Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:42:49.720Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cats just want to have fun: Associations between play and welfare in domestic cats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 January 2023

Julia Henning*
Affiliation:
School of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, The University of Adelaide, Mudla Wirra Rd, Roseworthy SA 5371, Australia
Torben Nielsen
Affiliation:
School of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, The University of Adelaide, Mudla Wirra Rd, Roseworthy SA 5371, Australia
Eduardo Fernandez
Affiliation:
School of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, The University of Adelaide, Mudla Wirra Rd, Roseworthy SA 5371, Australia
Susan Hazel
Affiliation:
School of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, The University of Adelaide, Mudla Wirra Rd, Roseworthy SA 5371, Australia
*
Author for correspondence: Julia Henning, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Play is often considered an indicator and promotor of animal welfare and may facilitate closer cat-human relationships. However, few studies have empirically investigated these associations. The current study aimed to investigate play-related factors associated with four welfare outcome measures in cats (Felis catus) including: cat quality of life; cat-guardian relationship quality; problem behaviour prevalence; and behavioural changes. An online survey was developed using demographic information, questions related to play and resources, free text sections and the following validated measures: cat quality of life (QOL), the cat owner relationship scale, and the adult playfulness trait scale. Responses were completed by 1,591 cat guardians from 55 countries. Higher cat playfulness scores and a greater number of games played were significantly associated with higher cat QOL scores while longer amounts of daily play, greater number of games, both cat and guardian initiating play and higher guardian playfulness scores were all significantly associated with higher cat-guardian relationship scores. Exclusively indoor housing was significantly associated with both higher cat QOL and higher cat-guardian relationships scores compared to cats with outdoor access. Behavioural changes associated with distress in cats were reported when play was absent. Play may be an important factor in assessing and maintaining cat welfare. Further research into the mechanisms of how play impacts welfare and cat-guardian relationships is needed.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

The potential of play as a behavioural tool to both identify and increase animal welfare has been a recent topic of interest for applied animal behaviour researchers (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011; Ahloy-Dallaire et al. Reference Ahloy-Dallaire, Espinosa and Mason2018). This link between play and welfare is most often observed when animals are experiencing good health, have adequate resources and are free from fitness threats such as predation (Fagen Reference Fagen1978; Oliveira et al. Reference Oliveira, Rossi, Silva, Lau and Barreto2010). In addition, research links play to juvenile development and long-term maintenance of neurological/physiological, cognitive-behavioural, and emotional skills (Burghardt Reference Burghardt2005; Graham & Burghardt Reference Graham and Burghardt2010; Vanderschuren & Trezza Reference Vanderschuren and Trezza2013; Pellis et al. Reference Pellis, Burghardt, Palagi and Mangel2015).

Play may be especially useful in managing the welfare of animals in human care such as cats (Felis catus), an increasingly popular pet choice within human homes (PDSA Animal Wellbeing Report 2022). Previous studies into play in cats have found associations between welfare issues such as social isolation, inconsistent husbandry, space availability, problem behaviours and changes in play behaviour (Seitz Reference Seitz1959; West Reference West1974; Guyot et al. Reference Guyot, Bennett and Cross1980; Carlstead et al. Reference Carlstead, Brown and Strawn1993; Strickler & Shull Reference Strickler and Shull2014; Loberg & Lundmark Reference Loberg and Lundmark2016). Of the previous studies that assessed play and welfare associations in cats, only a few used a specific welfare metric within their study. For instance, measures of cat physical condition (Arhant et al. Reference Arhant, Wogritsch and Troxler2015), occurrence of abnormal, repetitive behaviours (Kogan & Grigg Reference Kogan and Grigg2021) or physiological and behavioural signs of stress (Carlstead et al. Reference Carlstead, Brown and Strawn1993). In a recent review of the current literature, we highlighted the lack of specific welfare measures used in studies of play in cats (Henning et al. Reference Henning, Fernandez, Nielsen and Hazel2022a).

Cat welfare encompasses many elements, from the cats’ physical state and the quality of their environment as well as the resources available to them, to their mental state and social relationships (Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, Rodan, Carney, Heath, Rochlitz, Shearburn, Sundahl and Westropp2013; Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019; Henning et al. Reference Henning, Fernandez, Nielsen and Hazel2022a). For cats in human homes, all these elements rely heavily on their guardian and their relationship with that guardian. It is therefore important to consider guardian perceptions about their cat, their cat’s behaviour and their relationship with their cat when assessing cat welfare within a human home. Human-animal interactions, such as play, are likely to impact the dynamic and quality of relationships between cat and guardian. Considering play may be integral to forming and maintaining social skills and communication intra-specifically in animals (Guyot et al. Reference Guyot, Bennett and Cross1980, Bekoff & Allen Reference Bekoff, Allen, Bekoff and Allen2011; Vanderschuren & Trezza Reference Vanderschuren and Trezza2013; Palagi et al. Reference Palagi, Burghardt, Smuts, Cordoni, Dall’Olio, Fouts, Řeháková-Petrů, Siviy and Pellis2016), play may also be capable of assisting in establishing and maintaining healthy cat-guardian relationships (Henning et al. Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b).

Guardians are responsible for making homing and medical choices for their cats which may have serious welfare outcomes, such as surrender or euthanasia. How guardians perceive their cats may impact how they treat them and even what decisions they make concerning their lives. What guardians perceive as ‘problem behaviours’, such as scratching furniture or inappropriate urination are often the result of species-typical behaviours that are natural and functional within their usual environment but become problematic when viewed through the lens of a guardian who wishes to have a neat and undamaged home. Regardless of whether the behaviours are problematic for the cat or not, problem behaviours are the leading cat-related factor for surrender to shelters (Patronek et al. Reference Patronek, Glickman, Beck, McCabe and Ecker1996; Jensen et al. Reference Jensen, Sandøe and Nielsen2020) and the foremost cause of euthanasia in otherwise healthy pet cats (Carney et al. Reference Carney, Sadek, Curtis, Halls, Heath, Hutchison, Mundschenk and Westropp2014). Studies suggest that play may have a role in mitigating the occurrence of problem behaviours in cats, with a lack of play associated with greater occurrence of problem behaviours (Strickler & Shull Reference Strickler and Shull2014; Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019).

While previous studies show promise for some association between play and welfare in cats, there is still much to be understood about whether play is an adequate indicator of welfare, whether play can be used to promote welfare and how much or what kind of play is best suited to achieving these aims. A previous study by Henning et al. (Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b) investigated the factors associated with how much play occurs within human-cat dyads. Here, the present study follows on from this paper and aims to identify and assess whether play itself, its quantity or quality or its role in cat-guardian relationships, is associated with welfare outcomes in cats. To achieve this, four different potential welfare measures were used within a global online survey. These were: cat quality of life (QOL) scores; quality of the cat-guardian relationship scores; reported behavioural problems; and guardian observations of behavioural changes in times when play was absent. It was expected that higher play times would be positively associated with welfare measures.

Materials and methods

The survey development, validated measures, data management and study population have been described in more detail in Henning et al. (Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b). A short summary is included here as well as details relevant to the areas of the questionnaire analysed within this paper. The protocol for this study was conducted with approval from the Human Research Ethics Committee at the University of Adelaide, approval code: H-2021-091.

Survey

The survey was developed in consultation with veterinarians, animal behaviourists, and cat guardians. Participants were required to be over 18 years of age and the primary caregiver of a cat over one year of age. If the participant had multiple cats, they were asked to think of the cat they spent the most time with and answer questions as if they had been asked about that cat specifically, so that the survey was only answered for one cat per respondent. The resulting open survey comprised 105 questions and was hosted on Qualtrics XM®. The survey included demographic information, questions regarding duration and time of play, play type, guardian experiences of play, cat medical history, guardian perceptions of changes in behaviour, and three validated measures which included: the Feline Quality of Life Measure (QOL) (Tatlock et al. Reference Tatlock, Gober, Williamson and Arbuckle2017); the Cat Owner Relationship Scale (CORS) (Howell et al. Reference Howell, Bowen, Fatjó, Calvo, Holloway and Bennett2017); and the Adult Playfulness Trait Scale (APTS) (Shen et al. Reference Shen, Chick and Zinn2014). All included measures had previously been validated (Shen et al. Reference Shen, Chick and Zinn2014; Howell et al. Reference Howell, Bowen, Fatjó, Calvo, Holloway and Bennett2017; Tatlock et al. Reference Tatlock, Gober, Williamson and Arbuckle2017). Participants were asked to rate whether their cat exhibited certain problem behaviours on a 5-point Likert scale from ‘never’ to ‘most of the time.’ Only participants who completed every question of the cat QOL and CORS validated measures, and completed at least 98% of the survey overall, were included in final analysis (Henning et al. Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b). The survey was open for participation between the 22nd of June and the 17th of July 2021.

Analysis

A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was undertaken to assess normality. The data set were found not to be normally distributed. However, as the data set were large, and thus robust enough, to be tested using standard parametric inferential statistics, these were used (Ghasemi & Zahediasl Reference Ghasemi and Zahediasl2012; Hector Reference Hector2021). Eighteen reported problem behaviours were reduced to seven components using a Principal Component Analysis (PCA). After viewing the matrix, one component and one item were removed due to low loading, leaving six components and seventeen items. Components were then included in analyses, including ANOVAs, regression, and general linear models. Two general linear models were created with cat QOL and CORS scores as dependent variables. All variables were tested through two-way ANOVA against QOL and CORS scores, respectively, and all variables from regressions or ANOVAs with a P < 0.2 were included in subsequent modelling. Non-significant factors were removed using backwards elimination. Interactions that were relevant were checked for significance and included in the model where found. Statistical analyses were completed using SPSS® Statistics 27. A P-value of < 0.05 was considered significant. Descriptive and qualitative analyses of behaviour changes during times play was withheld were undertaken. Free text responses were collected and grouped into types of behaviour. Where quotes have been shortened a ‘[…]’ is used. Quotes were only shortened where necessary while maintaining and respecting the original meaning.

Results

A total of 1,591 completed responses were recorded from 55 different countries. Most participants were currently living in Australia (49.3%), identified as female (90.3%) and reported living in a two-person household at the time of the survey (47.8%). Most cats in the study were of mixed breed (76%) and almost equally reported to be male or female. Most cats were reported to be housed exclusively indoors (67%), between 3–5 years of age (28.8%), within a single cat home (38.9%), and had been living with their current guardian for 2–5 years at the time of the survey (42%). For further information on demographics of this study see Henning et al. (Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b).

Factors associated with problem behaviours

A list of problem behaviours and a free text option were presented to respondents along with a five-point Likert scale with options from ‘never’ to ‘most of the time.’ Answers above a three on the Likert scale were included in the following tallies and percentages. The most reported problem behaviour by participants was scratching furniture (47.5%), followed by aggression during play (40.2%), excessive vocalisation (37.6%), aggression towards unfamiliar cats (36.6%) and being overly active at night (35.8%) (Table 1).

Table 1. Problem behaviours reported by cat guardians (n = 1,591). Guardians were able to report multiple behaviours. Based on online survey responses of guardians between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Eighteen items of reported problem behaviours were reduced using a PCA. Six components presented with an eigenvalue exceeding 1, explaining 51.6% of variance. Sucking on material did not present with a high enough value in any component and was removed from the analysis, leaving seventeen items. These summary indices included the following components: inappropriate excretion subset, aggression towards unknown animals’ subset, annoyance behaviour subset, aggression towards people subset, stress behaviour subset, and aggression towards known animals’ subset (Table 2).

Table 2. Loading for 18 problem behaviours reported by cat guardians, generated by means of a Principal Component Analysis

* Not included due to low loading

Analysis of the PCA components showed no significant results between play and problem behaviour components. Analysis of PCA components and cat QOL scores showed only the component for stress behaviours (e.g., overgrooming, pica, anxiety) as significantly negatively associated with cat QOL scores (see Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3. Results of a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each factor associated with quality of life in cats (n = 1,590) except for: cat age (n = 1,584) and cat playfulness (n = 1,589). Based on online survey responses of cat guardians between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

a Total ‘games’ played related to games the guardian regularly played with their cat and included: Fetch, playing with catnip toys, playing with noisy toys, playing with boxes, playing with hands, playing with digital devices, playing with wand toys, playing with laser pointers, playing with food, playing with motorised toys, chasing each other and training.

b Housing: Outdoor was defined as regular unsupervised access to the outdoors without a harness, lead and not within a fully enclosed cat enclosure. Indoor/Outdoor was defined as mostly indoor with some access to the outdoors and exclusively indoors was defined as a cat with no access to the outdoors except on a harness or within a fully enclosed cat enclosure.

Table 4. General Linear Model parameter estimates of factors associated with cat quality of life scores (n = 1,583), based on online survey cat guardian responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

** Reference category.

a Total ‘games’ played related to games the guardian regularly played with their cat and included: Fetch, playing with catnip toys, playing with noisy toys, playing with boxes, playing with hands, playing with digital devices, playing with wand toys, playing with laser pointers, playing with food, playing with motorised toys, chasing each other and training.

b Housing: Outdoor access was defined as any regular unsupervised access to the outdoors without a harness, lead and not within a fully enclosed cat enclosure. Exclusively indoors was defined as a cat with no access to the outdoors except on a harness or within a fully enclosed cat enclosure.

c Stress behaviour score relates to a guardian-reported frequency for anxiety, pica and overgrooming respectively, reported on a five-point Likert scale and coded into values (1 being never and 5 being most of the time) with values summed to create a composite score.

Factors associated with cat quality of life

One-way ANOVAs showed that cat QOL scores were higher in cats who were younger, had no health issues, were housed exclusively indoors, were more playful, had access to a greater number of games, had lower stress behaviour PCA component scores, and higher CORS scores (Table 3).

A univariate linear regression showed a significant association between cat QOL and CORS scores (P < 0.001). The correlation coefficient for CORS and cat QOL scores was R = 0.375, indicating a moderate correlation (standard error = 0.015). A linear regression also showed a significant association between cat QOL scores and stress behaviour PCA component scores (P < 0.001). The correlation coefficient for stress behaviours and cat QOL scores was R = 0.154 indicating a weak correlation (standard error = 0.08).

A general linear model analysis of factors associated with cat QOL scores showed higher cat QOL scores where cats were housed exclusively indoors, were more playful, had access to a greater number of games, and where the guardian reported higher CORS scores (Table 4).

An interaction was found between reported health issues and cat age (P = 0.005). QOL scores reduced at a greater rate where the cat was older and had health issues.

Factors associated with cat-guardian relationships

One-way ANOVAs showed that CORS scores were higher in human-cat dyads where the cat was housed exclusively indoors, the number of games played within the dyad was greater, the cat had access to a higher number of resources, guardians did not report avoiding play, and where both cat and guardian initiated play (Table 5).

Table 5. Results of a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each factor associated with the cat owner relationship scale (CORS) (n = 1,590) except for: guardian gender (n = 1,582), do you avoid play (n = 1,580), who initiates play (n = 1,451) and relationship status (n = 1,567). Based on online survey cat guardian responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

a Total ‘games’ played related to games the guardian regularly played with their cat and included: Fetch, playing with catnip toys, playing with noisy toys, playing with boxes, playing with hands, playing with digital devices, playing with wand toys, playing with laser pointers, playing with food, playing with motorised toys, chasing each other and training.

b Housing: Outdoor access was defined as any regular unsupervised access to the outdoors without a harness, lead and not within a fully enclosed cat enclosure. Exclusively indoors was defined as a cat with no access to the outdoors except on a harness or within a fully enclosed cat enclosure.

c Total resources available included: scratching post, cat bed, hiding place, water fountain, wand toy, food puzzle, food treats, motorised toy, wall or window perch, harness walks, outdoor cat enclosure, cat grass, self-cleaning litter tray, FeliwayTM and automatic feeder.

A univariate linear regression showed significant associations between CORS scores and guardian playfulness (APTS) scores (P < 0.001), total guardian-cat daily play time (P < 0.001), cat QOL scores (P < 0.001), and the length of guardianship. The correlation coefficient for CORS and APTS was R = 0.120, indicating a weak correlation (standard error = 0.017). The correlation coefficient for CORS and total guardian-cat daily play time was R = 0.271, indicating a moderate correlation (standard error = 0.004). The correlation coefficient for CORS and cat QOL scores was R = 0.375 indicating a moderate correlation (standard error = 0.037). The correlation coefficient for CORS and length of guardianship was R = 0.058, indicating a weak correlation (standard error = 0.06).

A general linear model analysis of factors associated with CORS scores showed higher scores where cats were housed exclusively indoors, the number of games played within the human-cat dyad was greater, the cat had access to a larger number of resources, guardian age was younger, guardians identified as non-binary, guardians did not report avoiding play, both cat and guardian initiated play, the guardian had higher playfulness scores, were single, and where the cat was left home for less than 40 h (Table 6).

Table 6. General Linear Model parameter estimates of factors associated with cat-owner relationship scores (n = 1,372), based on online survey responses of cat guardians between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

** Reference category.

a Total ‘games’ played related to games the guardian regularly played with their cat and included: Fetch, playing with catnip toys, playing with noisy toys, playing with boxes, playing with hands, playing with digital devices, playing with wand toys, playing with laser pointers, playing with food, playing with motorised toys, chasing each other and training.

b Housing: Outdoor access was defined as any regular unsupervised access to the outdoors without a harness, lead and not within a fully enclosed cat enclosure. Exclusively indoors was defined as a cat with no access to the outdoors except on a harness or within a fully enclosed cat enclosure.

c Total resources available included: scratching post, cat bed, hiding place, water fountain, wand toy, food puzzle, food treats, motorised toy, wall or window perch, harness walks, outdoor cat enclosure, cat grass, self-cleaning litter tray, FeliwayTM and automatic feeder.

Guardian-perceived behaviour changes when play is withheld

Guardians were presented with the following question: “Does your cat’s behaviour change if you haven’t played with them for a while?” The most common reported behaviour changes when play was withheld (n = 468) were that their cat exhibited more attention-seeking behaviour, followed by increased vocalising, an increase in destructive behaviour, an increase in reclusive behaviour, and increased aggressive behaviour (Table 7). Examples of quotes relating to these reported behaviours are given in Table 8.

Table 7. Guardian-reported cat behaviour changes when play was withheld, reported by guardians (n = 468). Guardians could report more than one behaviour change. Based on online survey responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Table 8. Examples of guardian-reported cat behaviour changes when play was withheld (n = 468). Based on online survey responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Discussion

The objective of the present study was to investigate associations between play and welfare in cats by assessing factors associated with cat QOL score and closeness of cat-guardian relationships. Results indicated that cat playfulness and the number of games played by the cat and guardian were associated with QOL scores while the amount of daily play, the number of games played, who initiated play, whether the guardian avoided play, and guardian playfulness were associated with cat-guardian relationship scores. Cat behaviour, as perceived by their guardian, was reported to change during times when play was withheld. No significant associations were found between play and problem behaviours.

Play factors associated with cat quality of life: Cat playfulness and number of games

Cat playfulness was significantly correlated with higher cat QOL scores. Previous observations suggest that play is more likely to be exhibited during times when animals are physically healthy, have access to ample resources, and are not under stress (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011; Ahloy-Dallaire et al. Reference Ahloy-Dallaire, Espinosa and Mason2018). Cat playfulness may therefore be reflective of how a cat is feeling physically or emotionally, with sick, injured or stressed cats being less likely to exhibit playfulness (Fagen Reference Fagen1978). In a previous study into the effects of stress on cats, animals assigned to a stress condition of irregular or poor caretaking showed reduced or absent play behaviours (Carlstead et al. Reference Carlstead, Brown and Strawn1993). Interestingly, contrary to our expectations, daily play times were not significantly associated with cat QOL scores. However, the higher the number of games a cat was reported to regularly engage in was significantly correlated with higher cat QOL scores. This discrepancy may indicate that it is the quality or variety of play available and not simply the quantity of play that may be associated with positive welfare outcomes for cats (Carlstead et al. Reference Carlstead, Seidensticker and Baldwin1991; Ahloy-Dallaire et al. Reference Ahloy-Dallaire, Espinosa and Mason2018; Fernandez Reference Fernandez2022).

The following distinction was made in this study between toys and games: toys were defined as stimuli that the cat can interact with while games were defined as actions that a cat (and/or guardian) can take. A game may include a toy. Cats have previously been observed to become habituated to toys quickly, showing decreases in play intensity with increased exposure to a particular toy followed by regaining interest and intensity of play when presented with a new toy (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Bradshaw and Robinson2002). Cats may also become habituated to the types of games available to them. Where cats only had a few games available to them with their guardian, their guardian also reported lower QOL scores. If cats are given access to a greater number or regular variation of games and toys, this may help to minimise habituation and boredom with play, potentially resulting in higher playfulness, QOL scores and overall welfare.

Play factors associated with cat-guardian relationships: Number of games and daily play

As well as being associated with cat QOL scores, a higher number of games regularly engaged with was also associated with higher cat-guardian relationship scores. It is possible that guardians who report having a low CORS score were less likely to interact with their cat, or that regular engagement in a variety of play is beneficial for improving cat-guardian relationships. Similarly for humans, only engaging in a small number of games with their cats may lead guardians to experience boredom and dissatisfaction with play, potentially resulting in guardians playing less with their cats. It has previously been found that routine behaviours, which a person feels obligated to perform, undermine their sense of self and freedom (Iso-Ahola Reference Iso-Ahola2022). However, these negative aspects can be mitigated by increasing the variety in, and of, these behaviours, such as undertaking a variety of different activities that achieve the same purpose (Iso-Ahola Reference Iso-Ahola2022). Tasks that require too little cognitive engagement (such as repeating the same wand toy game with a cat) also can become boring and aversive, leading people to either disengage with the task or seek to enhance variety and novelty (Shenhav et al. Reference Shenhav, Rand and Greene2016). Therefore, engaging with a variety of games is likely to increase both cat and guardian feelings of satisfaction and engagement with their play sessions, enhancing the human-cat bond and making future play sessions more likely. Henning et al. (Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b) previously reported that the number of games engaged with by human and cat was also associated with longer play session lengths and total daily play times. Total daily play times were significantly associated with cat-guardian relationship scores. This may be because more play offers more opportunities for human-cat bonding, or it may also be that guardians who are more invested in their cat are more willing to play with them.

Play factors associated with cat-guardian relationships: Who initiates and guardian playfulness

Cat-guardian relationship scores were highest where both cat and guardian were reported to initiate play sessions. Being able to both initiate play and recognise initiation of play requires both cat and guardian to be observant and capable of comprehending each other’s communication signals. Communication is key to the health of many relationships; therefore, it is possible that better understanding of human and cat signals, be they vocalisations or body language, is likely to lead to a closer or more enriching cat-guardian relationship. Guardians who are able to understand their cat’s signals are also more likely to identify their cat’s needs (Heath Reference Heath and Heath2007). Self-determination, including the freedom to choose to initiate and engage in an activity, is also a critical component to enjoyment in activities (Dattilo et al. Reference Dattilo, Kleiber and Williams1998). Therefore, when a cat or guardian is choosing to participate in an activity, instead of being forced to do so, they may be more likely to derive benefit from the activity. Cats have been observed to respond well to being given choice and control within human-cat interactions (Mertens & Turner Reference Mertens and Turner1988; Haywood et al. Reference Haywood, Ripari, Puzzo, Foreman-Worsley and Finka2021). Allowing cats to have choice in their interactions is an important behaviour to encourage within cat guardians. Further, cats have been shown to engage more enthusiastically with humans who are more responsive when the cat is seeking attention (Turner Reference Turner1991). If a cat is regularly initiating play sessions, it indicates that they are applying, and able to utilise, their own self-determination.

Guardian playfulness was also found to be significantly associated with higher cat-guardian relationship scores. Guardians who are more playful may be more willing to engage in play with their cat and may enjoy playing with their cat more than guardians who are not playful. Henning et al. (Reference Henning, Nielsen, Fernandez and Hazel2022b), which utilised data from the same respondents as the current study, previously found that APTS scores were significantly associated with longer play session times between cats and guardians. A more engaged guardian who participates in longer play sessions may be better positioned to build social connections with their cat through increased interaction. In a previous study by Odendaal and Meintjes (Reference Odendaal and Meintjes2003) into dog-guardian relationships, interactions of between 5 and 24 min were associated (for both dog and human) with: decreases in blood pressure; significant increases in oxytocin, prolactin and phenylethylamine (neurochemicals that are known to be associated with bonding), plasma dopamine concentrations associated with pleasurable experiences and β-endorphin which is involved in learning, memory, analgesia, and euphoric states. Humans in the study also experienced a decrease in plasma cortisol, indicating stress relief (Odendaal & Meintjes Reference Odendaal and Meintjes2003). Increased interactions due to guardian playfulness may therefore benefit both cat and guardian.

However, it is important to note that this measure of cat-guardian relationship is solely guardian-reported and is not a direct measure of how the cat experiences the relationship. A recent study by Finka et al. (Reference Finka, Ripari, Quinlan, Haywood, Puzzo, Jordan, Tsui, Foreman-Worsley, Dixon and Brennan2022) found that cats preferred humans who interacted only when the cat showed interest in interacting and that people who self-reported as having a long history of experience with cats were more likely to force interaction, hold cats against their will and touch places that cats do not typically like to be touched (Finka et al. Reference Finka, Ripari, Quinlan, Haywood, Puzzo, Jordan, Tsui, Foreman-Worsley, Dixon and Brennan2022). Direct observational measures of cat experiences of cat-guardian relationships are needed.

Play factors associated with cat quality of life and cat-guardian relationships: Indoor or outdoor housing

Exclusively indoor housing was significantly associated with both higher guardian-reported cat QOL scores and higher cat-guardian relationship scores. To our knowledge, this is the first study to compare QOL scores of indoor- and outdoor-housed cats. Some previous studies have focused on cat adaptation to confinement in cattery, shelter, or laboratory settings. However, these are limited in their ability to assess the needs and welfare of cats within a long-term, indoor-only home (Ottway & Hawkins Reference Ottway and Hawkins2003; Kry & Casey Reference Kry and Casey2007; Stella et al. Reference Stella, Croney and Buffington2014; Rehnberg et al. Reference Rehnberg, Robert, Watson and Peters2015; Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019). It has been suggested that outdoor cats may benefit from higher quality of life generally than indoors cats due to their ability to find their own amusement, express their natural behaviours, and choose whether to be inside or out (Rochlitz Reference Rochlitz2005). For some, this potential for a higher quality of life outweighs the increased risks of outdoor access, such as infectious disease, road accidents, trauma, and risk to native wildlife (Yeates & Yates Reference Yeates and Yates2017). While for others, the inherent dangers of the outdoors and their potential to limit the length of their cat’s life outweigh any small difference in potential quality of life (Rochlitz Reference Rochlitz2005). However, contrary to the suggestion that outdoor cats may have a higher quality of life, the results of the current study showed that cats housed exclusively indoors recorded higher QOL scores than cats with outdoor access. While this is not a definitive result regarding the difference in QOL scores between indoor and outdoor cats, there are several reasons this result may have been observed in our study. Firstly, cat-guardian relationships are central to domestic cat welfare. Cats rely heavily on their guardian to care for their nutrition, shelter and both their physical and mental health. A good relationship with their guardian, therefore, is likely to increase their QOL score. Our results found a significant association with cat-guardian relationship scores and whether the cat was housed exclusively indoors or allowed outdoor access, with indoor cat guardians reporting higher cat-guardian relationship scores. Secondly, guardians who house their cats indoors are better able to observe their cat’s day-to-day health. Any changes in behaviour, mobility, or important health markers, such as urination and defaecation regularity and consistency, are more likely to be noticed by a guardian who is observing their cat more often. Conversely, cats with outdoor access may be harder to observe and may eliminate in outdoor areas where their excrement cannot be examined for changes. If indoor-cat guardians are more able to observe changes in their cat’s health, they are better placed to make any environmental changes needed, or seek veterinary attention, resulting in increased cat QOL scores. Finally, guardians who keep their cats indoors may have more opportunities to participate in enriching interactions such as play. A previous study by Pyari et al. (Reference Pyari, Uccheddu, Lenkei and Pongrácz2021) reported that indoor cats showed stronger interest in play stimuli than outdoor cats. It is possible then, that indoor cat play preferences and needs may differ from outdoor cats. Pyari et al. (Reference Pyari, Uccheddu, Lenkei and Pongrácz2021) discussed that this difference may be due to an absence of the ability to express hunting behaviour in indoor cats, leading to what they describe as an increase in predatory play behaviour to compensate. However, predatory play as a term and as a behaviour is not yet fully understood and may in fact be a misnomer. A previous study by Pellis et al. (Reference Pellis, O’Brien, Pellis, Teitelbaum, Wolgin and Kennedy1988) analysed ‘predatory play’ movements or playful movements during predation and found that these movement patterns were adaptive movements that functioned to protect the cat from injury during predation and in essence were not playful at all (Pellis et al. Reference Pellis, O’Brien, Pellis, Teitelbaum, Wolgin and Kennedy1988). The trope of cats being ‘psychopathic’ playful hunters (Evans et al. Reference Evans, Lyons, Brewer and Bethell2021) is a potentially dangerous one for cat welfare, considering the current attitudes to cats and their frequent use as a scapegoat for the more major pressures imposed by people on biodiversity and climate (Palmer Reference Palmer2014; Wald & Peterson Reference Wald and Peterson2020). It is important that we acknowledge the play needs of cats and that these needs may differ depending on that cat’s background and housing, while not anthropomorphising and casting moral judgments that do not apply to non-human animals and may not be an accurate assessment of their behaviour anyway, as observed here by Pellis et al. (Reference Pellis, O’Brien, Pellis, Teitelbaum, Wolgin and Kennedy1988).

While there are strong arguments for both camps of thought as to whether cats should be housed indoors or out, it is generally agreed that indoor cats require environmental enrichments to support their welfare in an indoor only home (Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019). It is also possible that guardians who are more closely bonded with their cat and therefore may be more worried for their cat’s safety may choose to house their cats indoors (Crowley et al. Reference Crowley, Cecchetti and McDonald2019, Reference Crowley, Cecchetti and McDonald2020; Foreman-Worsley et al. Reference Foreman-Worsley, Finka, Ward and Farnworth2021) (as indicated here by the higher cat-guardian relationship scores), and that they may also be more willing to fulfill the play and enrichment needs of their cat. This, in turn, may have resulted in the higher cat QOL scores observed within this study. Further, it may not be that indoor or outdoor housing is strictly better for cats, but instead, how their guardians perceive, monitor, and treat them that has more of an impact.

Behaviour changes during lowered play times

Guardians reported noticing certain behaviour changes when they had not played with their cat for some time. While many guardians reported their cat exhibiting an increase in attention-seeking behaviours, several reported that their cat became more reclusive when play had been withheld. This showcases an interesting dichotomy of reactions to lack of play, with both an increase and decrease in cat engagement with guardian observed depending on the individual cat. This may be due to individual differences between cats and/or their guardians, familial dynamics within the household, the specific environment, or the housing situation of the cat (indoor or outdoor). Unfortunately, no clear pattens of differences were able to be deduced between behaviour changes in indoor versus outdoor cats within this study, however it is likely that this may impact behaviours and future studies could investigate this further. For many of the welfare measures we have investigated previously, the emphasis has been on whether play is indicative of welfare (Henning et al. Reference Henning, Fernandez, Nielsen and Hazel2022a). Observations of behaviour change when play is withheld may also offer us an insight into whether play is a promotor of welfare. Often it is difficult to separate the two concepts from each other, not knowing if more play means more welfare or if greater welfare means more play, or both. Within this study, guardians reported that when play is withheld, observable behaviour changes occur that may indicate a decrease in welfare or well-being. Increased attention-seeking behaviour, especially vocalisations and destructive behaviour, may indicate that the cat is experiencing frustration, a negative affective state that may impact the cat’s well-being. Increased reclusive behaviour may also be detrimental to cat welfare. Cats in human homes must share their space with the humans of the household, who utilise most of the space in a house. A cat who becomes reclusive is limited in what areas of the house they feel comfortable accessing and this may limit their physical activity as well as their access to important resources such as food, water, and litter (Carlstead et al. Reference Carlstead, Brown and Strawn1993). Reclusive behaviour may also indicate that there has been a deterioration in the social relationship between cat and guardian. Within cat social groups, signs of affiliation include spending time around and interacting positively with another cat while avoidance of spaces where another cat is and aversion to interacting may indicate that the cats are not affiliated (Vitale Reference Vitale2022). Put in terms of cat and guardian, if a cat is avoiding spaces where their guardian is and is not interacting as usual, it may indicate that the cat is uncomfortable. Humans within relationships are often acutely attuned to the behaviour of those close to them, and how this behaviour may affect or be indicative of the health of their relationship. It is possible that cats are similarly aware of their guardian’s behaviour towards them and what this may indicate about their safety within the relationship. Recent research shows that cats regularly keep a mental tab on where their guardian is within the house (Takagi et al. Reference Takagi, Chijiiwa, Arahori, Saito, Fujita and Kuroshima2021). It is entirely possible that cats are also aware of what constitutes their guardian’s regular behaviour and therefore notice any changes to this behaviour. Previous studies have shown that cats are highly sensitive to changes in routine (Stella et al. Reference Stella, Lord and Buffington2011). It is possible that changes the cat perceives in their regular interactions with their guardians may similarly make them uncomfortable. Many of the behaviour changes listed, such as destructive or aggressive behaviour, may also constitute a problem or annoyance for the guardian which may impact their perception of their relationship with their cat. As cat welfare within human homes relies so heavily on human perceptions, this may also constitute an impact to cat welfare.

Limitations and future research

This study has several potential limitations. Since it is not a direct measure, guardian-reported surveys are inherently limited in their ability to accurately capture their animals’ behavioural data. Further, due to the self-reporting nature of surveys, an individual’s responses may be prone to respondent and recall bias and limited in their ability to predict or assess behaviour (Schwarz & Oyserman Reference Schwarz and Oyserman2001; Paulhus & Vazire Reference Paulhus and Vazire2007; Kormos & Gifford Reference Kormos and Gifford2014). In addition, the survey population sampled may also be biased. Participants were recruited during a global pandemic and overwhelmingly identified themselves as female; the survey also took approximately 20–30 min to complete, and guardians prepared to volunteer for a survey of this length may be more invested in their cat’s care than the average cat guardian. Therefore, survey responses may not be an accurate representation of the general population. Due to some overlap between the validated scales and questions such as frequency of stress and other problem behaviours, findings between the two may be limited in their ability for meaningful comparison. Finally, as this was a cross-sectional study it does not prove a causal link between factors that were associated.

Future studies should further investigate the role of play in cat welfare with special attention paid to indoor and outdoor requirements, the mechanisms of how play impacts cat-guardian relationships, communication, socialisation, and play preferences in both cats and guardians.

Animal welfare implications and conclusion

Play has potential to be used as both an indicator and promotor of cat welfare. The present study aimed to assess the play factors associated with welfare in cats. Within this study, multiple welfare-impacting measures were used, including cat QOL score, the quality of the relationship between cat and guardian, the prevalence of problem behaviours and observations of behavioural changes. Results showed significant associations between cat playfulness and the number of games played with cat QOL scores. Cat-guardian relationship scores were significantly associated with the amount of daily play and number of games engaged in, whether the cat and guardian both initiated play sessions and guardian playfulness. Problem behaviours that clustered on a PCA into a stress component were significantly associated with QOL scores, but no other problem behaviours were found to have significant associations with play or welfare measures. Behavioural changes that indicated stress, frustration, or unease were reported when play had been withheld. This study shows support for the association between play and welfare and provides several avenues for further research. Future investigations should focus on the role of play in assessments of cat welfare, the use of play as an intervention to promote welfare, and the mechanisms of how play impacts cat welfare and cat-guardian relationships.

Acknowledgements

In loving memory of Edward ‘Teddy’ Henning, a cat beyond measure. This research was funded by the University of Adelaide.

Competing interests

None. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: JSLH, TN, EF, SH; Data curation: JSLH, TN, EF, SH; Formal analysis: JSLH; Investigation: JSLH, TN, EF, SH; Methodology: JSLH, TN, EF, SH; Project administration: JSLH, SH; Resources: JSLH, TN, EF, SH; Software: JSLH; Supervision: JSLH, SH; Validation: JSLH, TN, EF, SH; Visualisation: JSLH; Writing –original draft: JSLH; Writing – review and editing: JSLH, TN, EF, SH

References

Ahloy-Dallaire, J, Espinosa, J and Mason, G 2018 Play and optimal welfare: does play indicate the presence of positive affective states? Behavioural processes, 156, 315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arhant, C, Wogritsch, R and Troxler, J 2015 Assessment of behavior and physical condition of shelter cats as animal-based indicators of welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 10(5), 399406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M and Allen, C 2011 Intentional communication, and social play: how and why animals negotiate and agree to play. In: Bekoff, M and Allen, C (Eds.) Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives pp 97114. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Burghardt, GM 2005 The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Mit Press: Cambridge, MA, USA.Google Scholar
Carlstead, K, Brown, JL and Strawn, W 1993 Behavioral and physiological correlates of stress in laboratory cats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 38(2), 143158. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(93)90062-TCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K, Seidensticker, J and Baldwin, R 1991 Environmental enrichment for zoo bears. Zoo Biology, 10(1), 316, New Jersey. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430100103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carney, HC, Sadek, TP, Curtis, TM, Halls, V, Heath, S, Hutchison, P, Mundschenk, K and Westropp, JL 2014 AAFP and ISFM guidelines for diagnosing and solving house-soiling behavior in cats. Journal of feline medicine and surgery, 16(7), 579598. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X14539092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowley, SL Cecchetti, M and McDonald, RA 2019 Hunting behaviour in domestic cats: An exploratory study of risk and responsibility among cat owners. People and Nature, 1(1), 1830, United States. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, SL Cecchetti, M and McDonald, RA 2020 Diverse perspectives of cat owners indicate barriers to and opportunities for managing cat predation of wildlife. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 18(10), 544549. https://doi.org/10.1002/fee.2254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dattilo, J, Kleiber, D and Williams, R 1998 Self-determination and enjoyment enhancement: A psychologically based service delivery model for therapeutic recreation. Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 32, 258271.Google Scholar
Ellis, S, Rodan, I, Carney, HC, Heath, S, Rochlitz, I, Shearburn, LD, Sundahl, E and Westropp, JL 2013 AAFP and ISFM feline environmental needs guidelines. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery, 15(3), 219230. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X13477537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, R Lyons, M Brewer, G and Bethell, E 2021 A domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus) model of triarchic psychopathy factors: Development and initial validation of the CAT-Tri+ questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 95: 104161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagen, R 1978 Evolutionary biological models of animal play behavior. The development of behavior: Comparative and evolutionary aspects: 385404.Google Scholar
Fernandez, EJ 2022. Training as enrichment: A critical review. Animal Welfare, 31(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.31.1.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finka, LR, Ripari, L, Quinlan, L, Haywood, C, Puzzo, J, Jordan, A, Tsui, J, Foreman-Worsley, R, Dixon, L and Brennan, ML 2022 Investigation of humans individual differences as predictors of their animal interaction styles, focused on the domestic cat. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foreman-Worsley, R and Farnworth, MJ 2019 A systematic review of social and environmental factors and their implications for indoor cat welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 220, 104841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104841CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foreman-Worsley, R, Finka, LR, Ward, SJ and Farnworth, MJ 2021 Indoors or outdoors? An international exploration of owner demographics and decision making associated with lifestyle of pet cats. Animals, 11(2), 253. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghasemi, A and Zahediasl, S 2012 Normality tests for statistical analysis: A guide for non-statisticians. International Journal of Endocrinology and Metabolism, 10(2): 486. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijem.3505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, KL and Burghardt, GM 2010 Current perspectives on the biological study of play: Signs of progress. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 85(4), 393418. https://doi.org/10.1086/656903CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guyot, GW, Bennett, TL and Cross, HA 1980 The effects of social isolation on the behavior of juvenile domestic cats. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 13(3), 317329. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420130307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, SL, Bradshaw, JW and Robinson, IH 2002 Object play in adult domestic cats: the roles of habituation and disinhibition. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 79(3), 263271. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00153-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haywood, C, Ripari, L, Puzzo, J, Foreman-Worsley, R and Finka, LR 2021 Providing humans with practical, best practice handling guidelines during human-cat interactions increases cats’ affiliative behaviour and reduces aggression and signs of conflict. Frontiers in Veterinary Science: 835 https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.714143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, S 2007 Behaviour problems and welfare. In Heath, S (Ed.) The welfare of cats pp 91118 Springer: New York City, NY, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hector, A 2021 The new statistics with R: An introduction for biologists. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198729051.003.0009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Held, SD and Špinka, M 2011 Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 891899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henning, JSL, Fernandez, EJ, Nielsen, N and Hazel, S 2022a Play and welfare in domestic cats: Current knowledge and future directions. Animal Welfare 31: 407421. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.31.4.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henning, JSL, Nielsen, T, Fernandez, EJ and Hazel, S 2022b Factors associated with play behaviour in human-cat dyads. Journal of Veterinary Behaviour 52–53, 2130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, TJ, Bowen, J, Fatjó, J, Calvo, P, Holloway, A and Bennett, PC 2017 Development of the cat-owner relationship scale (CORS). Behavioural Processes, 141, 305315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.02.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iso-Ahola, SE 2022 Toward a theory of conscious–nonconscious processing and getting hard (and easy) things done in everyday life. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(1), 40. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000291Google Scholar
Jensen, JB, Sandøe, P and Nielsen, SS 2020 Owner-related reasons matter more than behavioural problems—A study of why owners relinquished dogs and cats to a Danish animal shelter from 1996 to 2017. Animals, 10(6), 1064. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10061064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kogan, LR and Grigg, EK 2021 Laser light pointers for use in companion cat play: Association with guardian-reported abnormal repetitive behaviors. Animals, 11(8), 2178. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11082178CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kormos, C and Gifford, R 2014 The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 40, 359371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kry, K and Casey, R 2007 The effect of hiding enrichment on stress levels and behaviour of domestic cats (Felis sylvestris catus) in a shelter setting and the implications for adoption potential. Animal Welfare, 16(3), 375383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loberg, JM and Lundmark, F 2016 The effect of space on behaviour in large groups of domestic cats kept indoors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 182, 2329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mertens, C and Turner, DC 1988 Experimental analysis of human-cat interactions during first encounters. Anthrozoös, 2(2), 8397. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279389787058109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odendaal, JS and Meintjes, RA 2003 Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. The Veterinary Journal, 165(3), 296301. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-0233(02)00237-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveira, AFS, Rossi, AO, Silva, LFR, Lau, MC and Barreto, RE 2010 Play behaviour in nonhuman animals and the animal welfare issue. Journal of Ethology, 28(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-009-0167-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottway, D and Hawkins, D 2003 Cat housing in rescue shelters: A welfare comparison between communal and discrete-unit housing. Animal Welfare, 12(2), 173189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Burghardt, GM Smuts, B, Cordoni, G, Dall’Olio, S, Fouts, HN, Řeháková-Petrů, M, Siviy, SM and Pellis, SM 2016. Rough-and-tumble play as a window on animal communication. Biological Reviews, 91(2), 311327. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12172CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, C 2014 Value Conflicts in Feral Cat Management: Trap-Neuter-Return or Trap-Euthanize. CABI: Boston, MA, USA.Google Scholar
Patronek, GJ, Glickman, LT, Beck, AM, McCabe, GP and Ecker, C 1996 Risk factors for relinquishment of cats to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 209(3): 582588.Google Scholar
Paulhus, DL and Vazire, S 2007 The self-report method. Handbook of Research Methods in Personality Psychology, 1: 224239.Google Scholar
Pellis, SM, O’Brien, DP, Pellis, VC, Teitelbaum, P, Wolgin, DL and Kennedy, S 1988 Escalation of feline predation along a gradient from avoidance through “play” to killing. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102(5): 760. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.102.5.760CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellis, SM, Burghardt, GM, Palagi, E and Mangel, M 2015 Modeling play: Distinguishing between origins and current functions. Adaptive Behavior, 23(6), 331339. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712315596053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PDSA 2022 Animal Wellbeing (PAW) Report. www.pdsa.org.uk/paw-report-22Google Scholar
Pyari, MS, Uccheddu, S, Lenkei, R and Pongrácz, P 2021 Inexperienced but still interested–indoor-only cats are more inclined for predatory play than cats with outdoor access. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 105373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rehnberg, LK Robert, KA, Watson, SJ, and Peters, RA 2015 The effects of social interaction and environmental enrichment on the space use, behaviour and stress of owned housecats facing a novel environment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 169, 5161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.06.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochlitz, I 2005 A review of the housing requirements of domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus) kept in the home. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 93(1–2), 97109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N and Oyserman, D 2001 Asking questions about behavior: Cognition, communication, and questionnaire construction. The American Journal of Evaluation, 22(2), 127160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1098-2140(01)00133-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seitz, PF 1959 Infantile experience and adult behavior in animal subjects: II. Age of separation from the mother and adult behavior in the cat. Psychosomatic Medicine, 21(5), 353378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shen, XS, Chick, G and Zinn, H 2014 Validating the Adult Playfulness Trait Scale (APTS): An examination of personality, behavior, attitude, and perception in the nomological network of playfulness. American Journal of Play, 6(3), 345369.Google Scholar
Shenhav, A, Rand, DG and Greene, JD 2016 The relationship between intertemporal choice and following the path of least resistance across choices, preferences, and beliefs. Preferences, and Beliefs. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2724547Google Scholar
Strickler, BL and Shull, EA 2014 An owner survey of toys, activities, and behavior problems in indoor cats. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 9(5), 207214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stella, JL, Lord, LK, and Buffington, CT 2011 Sickness behaviors in response to unusual external events in healthy cats and cats with feline interstitial cystitis. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 238(1), 6773. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.238.1.67CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stella, J Croney, C and Buffington, T 2014 Environmental factors that affect the behavior and welfare of domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus) housed in cages. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 160, 94105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.08.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takagi, S, Chijiiwa, H, Arahori, M, Saito, A, Fujita, K and Kuroshima, H 2021 Socio-spatial cognition in cats: Mentally mapping owner’s location from voice. PloS One, 16(11), e0257611 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257611CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tatlock, S, Gober, M, Williamson, N and Arbuckle, R 2017 Development and preliminary psychometric evaluation of an owner-completed measure of feline quality of life. The Veterinary Journal, 228, 2232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2017.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, DC 1991 The ethology of the human-cat relationship. Schweizer Archiv Fur Tierheilkunde, 133(2), 6370.Google ScholarPubMed
Vanderschuren, LJ and Trezza, V 2013 What the laboratory rat has taught us about social play behavior: Role in behavioral development and neural mechanisms. The Neurobiology of Childhood, 189212. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2013_268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitale, KR 2022 The social lives of free-ranging cats. Animals, 12(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12010126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wald, DM and Peterson, AL 2020 Cats and Conservationists: The Debate Over who Owns the Outdoors. Purdue University Press: New York, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, M 1974 Social play in the domestic cat. American Zoologist, 14(1), 427436. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/14.1.427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, J and Yates, D 2017 Staying in or going out? The dilemma for cat welfare. The Veterinary Record, 180(8), 193. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.j938CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Problem behaviours reported by cat guardians (n = 1,591). Guardians were able to report multiple behaviours. Based on online survey responses of guardians between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Figure 1

Table 2. Loading for 18 problem behaviours reported by cat guardians, generated by means of a Principal Component Analysis

Figure 2

Table 3. Results of a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each factor associated with quality of life in cats (n = 1,590) except for: cat age (n = 1,584) and cat playfulness (n = 1,589). Based on online survey responses of cat guardians between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Figure 3

Table 4. General Linear Model parameter estimates of factors associated with cat quality of life scores (n = 1,583), based on online survey cat guardian responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Figure 4

Table 5. Results of a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each factor associated with the cat owner relationship scale (CORS) (n = 1,590) except for: guardian gender (n = 1,582), do you avoid play (n = 1,580), who initiates play (n = 1,451) and relationship status (n = 1,567). Based on online survey cat guardian responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Figure 5

Table 6. General Linear Model parameter estimates of factors associated with cat-owner relationship scores (n = 1,372), based on online survey responses of cat guardians between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Figure 6

Table 7. Guardian-reported cat behaviour changes when play was withheld, reported by guardians (n = 468). Guardians could report more than one behaviour change. Based on online survey responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021

Figure 7

Table 8. Examples of guardian-reported cat behaviour changes when play was withheld (n = 468). Based on online survey responses between June 22nd and July 17th, 2021