, Cushman, Grimshaw, & Moher, Reference Khangura, Konnyu, Cushman, Grimshaw and Moher2012; Tricco et al., Reference Tricco, Antony, Zarin, Strifler, Ghassemi, Ivory and Straus2015) over a systematic review methodology. Using guidance from Arksey and O'Malley (Reference Arksey and O'Malley2005), we compiled a rapid review protocol (available from the corresponding author upon request).

Information sources and literature search

To identify potentially relevant studies for inclusion, we adopted a search strategy that involved different sources: (1) electronic databases; (2) reference lists; (3) hand-searching of key journals; (4) existing networks; and (5) internet searches for unpublished papers. Specifically, searches were made on four electronic databases: Scopus, Web of Science, PubMed, and PsycInfo. We limited our search from January 2020 until June 2020. In addition, we undertook a gray literature search using Google Scholar. Finally, we consulted all citations of eligible articles and relevant review articles for supplementary references that were missed in the initial search (i.e. reference lists), hand-searched key journals to identify articles that have been missed in database (i.e. hand-searching of key journals), and we contacted experts in this field using existing knowledge and networks (i.e. existing networks).

Key search terms for mental health were these: adaptation, anxiety, depression, quality of life, mental health, mental illness, psychological symptoms, psychiatric symptoms, resilience, coping, stress, quality of life, well-being, distress, self-esteem, PTSD, loneliness, fear, social support, embeddedness, social cohesion, post-traumatic, post-traumatic, benefit findings, positive benefits, stress-related growth, and thriving. For the COVID-19 pandemic, the terms included COVID-19, coronavirus disease 2019, 2019-nCoV, novel coronavirus, SARS-CoV-2, quarantine, lockdown, and pandemic.

Criteria for including studies in the review

Outcomes

We included for review primary human research studies that measured change or difference post-lockdown in at least one outcome related to mental health symptoms or positive psychological functioning. Mental health symptoms included assessments of depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder, suicidal ideation, negative affect, substance use, sleep disturbances, and general distress. Positive psychological functioning outcomes included assessments of satisfaction with life, positive affect, well-being, and quality of life. Scores on mental health symptoms and positive functioning were coded such as higher scores correspond to greater mental health symptoms and higher well-being, respectively. Social outcomes included loneliness and social support. In addition, we conducted subgroup analyses separately for each outcome when data were available from at least three studies.

Study design

We included studies that met the following criteria: (a) longitudinal designs assessing psychological functioning before and after COVID-19 lockdowns using the same instruments; (b) natural experiments comparing participants who were in lockdown with those who did not have such restrictions; (c) natural experiments with at least two (i.e. before and during the COVID-19 pandemic) cross-sectional data collection points (with different individuals) in which samples were matched or collected using the same methodology. We excluded retrospective studies and studies comparing the scores to norms or to assessments obtained from different studies. Our focus was on the general population, without age restriction, and, therefore, we excluded studies that focused on specific populations such as health care workers, survivors, or patient populations as well as studies investigating home self-quarantine. Guidelines, reviews, commentary, and non-English articles were also excluded. No studies were excluded based on sample size or study duration. Finally, we excluded studies with incomplete reporting of findings and statistics necessary for computation of effect sizes. For instance, we excluded studies using regression coefficients as meta-analytic input because this approach results in biased findings (Roth, Le, Oh, Van Iddekinge, & Bobko, Reference Roth, Le, Oh, Van Iddekinge and Bobko2018).

Study selection

The literature search resulted in 2158 publications (Fig. 1). Moreover, we identified 40 publications through other sources (i.e. reference lists, hand-searching of key journals, existing networks, internet searches for unpublished paper). After removing duplicates, 1248 publications were screened for eligibility. Two reviewers (the authors of the present article) independently applied the inclusion and exclusion criteria to the records which were identified through the search. The reviewers screened for inclusion all publication titles and abstracts (n = 1248). The percentage of agreement between raters was high (98.7%). All disagreements were successfully resolved through discussion. The reviewers then independently assessed the full text of 63 publications for eligibility. There was no disagreement concerning the eligibility of studies identified for inclusion.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram of the study selection process.

Data items and data abstraction process

G.P. extracted data from the eligible studies into a customized Excel spreadsheet. The extracted data were independently verified by A.M. For each selected study, we recorded information as follows: first author, year of publication, study location(s), sources title, study populations (general population, adolescents, children, persons aged 60 or over), aims of the study, methodology, time passed since lockdown, peer-review status, participants' mean age, percentage of women, type of outcome measure, and main results. We conducted the following quality checks at each step: iterative consultation on data and any discrepancies, careful cross-checking of the data collected, and consensus decisions around methodology.

Statistical analysis

We conducted a meta-analysis using the R metafor package version 2.4-0 (Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2010). Only outcomes with data available from at least three studies were included. We calculated and transformed effect sizes to the bias-corrected Hedges' g following the guidelines of Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein (Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). Values of 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 were considered benchmarks for small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). We conducted a random-effects meta-analysis using restricted maximum likelihood as a heterogeneity variance estimator (Langan, Higgins, & Simmonds, Reference Langan, Higgins and Simmonds2015). We assessed statistical heterogeneity using the I 2 statistic which represents the percentage of total variation across effect size estimates that is due to heterogeneity rather than chance. According to Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, and Altman (Reference Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman2003), I 2 values of 25, 50, and 75% are considered low, moderate, and high heterogeneity, respectively. To investigate publication bias, we used Begg's adjusted rank correlation test for funnel plot asymmetry (Begg & Mazumdar, Reference Begg and Mazumdar1994). In addition, we used a fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis (Orwin, Reference Orwin1983). This test quantifies the association between sample size and effect size, providing an estimate of the number of studies with null results that would be needed to reduce the average effect size to half the observed effect size.

We estimated effect size for different categories of mental health indicators. Effect size estimates for each category of outcomes are not statistically independent because each participant may be assessed using several different measures of outcome. To handle dependence among study effects, we conducted a full multivariate analysis of stochastically dependent effect sizes based on a linear random-effects model (assuming ρ = 0.7) using a robust variance estimation (Hedges, Reference Hedges, Hedges, Cooper and Valentine2019).

To explain heterogeneity between studies we performed meta-regression analysis for outcomes with at least 20 effect size estimates. The following moderators were included: average age of participants, percentage of female participants, days passed since lockdown, peer-review status, study design, continent, and COVID-19 death rate. To determine COVID-19 death rate, we calculated COVID-19-related mortality per 1 000 000 population in the country or region of the study sample and at the midpoint of the time interval when data were collected (Johns Hopkins University Center for Systems Science and Engineering, 2020). We reported results from both univariate and multivariate meta-regression models. To assess the robustness of our meta-regression models, we conducted permutation tests (Higgins & Thompson, Reference Higgins and Thompson2004) and reported permutation-based p-values and confidence intervals. Unless stated otherwise, we set α at 0.05 and all tests were two-sided.

Results

We retrieved 2158 abstracts from the electronic databases and 40 additional records from other sources, of which 1248 remained after removing duplicates (Fig. 1). Following the screening of title and abstract, 63 articles were identified as potentially eligible studies. We assessed the full-text articles for eligibility, and 25 articles were included in the present meta-analysis because they fulfilled all eligibility criteria. This meta-analysis included 25 articles providing 58 effect size estimates. Of the 40 additional records from other sources, five were retained in the analysis. Online Supplementary Table S1 provides an overview of all included studies. Thirteen studies were conducted in Europe (Bojanowska, Kaczmarek, Kościelniak, & Urbańska, Reference Bojanowska, Kaczmarek, Kościelniak and Urbańska2020; Daly & Robinson, Reference Daly and Robinson2020; Jackson, Garnett, Shahab, Oldham, & Brown, Reference Jackson, Garnett, Shahab, Oldham and Brown2020; Kwong et al., Reference Kwong, Pearson, Adams, Northstone, Tilling, Smith and Timpson2020; Meda et al., Reference Meda, Pardini, Slongo, Bodini, Rigobello, Visioli and Novara2020; Niedzwiedz et al., Reference Niedzwiedz, Green, Benzeval, Campbell, Craig, Demou and Katikireddi2020; Ozamiz-Etxebarria, Dosil-Santamaria, Picaza-Gorrochategui, & Idoiaga-Mondragon, Reference Ozamiz-Etxebarria, Dosil-Santamaria, Picaza-Gorrochategui and Idoiaga-Mondragon2020; Pierce et al., Reference Pierce, Hope, Ford, Hatch, Hotopf, John and Abel2020; Recchi et al., Reference Recchi, Ferragina, Helmeid, Pauly, Safi, Sauger and Schradie2020; Schützwohl & Mergel, Reference Schützwohl and Mergel2020; Shanahan et al., Reference Shanahan, Steinhoff, Bechtiger, Murray, Nivette, Hepp and Eisner2020; Stevenson, Wakefield, Drury, & Felsner, Reference Stevenson, Wakefield, Drury and Felsner2020; Zacher & Rudolph, Reference Zacher and Rudolph2020), six in Asia (Guo, Feng, Wang, & van Ijzendoorn, Reference Guo, Feng, Wang and van Ijzendoorn2020; Lei et al., Reference Lei, Huang, Zhang, Yang, Yang and Xu2020; Li, Cao, Leung, & Mak, Reference Li, Cao, Leung and Mak2020; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Wei, Jia, Shang, Sun and Liu2020; Wang & Zhao, Reference Wang and Zhao2020; Xin et al., Reference Xin, Luo, She, Yu, Li, Wang and Lau2020), five in North America (Bryan, Bryan, & Baker, Reference Bryan, Bryan and Baker2020; Gratz et al., Reference Gratz, Tull, Richmond, Edmonds, Scamaldo and Rose2020; Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Lee, Aschwanden, Sesker, Strickhouser, Terracciano and Sutin2020; Tull et al., Reference Tull, Edmonds, Scamaldo, Richmond, Rose and Gratz2020; Zimmermann, Bledsoe, & Papa, Reference Zimmermann, Bledsoe and Papa2020), and one in Oceania (Sibley et al., Reference Sibley, Greaves, Satherley, Wilson, Overall, Lee and Barlow2020). The majority of the included studies (n = 16) were peer-reviewed. Twelve studies used a within-person longitudinal design in which the participants were assessed before and after lockdown orders. Thirteen studies used a natural experiment design comparing regions or groups with and without lockdown orders. The number of days passed since lockdown varied from one to 60. All studies involved adult participants. The percentage of female participants ranged from 40 to 81%. Whereas 19 studies reported effects of lockdown on mental health functioning (e.g. depression, anxiety, general distress), six studies reported effects on positive psychological functioning (e.g. well-being, life satisfaction). Ten studies reported effect size estimates for depression and nine for anxiety. Seven studies provided effect size estimates for general distress. Five studies reported effect size estimates for social support. Three studies reported effect size estimates for loneliness and suicide risk.

Meta-analysis

We calculated two summary estimates (estimated average effects), one for positive psychological functioning and another one for mental health symptoms (Table 1). Concerning mental health symptoms, the effect of lockdown was small and significant, g = 0.17, s.e. = 0.05, 95% CI (0.070.26), p < 0.001, with I 2 = 99.05%, 95% CI (98.22–99.58), signifying large heterogeneity. Figure 2 depicts the forest plot on the impact of lockdown on effect size estimates for mental health symptoms. The Begg's adjusted rank correlation test for funnel plot asymmetry did not indicate the presence of publication bias, τ = −0.04, p = 0.823. Fail-safe N was equal to 19, indicating that 19 studies with non-significant results would have to be added to the meta-analysis to decrease the average effect size to half the observed effect size.

Table 1. Univariate analysis of effect sizes, heterogeneity, and fail-safe N

Note. k refers to the number of studies available for computation of a specific effect size (Hedges' g).

a Anxiety, depression, substance use, sleep disturbances, suicide risk, negative affect, and general distress symptoms.

b Anxiety and posttraumatic stress symptoms.

c Symptoms of general mental illness, perceived stress, psychological distress, and emotional distress. Higher scores on positive functioning correspond to higher well-being.

Fig. 2. Forest plot of the 20 effect size estimates for the association between lockdown and mental health symptoms.

Note. The year of publication was 2020 for all studies.

The effect of lockdown on positive mental health was slightly smaller in magnitude than mental health symptoms and non-significant, g = −0.12, s.e. = 0.11, 95% CI (−0.33 to 0.09), p = 0.274, with I 2 = 98.57%, 95% CI (96.19–99.78), indicating large heterogeneity. Figure 3 displays the forest plot on the impact of lockdown on effect size estimates for positive functioning. The Begg's adjusted rank correlation test for funnel plot asymmetry did not reveal evidence of publication bias, τ = −0.20, p = 0.719.

Fig. 3. Forest plot of the six effect size estimates for the association between lockdown and positive functioning.

Note. The year of publication was 2020 for all studies. Higher scores on positive functioning correspond to higher well-being.

Next, we conducted a multivariate analysis of effect sizes. Using multivariate meta-analyses, it is possible to estimate the effect sizes for different dependent variables simultaneously in one model, while taking the relationship between the dependent variables into account. The results from basic central tendency statistics and publication bias are presented in Table 2. The results were very similar to the univariate analysis. The effect size estimates for anxiety and depression were quite small and significant, while those of social support, loneliness, distress, negative affect, and suicide risk were not significant. Except for social support, the effect size estimates showed very high heterogeneity. Table 3 displays results from full multivariate analysis of effect sizes based on a linear random-effects model. Consistent with univariate analysis, the only significant effect size estimates were those of anxiety and depression.

Table 2. Multivariate analysis of effect sizes based on random-effects model

Note: Mental health symptoms were not included in the multivariate model because they were calculated using scores from anxiety, depression, substance use, sleep disturbances, suicide risk, negative affect, and general distress symptoms.

Table 3. Results of univariate and multivariate meta-regression models

Note. Study design was coded as 1 = longitudinal, 0 = other study design; publication status was coded as 0 = non peer-reviewed, 1 = peer-reviewed. We reported results from permutation test on multivariate meta-regression model.

Moderator analyses

Table 3 summarizes results of the univariate and multivariate meta-regression analyses for mental health symptoms. The omnibus test for the multivariate univariate model was not significant, Q = 6.73, p = 0.666. In both univariate and multivariate analysis, we found no effects for mean age, gender, continent, COVID-19 death rate, days of lockdown, publication status or study design.

Discussion

The findings of our meta-analysis indicate a small but significant effect of COVID-19 lockdowns on mental health symptoms among the general population. Subgroup analyses indicated that depression and anxiety showed consistently small but significant effects of lockdown. However, we did not find evidence that lockdowns reduced positive psychological functioning, such as well-being, life satisfaction, or well-being. Furthermore, we did not find evidence that COVID-19 lockdowns increased loneliness or decreased perceptions of social support. Together these findings suggest that COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns had a selective and modest impact on mental health indicators but no effect on positive functioning (Keyes, Reference Keyes2005).

Although the point estimates indicate a small effect, this estimate should be interpreted cautiously given the relatively wide confidence intervals. Indeed, these findings should be understood in the context of substantial heterogeneity in the effect size estimates. Moderation analyses offered little to illuminate this heterogeneity, as days in lockdown, continent, publication status, COVID-19 death rate, sample composition, and study design were not associated with effect size. Heterogeneity between studies is likely due, in part, to the methodological challenges of studying the effects of lockdowns. These challenges included variation in research design, sampling strategies, mental health measures, and the availability of pre-pandemic assessments. Nevertheless, the heterogeneity in effect sizes is consistent with the wide degree of inter-individual variation in people's reactions to acute stress (Bonanno & Mancini, Reference Bonanno and Mancini2012). It is also consistent with the idea that the effects of lockdowns are not uniform and likely depend on a host of other contextual and person-centered factors.

Among the contextual factors that might have played an important role are citizens' attitudes toward lockdown measures. Indeed, there is evidence that positive attitudes toward lockdown measures predicted higher well-being and lower mental health symptoms during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy (Prati, Reference Prati2020). Polling data suggest that, in the early months of the pandemic, public approval of lockdown measures was high across countries YouGov (2020). Had attitudes toward lockdown been more negative, it is plausible that their effects on mental health would have been considerably more detrimental. However, this remains an open question and an important challenge for future research.

Most studies investigated the impact of COVID-19 lockdowns on anxiety and depression. In subgroup analyses, these were the only outcomes to produce significant effects. On the one hand, the inclusion of more studies may have allowed for effects to emerge for additional outcomes by increasing the statistical power of our tests. On the other hand, the effects were remarkably similar in magnitude across almost all outcomes, and we believe that if additional significant effects emerged because of increased power they would be likely to remain small.

In contrast to mental health outcomes, it is interesting to note that social support and loneliness were substantially unaffected by COVID-19 lockdowns. Although there have been concerns that the pandemic and the related containment measures would cause large increases in loneliness in scientific publications (e.g. Fiorillo & Gorwood, Reference Fiorillo and Gorwood2020; Killgore, Cloonan, Taylor, & Dailey, Reference Killgore, Cloonan, Taylor and Dailey2020), we did not find evidence supporting these predictions. It is plausible that containment measures such as lockdown, social distancing, and self-isolation, altered the ways in which people interacted but did not alter their perceived quality. Early in the pandemic, for example, a sharp increase in texting, social media, and video conference activity was observed (Richter, Reference Richter2020). These technologies may have facilitated people's adaptation to the social restrictions imposed by lockdowns.

Another key point is that the widespread shared experience of the COVID-19 pandemic may have strengthened social cohesion and closeness because people may feel that ‘we are all in this together’ (Courtet, Olié, Debien, & Vaiva, Reference Courtet, Olié, Debien and Vaiva2020; Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Lee, Aschwanden, Sesker, Strickhouser, Terracciano and Sutin2020; Tull et al., Reference Tull, Edmonds, Scamaldo, Richmond, Rose and Gratz2020). Although there is some indication from repeated cross-sectional surveys that this may not be the case for COVID-19 (Borkowska & Laurence, Reference Borkowska and Laurence2020), there is prior evidence that disasters and pandemics can stimulate social cohesion and solidarity (e.g. Calo-Blanco, Kovářík, Mengel, & Romero, Reference Calo-Blanco, Kovářík, Mengel and Romero2017; Hawdon & Ryan, Reference Hawdon and Ryan2011). After the severe acute respiratory syndrome epidemic in Hong Kong in 2003, for example, people reported increased feelings of embeddedness in the community and caring for friends and family members (Lau et al., Reference Lau, Chi, Cummins, Lee, Chou and Chung2008; Lau, Yang, Tsui, Pang, & Wing, Reference Lau, Yang, Tsui, Pang and Wing2006). The Fukushima nuclear disaster in 2011 also enhanced the importance of social connections (Uchida, Takahashi, & Kawahara, Reference Uchida, Takahashi and Kawahara2014). Indeed, as suggested by the ‘tend and befriend’ and ‘psychosocial gains from adversity’ theoretical perspectives, adversity can have favorable effects on affiliative behavior and potentially improve psychosocial functioning (Mancini, Reference Mancini2019; Mancini, Littleton, & Grills, Reference Mancini, Littleton and Grills2016; Taylor, Reference Taylor2006). Despite the substantial effects of lockdowns on everyday life, the pandemic's capacity to enhance a sense of supportive others may have been a key factor in the small effect of the lockdown on mental health symptoms.

These potentially beneficial effects may also explain why we did not find evidence of significant increased suicide risk associated with lockdown (Joiner, Hollar, & Orden, Reference Joiner, Hollar and Orden2006; Reger, Stanley, & Joiner, Reference Reger, Stanley and Joiner2020). Indeed, there is evidence that social isolation and loneliness are associated with suicide risk (e.g. Calati et al., Reference Calati, Ferrari, Brittner, Oasi, Olié, Carvalho and Courtet2019; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). The experience of the COVID-19 pandemic may have changed the way people view health and mortality and make suicide less likely (Reger et al., Reference Reger, Stanley and Joiner2020). As a result of the struggle with highly challenging life crises, people may change their views on mortality and experience positive psychological change including an increased appreciation for life (e.g. Tedeschi & Calhoun, Reference Tedeschi and Calhoun2004). However, we acknowledge that the present findings apply to the general population and that the impact of lockdown on suicide is likely to differ according to contextual and individual characteristics (John, Pirkis, Gunnell, Appleby, & Morrissey, Reference John, Pirkis, Gunnell, Appleby and Morrissey2020).

One way of understanding the small or non-significant effects of lockdown is people's innate capacity for psychological resilience. A considerable literature on widely varied stressful experiences has found that most people experience a stable pattern of adaptive functioning, or resilience, after an acute stressor (Bonanno, Reference Bonanno2004). The present results remind us that – even after one of the most pervasive and restrictive interventions ever imposed on human beings – the average impact is small or non-significant (depending on the type of outcome), suggesting that most people retain their capacity for psychological resilience.

The present findings should not be taken as evidence that mental health problems do not occur in response to lockdowns. Moreover, the current findings only apply to the first lockdowns that were enforced during the early months of the COVID-19 pandemic. Given that the COVID-19 pandemic is likely to persist through 2021, the question of the psychological impact of repeated or prolonged lockdowns will remain open. Another important point is that small effects do not mean that the impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns on mental health is trivial in applied terms. Even a very small effect size applied to the whole population could pose a significant public health problem. Moreover, this small effect size represents a mean value throughout the samples.

Meta-regression analysis failed to demonstrate that duration of lockdown moderated the effect of COVID-19 lockdowns on mental health. This finding does not mean that longer COVID-19 lockdown durations do not have any consequences for the mental health of the population. For instance, there are clear social and economic costs of lockdown policies (e.g. Miles, Stedman, & Heald, Reference Miles, Stedman and Heald2020), such as lost jobs and business closures which are thought to exert an important impact on mental health (e.g. Crayne, Reference Crayne2020). We contend that longer COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns policy per se may have a small and transient impact on population mental health as long as the social and economic consequences are limited. The findings that effect size estimates were not associated with both lockdown duration and death rates are in line with the results of a longitudinal study conducted in China during the pandemic (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Pan, Wan, Tan, Xu, McIntyre and Ho2020). Although the number of COVID-19 deaths increased from the first to the second wave, no significant longitudinal changes in stress, anxiety and depression levels were found, and traumatic stress symptoms even decreased. Confidence in public health measures to control the spread of SARS-CoV-2 may help offset the psychological distress caused by lockdown. Finally, we point out that during a pandemic there are different stressors that may affect mental health, such as financial insecurity, perceived susceptibility to COVID-19 infections, and job stress. These stressors are not equally shared across countries, groups, and individuals. The findings of our meta-analysis cannot estimate or rule out the importance of other factors involved in a pandemic.

The meta-analytical findings should be interpreted with a number of limitations in mind. First, the small number of studies that investigated the outcomes of the present meta-analysis such as loneliness and suicide risk raise concerns about statistical power. We cannot rule out the possibility that non-significant effects are simply a power issue rather than a null finding. Due to the low number of studies investigating some of our outcomes, it is currently unclear whether our results can be generalized to these domains. A least five studies are needed to reasonably achieve adequate power from random-effects meta-analyses (Jackson & Turner, Reference Jackson and Turner2017), but it is worth noting that many of our analyses were based on five to 20 studies. Second, regarding participants' age, we were not able to find any studies involving children and adolescents and participants from different geographic areas such as Africa and South America. The psychological impact of lockdown on children may be different from that of adults, particularly in the context of widespread school closures. Therefore, the findings of our meta-analysis cannot be generalized to children and adolescents or to people all over the world. Indeed, while at a population level we found a small psychological impact of lockdown, we cannot rule out that specific subgroups would show different effects (e.g. children, care home residents, healthcare workers, people with preexisting mental health disorders, and people infected with COVID-19 virus). Third, the absence of a relationship between age and gender and effect size estimates should be interpreted with caution because the number of studies was not large. Fourth, moderator analyses of individual characteristics were limited to gender and age, because characteristics such as socioeconomic status, education, and working status were not reported in some studies. Fifth, another limitation of the review is that it does not tell us about the longer-term impact of lockdown on mental health. This is an especially important direction for future research, given that repeated or prolonged lockdowns were introduced to prevent the spread of the COVID-19 virus and that infections themselves may contribute to psychiatric disorders (Taquet, Luciano, Geddes, & Harrison, Reference Taquet, Luciano, Geddes and Harrison2020). Sixth, in the current analysis, we did not take into account the stringency of the lockdown. We acknowledge that stringency of lockdowns varied somewhat, both across countries and across time. However, as we noted, all lockdowns share unique features, insofar as they restrict a wide variety of normative human behaviors. Future studies should focus on the psychological impact of different forms of lockdowns.

Conclusion

The results of our meta-analysis indicate that the impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns on mental health symptoms among the general population is small in magnitude. Therefore, claims that COVID-19 pandemic lockdown policies have a dramatic effect on population mental health are unsupported by the current findings. On the contrary, the findings suggest that people are largely psychologically resilient to stay-at-home orders, lockdowns, and similar restrictions that were enforced at the national or regional level around the world in response to the COVID-19 pandemic. Given the substantial degree of heterogeneity in our data, we posit that the impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns on mental health may be different across different social groups and across different contexts and countries. Disparate health impacts have been an important focus during the COVID-19 pandemic and can be applied to the impact of lockdowns on mental health as well. Future research should attempt to establish a more precise relationship between lockdowns and positive and negative mental health indicators and to investigate the socio-contextual factors that are likely to influence such relationships. In conclusion, the effect size estimates of our meta-analysis of longitudinal studies and natural experiments represent the best available evidence on the effect of lockdown on mental health of the general population. Based on these estimates, the initial effect of lockdowns on mental health is relatively small, providing evidence of people's robust capacity for resilience.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000015.

Author contribution

G. Prati developed the manuscript concept, conducted the literature search, and performed the data analyses. A.D. Mancini contributed to study design, and both authors screened manuscripts for inclusion in the meta-analysis. G. Prati drafted sections of the manuscript, and A.D. Mancini provided critical revisions to all sections. Both authors approved the final article for submission.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors

Conflicts of interest

We have no conflicts of interest to disclose..The present research was not supported by specific grants from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Ethical standards

The author asserts that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Arksey, H., & O'Malley, L. (2005). Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 8(1), 1932. doi:10.1080/1364557032000119616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50(4), 10881101. doi:10.2307/2533446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bojanowska, A., Kaczmarek, L. D., Kościelniak, M., & Urbańska, B. (2020). Values and well-being change amidst the COVID-19 pandemic in Poland. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/xr87s.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience – have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, 59(1), 2028. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.59.1.20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonanno, G. A., & Mancini, A. D. (2012). Beyond resilience and PTSD: Mapping the heterogeneity of responses to potential trauma. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(1), 7483. doi:10.1037/a0017829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonanno, G. A., Westphal, M., & Mancini, A. D. (2011). Resilience to loss and potential trauma. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 511535. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borkowska, M., & Laurence, J. (2020). Coming together or coming apart? Changes in social cohesion during the Covid-19 pandemic in England. European Societies. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/14616696.2020.1833067.Google Scholar
Bryan, C. J., Bryan, A. O., & Baker, J. C. (2020). Associations among state-level physical distancing measures and suicidal thoughts and behaviors among U.S. adults during the early COVID-19 pandemic. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 50(6), 12231229. doi:10.1111/sltb.12653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calati, R., Ferrari, C., Brittner, M., Oasi, O., Olié, E., Carvalho, A. F., & Courtet, P. (2019). Suicidal thoughts and behaviors and social isolation: A narrative review of the literature. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 653667. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calo-Blanco, A., Kovářík, J., Mengel, F., & Romero, J. G. (2017). Natural disasters and indicators of social cohesion. PLoS ONE, 12(6), e0176885. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0176885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Courtet, P., Olié, E., Debien, C., & Vaiva, G. (2020). Keep socially (but not physically) connected and carry on: Preventing suicide in the age of COVID-19. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 81(3), e20com13370e13320com13370. doi:10.4088/JCP.20com13370.Google ScholarPubMed
Crayne, M. P. (2020). The traumatic impact of job loss and job search in the aftermath of COVID-19. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 12(S1), S180S182. doi:10.1037/tra0000852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, M., & Robinson, E. (2020). Problem drinking before and during the COVID-19 crisis in US and UK adults: Evidence from two population-based longitudinal studies. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.25.20139022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorillo, A., & Gorwood, P. (2020). The consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health and implications for clinical practice. European Psychiatry, 63(1), e32. doi:10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratz, K. L., Tull, M. T., Richmond, J. R., Edmonds, K. A., Scamaldo, K. M., & Rose, J. P. (2020). Thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness explain the associations of COVID-19 social and economic consequences to suicide risk. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 50(6), 11401148. doi:10.1111/sltb.12654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, J., Feng, X. L., Wang, X. H., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2020). Coping with COVID-19: Exposure to Covid-19 and negative impact on livelihood predict elevated mental health problems in Chinese adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(11). doi:10.3390/ijerph17113857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haider, N., Osman, A. Y., Gadzekpo, A., Akipede, G. O., Asogun, D., Ansumana, R., … McCoy, D. (2020). Lockdown measures in response to COVID-19 in nine sub-Saharan African countries. BMJ Global Health, 5(10), e003319. doi:10.1136/bmjgh-2020-003319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawdon, J., & Ryan, J. (2011). Social relations that generate and sustain solidarity after a mass tragedy. Social Forces, 89(4), 13631384. doi:10.1093/sf/89.4.1363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedges, L. V. (2019). Stochastically dependent effect sizes. In Hedges, L. V., Cooper, H., & Valentine, J. C. (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (3rd ed., pp. 281298). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2004). Controlling the risk of spurious findings from meta-regression. Statistics in Medicine, 23(11), 16631682. doi:10.1002/sim.1752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, S. E., Garnett, C., Shahab, L., Oldham, M., & Brown, J. (2020). Association of the Covid-19 lockdown with smoking, drinking, and attempts to quit in England: An analysis of 2019-2020 data. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.05.25.20112656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, D., & Turner, R. (2017). Power analysis for random-effects meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(3), 290302. doi:10.1002/jrsm.1240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
John, A., Pirkis, J., Gunnell, D., Appleby, L., & Morrissey, J. (2020). Trends in suicide during the Covid-19 pandemic. BMJ, 371, m4352. doi:10.1136/bmj.m4352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns Hopkins University Center for Systems Science and Engineering. (2020). Novel Coronavirus (COVID-19) Cases Data Retrieved from https://data.humdata.org/dataset/novel-coronavirus-2019-ncov-cases.Google Scholar
Joiner, T. E., Hollar, D., & Orden, K. V. (2006). On Buckeyes, Gators, Super Bowl Sunday, and the Miracle on Ice: “pulling together” is associated with lower suicide rates. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25(2), 179195. doi:10.1521/jscp.2006.25.2.179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keyes, C. L. M. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 539548. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.73.3.539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khangura, S., Konnyu, K., Cushman, R., Grimshaw, J., & Moher, D. (2012). Evidence summaries: The evolution of a rapid review approach. Systematic Reviews, 1(1), 10. doi:10.1186/2046-4053-1-10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killgore, W. D. S., Cloonan, S. A., Taylor, E. C., & Dailey, N. S. (2020). Loneliness: A signature mental health concern in the era of COVID-19. Psychiatry Research, 290, 113117. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwong, A. S. F., Pearson, R. M., Adams, M. J., Northstone, K., Tilling, K., Smith, D., … Timpson, N. J. (2020). Mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic in two longitudinal UK population cohorts. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.16.20133116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langan, D., Higgins, J. P. T., & Simmonds, M. (2015). An empirical comparison of heterogeneity variance estimators in 12894 meta-analyses. Research Synthesis Methods, 6(2), 195205. doi:10.1002/jrsm.1140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau, A. L. D., Chi, I., Cummins, R. A., Lee, T. M. C., Chou, K.-L., & Chung, L. W. M. (2008). The SARS (Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome) pandemic in Hong Kong: Effects on the subjective wellbeing of elderly and younger people. Aging and Mental Health, 12(6), 746760. doi:10.1080/13607860802380607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lau, J. T. F., Yang, X., Tsui, H. Y., Pang, E., & Wing, Y. K. (2006). Positive mental health-related impacts of the SARS epidemic on the general public in Hong Kong and their associations with other negative impacts. Journal of Infection, 53(2), 114124. doi:10.1016/j.jinf.2005.10.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lei, L., Huang, X., Zhang, S., Yang, J., Yang, L., & Xu, M. (2020). Comparison of prevalence and associated factors of anxiety and depression among people affected by versus people unaffected by quarantine during the COVID-9 epidemic in Southwestern China. Medical Science Monitor, 26, e924609e924609. doi:10.12659/MSM.924609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H. Y., Cao, H., Leung, D. Y. P., & Mak, Y. W. (2020). The psychological impacts of a covid-19 outbreak on college students in China: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(11), 3933. doi:10.3390/ijerph17113933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, N., Zhang, F., Wei, C., Jia, Y., Shang, Z., Sun, L., … Liu, W. (2020). Prevalence and predictors of PTSS during COVID-19 outbreak in China hardest-hit areas: Gender differences matter. Psychiatry Research, 287, 112921. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luchetti, M., Lee, J. H., Aschwanden, D., Sesker, A., Strickhouser, J. E., Terracciano, A., … Sutin, A. R. (2020). The trajectory of loneliness in response to COVID-19. American Psychologist, 75(7), 897908. doi:10.1037/amp0000690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, M., Guo, L., Yu, M., Jiang, W., & Wang, H. (2020). The psychological and mental impact of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) on medical staff and general public – A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 291, 113190. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancini, A. D. (2019). When acute adversity improves psychological health: A social–contextual framework. Psychological Review, 126(4), 486505. doi:10.1037/rev0000144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancini, A. D. (2020). Heterogeneous mental health consequences of COVID-19: Costs and benefits. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 12(S1), S15S16. doi:10.1037/tra0000894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancini, A. D., Littleton, H. L., & Grills, A. E. (2016). Can people benefit from acute stress? Social support, psychological improvement, and resilience after the Virginia Tech campus shootings. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(3), 401417. doi:10.1177/2167702615601001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meda, N., Pardini, S., Slongo, I., Bodini, L., Rigobello, P., Visioli, F., & Novara, C. (2020). COVID-19 and depressive symptoms in students before and during lockdown. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.27.20081695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meda, N., & Slongo, I. (2020). Caution when linking COVID-19 to mental health consequences. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 87, 152. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merton, R. K. (1948). The self-fulfilling prophecy. The Antioch Review, 8(2), 193210. doi:10.2307/4609267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, D., Stedman, M., & Heald, A. (2020). Living with COVID 19: Balancing costs against benefits in the face of the virus. National Institute Economic Review, 253, R60R76. doi:10.1017/nie.2020.30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedzwiedz, C. L., Green, M., Benzeval, M., Campbell, D., Craig, P., Demou, E., … Katikireddi, S. V. (2020). Mental health and health behaviours before and during the COVID-19 lockdown: Longitudinal analyses of the UK Household Longitudinal Study. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.21.20136820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Statistics, 8(2), 157159. doi:10.3102/10769986008002157.Google Scholar
Ozamiz-Etxebarria, N., Dosil-Santamaria, M., Picaza-Gorrochategui, M., & Idoiaga-Mondragon, N. (2020). Stress, anxiety, and depression levels in the initial stage of the COVID-19 outbreak in a population sample in the northern Spain. Cadernos de Saude Publica. Advance online publication. doi:10.1590/0102-311x00054020.Google Scholar
Pierce, M., Hope, H., Ford, T., Hatch, S., Hotopf, M., John, A., … Abel, K. M. (2020). Mental health before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal probability sample survey of the UK population. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(10), 883892. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30308-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prati, G. (2020). Mental health and its psychosocial predictors during national quarantine in Italy against the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). Anxiety, Stress, & Coping. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/10615806.2020.1861253.Google Scholar
Recchi, E., Ferragina, E., Helmeid, E., Pauly, S., Safi, M., Sauger, N., & Schradie, J. (2020). The “eye of the hurricane” paradox: An unexpected and unequal rise of well-being during the Covid-19 lockdown in France. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 68, 100508. doi:10.1016/j.rssm.2020.100508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reger, M. A., Stanley, I. H., & Joiner, T. E. (2020). Suicide mortality and coronavirus disease 2019 – A perfect storm? JAMA Psychiatry, 77(11), 10931094. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.1060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richter, F. (2020). The video apps we're downloading amid the coronavirus pandemic. Retrieved from https://www.weforum.org/agenda/2020/03/infographic-apps-pandemic-technology-data-coronavirus-covid19-tech/.Google Scholar
Rogers, J. P., Chesney, E., Oliver, D., Pollak, T. A., McGuire, P., Fusar-Poli, P., … David, A. S. (2020). Psychiatric and neuropsychiatric presentations associated with severe coronavirus infections: A systematic review and meta-analysis with comparison to the COVID-19 pandemic. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(7), 611627. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30203-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, P. L., Le, H., Oh, I.-S., Van Iddekinge, C. H., & Bobko, P. (2018). Using beta coefficients to impute missing correlations in meta-analysis research: Reasons for caution. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(6), 644658. doi:10.1037/apl0000293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salari, N., Hosseinian-Far, A., Jalali, R., Vaisi-Raygani, A., Rasoulpoor, S., Mohammadi, M., … Khaledi-Paveh, B. (2020). Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Globalization and Health, 16(1), 57. doi:10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schützwohl, M., & Mergel, E. (2020). Social participation, inclusion and mental well-being following SARS-CoV-2 related lockdown restrictions in adults with and without mental disorders. Results from a follow-up study in Germany. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6svxw.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanahan, L., Steinhoff, A., Bechtiger, L., Murray, A. L., Nivette, A., Hepp, U., … Eisner, M. (2020). Emotional distress in young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence of risk and resilience from a longitudinal cohort study. Psychological Medicine. Advance online publication. doi:10.1017/S003329172000241X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, C. G., Greaves, L. M., Satherley, N., Wilson, M. S., Overall, N. C., Lee, C. H. J., … Barlow, F. K. (2020). Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic and nationwide lockdown on trust, attitudes toward government, and well-being. American Psychologist, 75(5), 618630. doi:10.1037/amp0000662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevenson, C., Wakefield, J., Drury, J., & Felsner, I. (2020). Collectively coping with coronavirus: local community identification predicts giving support and lockdown adherence during the COVID-19 pandemic. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/2uyw7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taquet, M., Luciano, S., Geddes, J. R., & Harrison, P. J. (2020). Bidirectional associations between COVID-19 and psychiatric disorder: Retrospective cohort studies of 62 354 COVID-19 cases in the USA. The Lancet Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30462-4.Google ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 273277. doi:10.1111%2Fj.1467-8721.2006.00451.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Target article: “posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence”. Psychological Inquiry, 15(1), 118. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tricco, A. C., Antony, J., Zarin, W., Strifler, L., Ghassemi, M., Ivory, J., … Straus, S. E. (2015). A scoping review of rapid review methods. BMC Medicine, 13(1), 224. doi:10.1186/s12916-015-0465-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tull, M. T., Edmonds, K. A., Scamaldo, K. M., Richmond, J. R., Rose, J. P., & Gratz, K. L. (2020). Psychological outcomes associated with stay-at-home orders and the perceived impact of COVID-19 on daily life. Psychiatry Research. Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uchida, Y., Takahashi, Y., & Kawahara, K. (2014). Changes in hedonic and eudaimonic well-being after a severe nationwide disaster: The case of the great east Japan earthquake. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15(1), 207221. doi:10.1007/s10902-013-9463-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E. Jr. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review, 117(2), 575600. doi:10.1037/a0018697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 1(3), 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03.Google Scholar
Vindegaard, N., & Benros, M. E. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic and mental health consequences: Systematic review of the current evidence. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 89, 531542. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., McIntyre, R. S., … Ho, C. (2020). A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 87, 4048. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, C., & Zhao, H. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 on anxiety in Chinese university students. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(1168). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01168.Google ScholarPubMed
Xin, M., Luo, S., She, R., Yu, Y., Li, L., Wang, S., … Lau, J. T.-F. (2020). Negative cognitive and psychological correlates of mandatory quarantine during the initial COVID-19 outbreak in China. American Psychologist, 75(5), 607617. doi:10.1037/amp0000692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
YouGov. (2020). COVID-19: Level of support for actions governments could take. Retrieved from https://yougov.co.uk/topics/international/articles-reports/2020/03/17/level-support-actions-governments-could-take.Google Scholar
Zacher, H., & Rudolph, C. W. (2020). Individual differences and changes in subjective wellbeing during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/amp0000702.Google ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, M., Bledsoe, C., & Papa, A. (2020). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on college student mental health: A longitudinal examination of risk and protective factors. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2y7hu.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram of the study selection process.

Figure 1

Table 1. Univariate analysis of effect sizes, heterogeneity, and fail-safe N

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Forest plot of the 20 effect size estimates for the association between lockdown and mental health symptoms.Note. The year of publication was 2020 for all studies.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Forest plot of the six effect size estimates for the association between lockdown and positive functioning.Note. The year of publication was 2020 for all studies. Higher scores on positive functioning correspond to higher well-being.

Figure 4

Table 2. Multivariate analysis of effect sizes based on random-effects model

Figure 5

Table 3. Results of univariate and multivariate meta-regression models

Supplementary material: File

Prati and Mancini supplementary material

Prati and Mancini supplementary material 1

Download Prati and Mancini supplementary material(File)
File 23.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Prati and Mancini supplementary material

Prati and Mancini supplementary material 2

Download Prati and Mancini supplementary material(File)
File 12.6 KB