Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T05:05:22.081Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Frequency and extent of cognitive complaint following adult civilian mild traumatic brain injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2022

Arielle M. Levy
Affiliation:
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia
Michael M. Saling
Affiliation:
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia
Jacqueline F. I. Anderson*
Affiliation:
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia Psychology Department, The Alfred Hospital, Melbourne, VIC 3004, Australia
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Objective:

Cognitive symptoms are associated with return to work, healthcare use and quality of life after mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). Additionally, while overall ‘post-concussion’ symptoms are often present at similar levels in mTBI and control groups, cognitive complaints may be specifically elevated in mTBI. A systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to investigate the frequency and extent of cognitive complaints following adult civilian mTBI, and compare it to the frequency and extent of complaints in control populations (PROSPERO: CRD42020151284).

Method:

This review included studies published up to March 2022. Thirteen studies were included in the systematic review, and six were included in the meta-analysis. Data extraction and quality assessment were conducted by two independent reviewers.

Results:

Cognitive complaints are common after mTBI, although reported rates differed greatly across studies. Results suggested that mTBI groups report cognitive complaints to a significantly greater extent than control groups (Hedges’ g = 0.85, 95% CI 0.31–1.40, p = .0102). Heterogeneity between studies was high (τ2 = 0.20, 95% CI 0.04–1.58; I2 = 75.0%, 95% CI 43.4%–89.0%). Between-group differences in symptom reporting were most often found when healthy rather than injured controls were employed.

Conclusions:

Cognitive complaints are consistently reported after mTBI, and are present at greater levels in mTBI patients than in controls. Despite the importance of these complaints, including in regards to return to work, healthcare use and quality of life, there has been limited research in this area, and heterogeneity in research methodology is common.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Australasian Society for the Study of Brain Impairment

Mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is the most common type of traumatic brain injury, making up approximately 70%–90% of all traumatic brain injuries, and resulting in about 100-300/100,000 hospital-evaluated cases per year worldwide (Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Carroll, Peloso, Borg, Von Holst, Holm and Coronado2004). Since many people with mTBI do not seek medical attention, researchers have estimated that the true prevalence of mTBI may be upwards of 600 people per 100,000 (Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Carroll, Peloso, Borg, Von Holst, Holm and Coronado2004).

The prognosis following mTBI is considered to be largely positive, with reports suggesting that the majority of individuals recover fully within approximately 3 months (Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, Borg, Von Holst, Holm and Pépin2004). Nevertheless, there is a subgroup of patients who experience poor recovery, evident by the reporting of ongoing ‘post-concussion’ symptoms. Recent research has suggested that this subgroup may be much larger than previously recognised, with potentially a majority of patients experiencing long-term post-concussion symptoms (Machamer et al., Reference Machamer, Temkin, Dikmen, Nelson, Barber, Hwang and Zafonte2022).

Some of the most common self-reported post-concussion symptoms are cognitive symptoms, or cognitive complaints (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012). These refer to subjective reports of reduced cognitive ability, typically within the domains known to be affected in mTBI, including memory, attention, processing speed and executive function (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Ngwenya et al., Reference Ngwenya, Gardner, Yue, Burke, Ferguson, Huang and Manley2018; Rabinowitz & Levin, Reference Rabinowitz and Levin2014). Research suggests these symptoms can persist even several years following injury (Theadom et al., Reference Theadom, Starkey, Barker-Collo, Jones, Ameratunga and Feigin2018), emphasising the need for further research in this area.

Cognitive complaints have received limited attention in mTBI research, possibly due to the fact that these symptoms do not reliably correspond to objective cognitive performance (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Stillman et al., Reference Stillman, Madigan, Torres, Swan and Alexander2019). In fact, research on cognitive performance suggests that the majority of individuals return to premorbid levels of cognitive functioning after mTBI (Iverson et al., Reference Iverson, Karr, Gardner, Silverberg and Terry2019; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Huie, Boscardin, Nelson, Barber, Yaffe and Gardner2022). Nevertheless, cognitive symptoms continue to be reported, and these symptoms are associated with other important outcome factors after mTBI, including quality of life and return to work (Schraa, Reference Schraa1995; Theadom et al., Reference Theadom, Barker-Collo, Jones, Kahan, Te Ao, McPherson and Te Ao2017; Voormolen et al., Reference Voormolen, Polinder, von Steinbuechel, Vos, Cnossen and Haagsma2019; Wrightson & Gronwall, Reference Wrightson and Gronwall1981; Yousefzadeh-Chabok et al., Reference Yousefzadeh-Chabok, Kapourchali and Ramezani2021). In addition, cognitive symptoms are often the precipitant for referral to specialist neuropsychological services. Thus, it is important to understand these symptoms in order to improve patient outcomes, and to minimise the substantial financial burden of mTBI, of which healthcare use and delayed return to work are both large contributors (Te Ao et al., Reference Te Ao, Brown, Tobias, Ameratunga, Barker-Collo, Theadom and Feigin2014). There is a paucity of research in this area, however, as most mTBI symptomatology research focuses only on overall post-concussion symptoms.

Cognitive symptoms are also particularly important because, in contrast to general post-concussion symptoms, cognitive complaints may differentiate between mTBI patients and control groups. There is a large body of research on the non-specificity of overall post-concussion symptoms, which are often found to be present at similar levels in mTBI patients and controls (Dean et al., Reference Dean, O’Neill and Sterr2012; Meares et al., Reference Meares, Shores, Taylor, Batchelor, Bryant, Baguley and Marosszeky2011). There are intuitive reasons to expect that cognitive complaints, specifically, might be elevated in mTBI, and there is an assumption in clinical practice that this is the case. However, this assumption has not been formally assessed through prior review studies, and in actuality, cognitive symptoms are observed in a range of populations, including in non-brain-injured trauma patients and healthy individuals (Cargin et al., Reference Cargin, Collie, Masters and Maruff2008; Iverson & Lange, Reference Iverson and Lange2003; Meares et al., Reference Meares, Shores, Taylor, Batchelor, Bryant, Baguley and Marosszeky2011; Pullens et al., Reference Pullens, De Vries and Roukema2010). Thus, it is not currently clear whether these symptoms are greater (in frequency and/or severity) in patients with mTBI than in control populations, and further research is necessary to examine this hypothesis.

There are a number of factors to consider when exploring cognitive complaints after mTBI. Psychological factors (e.g. depression and anxiety) and female sex have consistently been linked to increased post-concussion symptoms after mTBI (Anderson & Jordan, Reference Anderson and Jordan2021; Cnossen et al., Reference Cnossen, van der Naalt, Spikman, Nieboer, Yue, Winkler and Lingsma2018; Meares et al., Reference Meares, Shores, Batchelor, Baguley, Chapman, Gurka and Marosszeky2006), and are therefore relevant, potentially confounding factors to consider in this area of research. Age is also known to affect symptoms after mTBI and is therefore another potential confound to consider (Cassidy, Boyle, et al., Reference Cassidy, Boyle and Carroll2014; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Hunt and Ouchterlony2017; Li et al., Reference Li, Risacher, McAllister and Saykin2017). Symptom reporting is also likely to be impacted by the duration after injury at which follow-up occurs (McCrea et al., Reference McCrea, Iverson, McAllister, Hammeke, Powell, Barr and Kelly2009). Other considerations involve how mTBI is defined and how diagnosis is ascertained. Both of these factors determine the nature and representativeness of mTBI samples, and both commonly vary between studies and contribute to variability in study findings (Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus & Coronado, Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus and Coronado2004).

This review is specifically interested in the patterns of cognitive complaints observed following civilian mTBI, as this represents the majority of mTBI. Civilians with mTBI represent a distinct subgroup in the mTBI literature as they experience different symptom burdens than individuals with military- or sports-related injuries (Beauchamp et al., Reference Beauchamp, Boucher, Neveu, Ouellet, Archambault, Berthelot and Le Sage2021; Chapman & Diaz-Arrastia, Reference Chapman and Diaz-Arrastia2014).

The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis is to determine (1) the frequency and extent of cognitive complaints following adult civilian mTBI, and (2) whether these complaints are greater in mTBI relative to control groups. An additional aim is to review the quality of the existing literature on this topic.

Methods

Study protocol and search strategy

The protocol for this systematic review and meta-analysis was preregistered through PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42020151284). Reporting of this review followed Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) and Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, Altman, Antes and Tugwell2009; Stroup et al., Reference Stroup, Berlin, Morton, Olkin, Williamson, Rennie and Thacker2000).

The following databases were searched for studies published prior to 25 March 2022: Medline, PsycINFO, Emcare, Embase, Web of Science and Scopus. The full electronic search strategy is given in the Appendix (see Supplementary Material).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies of interest for inclusion in the systematic review were peer-reviewed journal articles reporting cognitive complaint data following adult civilian mTBI. Studies were eligible for inclusion whether they reported this data as frequency data (e.g. the proportion of a sample reporting cognitive complaints) or as continuous data (e.g. scores on a cognitive complaint scale). Note that continuous data was interpreted as representing the overall extent of cognitive complaint, as higher scores on each scale could indicate either a greater number of symptoms endorsed, symptoms endorsed to a greater severity or both. Studies were also eligible whether or not they included a control group. The complete list of inclusion and exclusion criteria is listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for each study

a If studies included an mTBI group within the required age range and a control group outside of the age range, the study was included and treated as a case series.

b This includes studies that reported overall post-concussion symptom data without reporting cognitive complaint data separately, and studies that only reported inferential statistics involving cognitive complaint data (e.g. correlation coefficients or beta values) as there was no way to extract participant ratings of cognitive complaint from these studies.

Study screening, data extraction and quality analysis

Covidence online systematic review software (Veritas Health Innovation, 2019) was used for study screening, data extraction and quality analysis. The first author conducted title and abstract screening and full-text screening. Studies that passed full-text screening were reviewed with the final author for consensus for inclusion in the review.

Two independent reviewers completed data extraction and quality analysis for each included study. Any disagreements were settled through discussion until consensus was reached. Where the two primary reviewers could not reach consensus, a third reviewer was engaged and majority opinion was taken. Data extracted from each study included study design data, participant characteristics and demographics, and cognitive symptom data. Study authors were contacted for numerical data values in cases where symptom data was reported in figures.

The quality of each study was evaluated using the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS), designed to assess the quality of non-randomised studies included in systematic reviews (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Shea, O’Connell, Peterson, Welch, Losos and Tugwell2000). The NOS is recommended by the Cochrane Collaboration as well as various methodological review papers (Deeks et al., Reference Deeks, Dinnes, D’Amico, Sowden, Sakarovitch, Song and Altman2003; Higgins & Green, Reference Higgins and Green2011; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Zhang, Kwong, Zhang, Li, Sun and Du2015). The cohort study version of this scale evaluates studies on the domains of group selection, comparability of study groups and ascertainment of outcome. Studies are eligible for a maximum of one ‘star’ for each item within the selection and outcome categories, and a maximum of two stars for the comparability item. The form was modified to fit the current research question. As several included studies were case series and did not include a control group, the scale was additionally modified for use with these studies, similar to previous approaches (Lawley et al., Reference Lawley, Lain, Algert, Ford, Figtree and Roberts2015; Murad, Sultan, Haffar & Bazerbachi, Reference Murad, Sultan, Haffar and Bazerbachi2018).

Data analysis

Intended analyses included determining the overall frequency of cognitive complaints, subgroup analyses examining cognitive domain and time since injury, and comparison of the extent of cognitive complaints in mTBI and control groups. Due to their non-comparability, frequency data and continuous data were analysed separately.

The summary measure used for meta-analysis was the standardised mean difference (SMD). The SMD is a measure of effect size that allows for pooling of outcome data across the use of different outcome scales through standardising the difference between groups in each study. The current meta-analysis used Hedges’ g, a specific form of SMD, which corrects for bias in effect size estimations when small samples are used (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009).

A random-effects model was used to pool outcomes, based on the expectation that included studies would differ in their underlying true effects, a scenario that is highly likely due to methodological differences in studies being combined (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). The Hartung–Knapp adjustment was applied to the model to reduce the risk of a false positive result, which meta-analyses can be particularly susceptible to when they contain a small number of studies with substantial heterogeneity (Inthout, Ioannidis & Borm, Reference Inthout, Ioannidis and Borm2014).

Heterogeneity between studies was quantified using τ2 and I 2. τ2 is defined as the variance of the true effect sizes of the population of studies, on the same scale as the SMD. Thus, on a distribution of the true underlying effect sizes, the SMD is the estimate of the mean of the distribution, and τ2 is the variance of the distribution (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). To estimate τ2, the restricted maximum-likelihood method was used, as recommended by Veroniki et al. (Reference Veroniki, Jackson, Viechtbauer, Bender, Bowden, Knapp, Kuss and Salanti2016). I 2 gives the percentage of the total observed variation that is attributed to differences in true effect sizes underlying the included studies, as opposed to random error (where total variation can be thought of as the sum of the true between-studies variation and the within-study error) (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). I 2 can range from 0% to 100% and I 2 values of 25%, 50% and 75% can be interpreted as indicating low, moderate and high heterogeneity, respectively (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks & Altman, Reference Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman2003).

All quantitative analysis was conducted in R (version 3.6.1; R Development Core Team, 2011) using the following packages: tidyverse (Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François and Yutani2019), meta (Balduzzi, Rücker & Schwarzer, Reference Balduzzi, Rücker and Schwarzer2019) and dmetar (Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa & Ebert, Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2019).

Results

As shown in Fig. 1, 3057 studies were screened by the first author for inclusion in the systematic review, of which 445 were assessed through full-text review. The resulting 19 papers underwent review by the first and last author, with a further 6 papers excluded by consensus (see Fig. 1 for exclusion reasons). The 13 remaining papers were included in the systematic review (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Clarke, Genat & Anderson, Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Norman, Shah & Turkstra, Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang & Suffoletto, Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011; Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris & Vos, Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012; Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017; Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg & van der Werf, Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Edmed, Greenslade, White, Chu, Lukin and Lurie2017).

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram. Modified from Moher et al. (Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, Altman, Antes and Tugwell2009).

One of the most common reasons for exclusion during full-text review was on the basis of age range of participants. Importantly, this was often due to insufficient reporting of information, as approximately one quarter of the studies excluded on this basis did not report participant age range.

Of the 13 included papers, study authors were contacted for numerical data values in the case of four studies where outcome data was reported in figures (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007). This additional data was provided for one study (Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017). For the remaining studies, figures reporting cognitive complaint data were used to derive ranges of symptom endorsement. These data ranges were included in the qualitative synthesis but were not included in the meta-analysis.

Systematic review

Quality assessment of included studies

There was 83.33% agreement between raters across the three NOS domains. Inter-rater reliability yielded a Cohen’s kappa of 0.73. This reflects moderate agreement by conservative approaches, and is high relative to previous research using the same scale (Hartling et al., Reference Hartling, Hamm, Milne, Vandermeer, Santaguida, Ansari and Dryden2012; McHugh, Reference McHugh2012).

Table 2 presents ratings on the modified NOS scale for each included study. The total number of stars that each study was eligible for varied due to variability in study design. Only four of the 13 included studies were awarded a star on 80% or more of the eligible items (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011).

Table 2. Star ratings on the modified NOS scale for each included study

a Items not relevant for specific studies are left blank. This includes studies that did not include a control group, and/or those that did not involve a longitudinal component.

Each study was deemed to have a sample that was sufficiently representative of adult civilian mTBI, which was expected given that this was an inclusion criterion for the review. Additionally, almost all studies were considered to have employed a follow-up length sufficient for outcomes to occur, defined as follow-up ≥24 h after injury. However, there were some methodological shortcomings in other areas assessed by the NOS scale. Where follow-up was present, nearly all studies either had insufficient follow-up rates – defined as rates <80% (Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017) – or failed to provide this information (Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007). Studies were also likely to introduce potential bias through failing to ensure comparability of mTBI and control cohorts by not controlling for sex (Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019) or psychological factors (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012).

Characteristics of included studies

The characteristics of included studies are listed in Table 3. The sample size of each study ranged from 15 mTBI participants to 107 mTBI participants. Across the included studies, there were a total of 546 mTBI participants with cognitive complaint data available.

Table 3. Characteristics of studies included in systematic review

CC, cognitive complaint; CCAMCHI, Cognitive Complaint After Mild Closed Head Injury; CFQ, Cognitive Failures Questionnaire; ED, emergency department; HC, healthy control; HI, head injury; ImPACT PCSS, Immediate Post-Concussion Assessment and Cognitive Test post-concussion symptom scale; IQR, interquartile range; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; NSI, Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory; OI, orthopedic injury; PCSC, Post-concussive Symptom Checklist, PACCQ, Post-Hospital Admission Cognitive Complaint Questionnaire; RPQ, Rivermead Post-Concussion Symptoms Questionnaire; SD, standard deviation; SI, spinal injury; SOL, Self-Observation List; TC, trauma control

a Studies included in the quantitative synthesis are listed in bold.

b Control groups are only listed if they are within the required age range for this review (18–60), with cognitive complaint data reported.

c Based on participants with cognitive complaint data available.

d Where information on age of study participants was not reported, this information was based on study inclusion criteria (indicated by italics).

e Follow-up times are only listed for those follow-ups where cognitive complaint data was reported.

f Where frequency data was reported in figures and raw data was unable to be obtained from study authors, frequency ranges (extracted from figures) are given.

g Based on characteristics of sample at baseline before dropout (n = 126).

h Based on characteristics of full sample including those with cognitive complaint data unavailable (n = 35). Age data represents median values, not means.

i Before dropout, for n = 30.

j As cognitive outcome data for this study was reported in a figure, numerical values were obtained from study authors.

The features required for diagnosis of mTBI differed greatly between included studies, despite many studies basing their criteria on the same published definitions of mTBI. A comparison of mTBI definition between studies is shown in Table 4. This table highlights the variation in definitions of mTBI across studies. Note that if study definitions were equivalent, each column of the table would be identical.

Table 4. Definition of mTBI in studies included in systematic review

Note. A standard checkmark indicates required features, and a checkmark in brackets indicates features that were considered in the definition but not specifically required (i.e. optional features, or situations in which only one of a set of features was required). Some definitions were unclear; this represents best interpretation. For studies where comprehensive diagnostic information was not provided (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007), additional information was obtained from published definitions cited within these studies (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Meehan and Mannix2014; Vos et al., Reference Vos, Battistin, Birbamer, Gerstenbrand, Potapov, Prevec and Von Wild2002). The variation both in features of the definition (represented by each row) and whether the feature was required (represented by each symbol) highlights the degree of heterogeneity in definitions of mTBI across studies.

ACRM, American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine; EFNS, European Federation of Neurological Societies; GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale; ICD, International Classification of Diseases; WHO, World Health Organization

Ten of the 13 studies included a control group, but only seven had control groups within the required age range of this review (i.e. age 18–60) and with cognitive complaint data available. Of these studies, three employed healthy controls (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012), three employed injured controls (Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018) and one employed both (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012). One study also employed a ‘head injury’ group of trauma patients who sustained an injury to the head but did not meet criteria for mTBI (Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018). Across these studies, there were a total of 148 injured controls and 155 healthy controls with cognitive complaint data available.

Follow-up times within included studies ranged from approximately 1 day post-injury (Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018) to approximately 1.5 years post-injury (Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011). Six studies collected cognitive complaint data at or within an average of 2 weeks post-injury (Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Edmed, Greenslade, White, Chu, Lukin and Lurie2017), three studies collected this data between 2 weeks and 3 months post-injury (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019), five studies collected this data at or between 3 and 12 months post-injury (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995; Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007) and one study collected this data at more than 1 year post-injury (Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011). Four studies reported cognitive complaint data at multiple time-points (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007).

Most studies reported continuous outcome data rather than frequency data. This was either in the form of mean total scores on a scale of cognitive complaints (n = 3; Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995) or mean cognitive subscores on a scale of post-concussive symptoms (n = 6; Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012; Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Edmed, Greenslade, White, Chu, Lukin and Lurie2017). The specific cognitive functions assessed by each scale are listed in Table 5.

Table 5. Cognitive functions assessed by each scale

a List represents cognitive domains assessed by each scale, with multiple items evaluating each domain.

b The CCAMCHI is a modified version of the PACCQ scale, and assesses the same domains.

c List represents individual cognitive items from each scale.

In contrast to the studies reporting continuous outcome data, four studies reported dichotomous outcome data (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007), that is, frequencies or percentages of the sample endorsing cognitive complaints.

Cognitive symptoms overall

One study looked at the overall prevalence of cognitive complaints (Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007). This study found that 39% of their sample reported ‘serious’ cognitive complaints, defined as scores on a cognitive subscale that fell two or more standard deviations above the mean of an injured control group.

With respect to overall cognitive symptoms over time, one study found that subjective cognitive symptoms decreased significantly from 2 weeks to 1 month to 3 months (Marsh & Smith, Reference Marsh and Smith1995). Results from a second study appeared to suggest a slight decrease in frequency of cognitive symptom endorsement from 3 months to 6 months, but this was not statistically investigated (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012).

Cognitive symptoms in patients versus controls

All studies employing healthy control groups found a greater extent of cognitive complaints in mTBI patients relative to controls (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012). This was the case early after injury and in the chronic phase post-injury.

In the studies employing injured control groups, three found no differences in cognitive complaint between mTBI and control groups (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019). These studies were conducted at a range of time points post-injury, at less than 1 week, 3–12 weeks and 3–12 months post-injury. A fourth study found that on day 1 following injury, mTBI patients were 39 times more likely to report concentration complaints than trauma controls, but the odds difference between groups was non-significant by day 8 post-injury (Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018). The study also found no difference in concentration difficulties between mTBI patients and ‘head injured’ controls who did not meet criteria for mTBI.

Cognitive symptoms by cognitive domain

With regard to the assessment of individual cognitive domains, most studies reported this data in the form of frequencies of symptom endorsement. The two domains most commonly assessed individually were memory (n = 3; Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007) and concentration (n = 3; Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007). Only one study reported frequency of endorsement of processing speed symptoms (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012). None of the studies individually reported executive function symptom data.

With respect to memory symptoms, the frequency of symptom endorsement ranged from 9% to approximately 30% across studies (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007). It is noteworthy that these studies used different methods of symptom evaluation, making the data non-equivalent. The higher end of the range (15%–30%) represents studies reporting the percentage of their sample endorsing memory complaints, based on ratings on symptom scales (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Jensen, Ge, Babb, Miles, Reaume and Inglese2011). The lower end of this range (9% and 10%) was drawn from a study involving daily self-monitoring of memory symptoms and represents the percentage of the time during the 12-day study period that memory symptoms were experienced (Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007).

With respect to concentration symptoms, frequency of endorsement ranged from 6% to approximately 60% across studies (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007). As with memory symptom data, concentration symptom data was derived using varied approaches, limiting between-study comparison. Point estimates for daily self-monitoring of symptoms fell at the lower end of the range (6% and 11%; Stulemeijer et al., Reference Stulemeijer, Vos, Bleijenberg and van der Werf2007), estimates of frequency of the sample experiencing symptoms fell in the middle of the range (15%–25%; Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012) and estimates of the percentage of a sample experiencing concentration difficulties every day over 14 days fell at the higher end of the range (40%–60%; Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018). In regards to symptoms over time, the latter study showed that concentration complaints decreased over the first 14 days after injury, with about a 28% reduction each day in the odds of reporting these complaints (Pacella et al., Reference Pacella, Prabhu, Morley, Huang and Suffoletto2018).

Processing speed symptoms were reported by between 15% and 25% of individuals in one mTBI sample, with this range again based on extrapolation from a figure (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Moss-Morris, Peveler, Mogg, Bradley and Belli2012).

In addition to the studies that reported individual cognitive domain data as frequency data, one study reported this as continuous data, that is, mean scores on a symptom scale (Studerus-Germann et al., Reference Studerus-Germann, Engel, Stienen, von Ow, Hildebrandt and Gautschi2017). Symptom scores were highest for the items ‘difficulty concentrating’ and ‘difficulty remembering’, lower for ‘feeling slowed down’ and zero for the symptom ‘feeling mentally “foggy”’.

Meta-analysis

A meaningful meta-analysis on frequency data was not feasible as a result of the limited number of studies reporting frequency data, methodological differences between studies (e.g. variation in cognitive domains assessed), lack of control groups employed and lack of availability of numerical outcome data (i.e. where data was reported in figures). Therefore, a single meta-analysis was conducted, synthesising studies reporting continuous data. These studies were also highly varied in their methodology. However, they were consistent in reporting overall cognitive symptom scores rather than differing cognitive domains, and most studies had control data available. We therefore included studies that 1) reported continuous outcome data, and 2) employed a control group, to address the research question, Do patients with mTBI report cognitive symptoms to a greater extent than control groups? Based on the small number of included studies, intended subgroup analyses were unable to be conducted.

A total of six studies were included in the final meta-analysis (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012; Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012). Three of the studies employed healthy controls (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012) and two employed injured controls (Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019). The remaining study employed both healthy and trauma controls, and in this case the trauma group was used as the control group in the analysis, as this was expected to be a closer comparison (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012). The resulting meta-analysis included a total of 208 mTBI participants and 214 control participants (135 healthy controls and 79 injured controls).

Each of the six included studies reported cognitive symptom data for a single time point only; these studies ranged from assessing participants approximately 4–5 days after injury (Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006), to assessing participants 3–12 months after injury (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012).

Five different symptom scales were employed across the six studies. Available data from the five studies consisted of cognitive subscores on measures of post-concussive symptoms (Landre et al., Reference Landre, Poppe, Davis, Schmaus and Hobbs2006; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Hanlon, Dodd, Ling, Klimaj and Meier2015; Norman et al., Reference Norman, Shah and Turkstra2019; Shumskaya et al., Reference Shumskaya, Andriessen, Norris and Vos2012) or total scores on a specific cognitive symptom scale (Anderson, Reference Anderson2021; Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Genat and Anderson2012). In one case where standard deviations were not reported, they were estimated from interquartile ranges using published methods (Wan et al., Reference Wan, Wang, Liu and Tong2014).

The results of the meta-analysis are presented in Fig. 2. Given the structure of the symptom scales, scores represent the number of symptoms endorsed in combination with the severity of endorsed symptoms.

Figure 2. Results of the meta-analysis.

The meta-analysis revealed significant effects for cognitive symptom reporting in mTBI patients versus controls, suggesting that mTBI patients report cognitive symptoms to a greater extent than controls (SMD = 0.85, 95% CI 0.31–1.40, p = .0102). There was a large degree of heterogeneity between studies (τ2 = 0.20, 95% CI 0.04–1.58; I 2 = 75.0%, 95% CI 43.4%–89.0%). This was expected due to the methodological differences between studies, including differences in control groups employed and in time post-injury at which assessments took place.

No statistical outliers were detected in the analysis. Statistical tests of publication bias were not conducted given the low power of these tests when the number of included studies is low (Murad, Chu, et al., Reference Murad, Chu, Lin and Wang2018).

Discussion

This review has demonstrated that cognitive complaints are consistently reported after mTBI, although reported rates differed greatly across studies. Importantly, this study has provided the first meta-analytic evidence to suggest that cognitive complaints are reported to a greater extent (using a combined measure of frequency and severity) in mTBI patients than in control groups. This indicates that cognitive complaints may be specifically elevated in mTBI, in contrast to overall post-concussion symptoms which are typically found to be present at similar levels in mTBI and control groups (Dean et al., Reference Dean, O’Neill and Sterr2012; Meares et al., Reference Meares, Shores, Taylor, Batchelor, Bryant, Baguley and Marosszeky2011). The current review also highlighted several limitations in the literature, including inconsistencies in methodology between studies and insufficient reporting of study information.

The finding that cognitive complaints occur to a greater extent in mTBI patients than in control groups (in frequency and/or severity) indicates that, after mTBI, cognitive complaints are present beyond ‘normal’ levels. Given cognitive complaints are highly important for successful return to work and extent of healthcare use (Schraa, Reference Schraa1995; Theadom et al., Reference Theadom, Barker-Collo, Jones, Kahan, Te Ao, McPherson and Te Ao2017, Reference Theadom, Starkey, Barker-Collo, Jones, Ameratunga and Feigin2018; Wrightson & Gronwall, Reference Wrightson and Gronwall1981), they clearly warrant further attention. Further, whereas measures of overall post-concussion symptoms often do not differ between mTBI and control groups (Dean et al., Reference Dean, O’Neill and Sterr2012; Meares et al., Reference Meares, Shores, Taylor, Batchelor, Bryant, Baguley and Marosszeky2011), this review suggested that cognitive symptoms, when isolated from other post-concussion symptoms, are specifically elevated in mTBI. Thus, cognitive symptoms may be of specific clinical importance in individuals who are recovering after a mTBI, and deserve further investigation.

The meta-analysis revealed large amounts of heterogeneity between studies, of which underlying causes could not be statistically explored due to insufficient sample size. However, one potentially important source of heterogeneity may have been due to differences in the type of control groups employed. The systematic review revealed that differences in cognitive symptoms between mTBI patients and controls were most often found when healthy controls, rather than injured controls, were employed. This appeared to hold true whether studies were conducted in the acute or chronic phase following injury. Further, in the meta-analysis, the three studies that had the greatest between-group differences were those that employed healthy controls. It is therefore possible that the significant results from the meta-analysis were in part driven by the subgroup of studies that employed healthy controls rather than injured controls.

Due to the limited numbers of studies available for inclusion, the current meta-analysis was unable to determine whether mTBI patients report more cognitive complaints than injured controls specifically. This is an important question to pursue. If there is no difference in cognitive complaints between these groups, it suggests that some of the factors contributing to cognitive complaints in mTBI patients may also be present in injured control patients. In particular, cognitive symptoms may be related to stressors associated with general injury rather than mTBI-related impairment (Cassidy, Cancelliere, et al., Reference Cassidy, Cancelliere, Carroll, Côté, Hincapié, Holm and Borg2014). It is also possible, however, that there are factors specific to mTBI which elevate cognitive complaints, either due to true cognitive changes resulting from brain injury or due to psychosocial factors inherent to mTBI (e.g. illness perceptions (Anderson & Fitzgerald, Reference Anderson and Fitzgerald2018; Whittaker et al., Reference Whittaker, Kemp and House2007) or ‘diagnosis threat’ (Ozen & Fernandes, Reference Ozen and Fernandes2011)).

With regards to time since injury, findings from the systematic review suggested that cognitive symptoms appear to decrease over time. This aligns with previous research, which has shown that overall post-concussive complaints decrease over time, and that cognitive performance normalises over time (Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Peloso, Borg, Von Holst, Holm and Pépin2004; Frencham et al., Reference Frencham, Fox and Maybery2005). It is important to consider that the studies included in this review spanned a very wide range with regard to when participants were assessed post-injury, ranging from days to years. Unfortunately, intended subgroup analyses on time since injury could not be conducted due to small sample size. However, given the likely role of time since injury in the reporting of cognitive complaints, it will be important for future studies to investigate this topic in samples that are within discrete post-injury time periods.

With regards to cognitive domains, mTBI patients endorsed symptoms across each of the domains that were individually assessed, that is, memory, attention and processing speed. However, studies varied greatly in regards to how domain-level data was derived, making the data non-equivalent and limiting between-study comparison. Across all included studies, including those that examined domains collectively, memory, attention and processing speed symptoms were commonly assessed. This is consistent with the literature that shows that these are common domains of objective impairment after mTBI (Rabinowitz & Levin, Reference Rabinowitz and Levin2014). In contrast, only three of the 13 included studies assessed executive function symptoms, despite this domain also being commonly impaired after mTBI (Frencham et al., Reference Frencham, Fox and Maybery2005; Rabinowitz & Levin, Reference Rabinowitz and Levin2014). Given that intended subgroup analyses on type of cognitive complaint were unable to be conducted due to limited sample size, further research is warranted to determine the domain(s) most commonly subjectively impaired after mTBI.

In considering these findings, it is noteworthy that the quality of included studies varied, with four studies meeting less than or equal to 50% of evaluated criteria, and four studies meeting over 80% of evaluated criteria. Similarly, this review found a large degree of inconsistency in methodology between studies, and this was identified as one of the primary methodological issues in the current literature. In particular, inconsistencies and vagueness in defining mTBI have been apparent in the literature for several decades now (Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus and Coronado2004; Pertab et al., Reference Pertab, James and Bigler2009; Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Iverson, Barth, Bush and Broshek2009) and this review has shown that these issues remain prevalent.

It is important to note that there were a large number of studies excluded from this review on the basis of the age of included participants. There is a well-established relationship between age and outcome after mTBI, both at the higher and lower ends of the spectrum (Cassidy, Boyle, et al., Reference Cassidy, Boyle and Carroll2014; Jacobs et al., Reference Jacobs, Beems, Stulemeijer, van Vugt, van der Vliet, Borm and Vos2010; Li et al., Reference Li, Risacher, McAllister and Saykin2017). Given this relationship, including studies involving older or younger participants – or studies where the age of participants could not be determined – would have created a potential bias for this review. Therefore, to minimise bias, only studies that directly specified that their sample fell between the ages of 18 and 60 were included. Given the impact of age on recovery from mTBI, it is recommended that future studies consider younger and older adults with mTBI as separate, unique populations.

Study limitations

The primary limitation of this systematic review and meta-analysis was small sample size. This resulted in an inability to conduct subgroup analyses, as well as an inability to formally evaluate publication bias. When only a small number of studies are available for quantitative synthesis, limitations primarily include difficulties in accurately estimating meta-analysis parameters (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). Despite these difficulties, it is still preferable to synthesise studies statistically through meta-analysis in these situations, as this provides for a more accurate representation of data than intuitive ad hoc data summaries, which can be misleading (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). This limitation was mitigated in the current study through the use of the Hartung–Knapp model adjustment in our meta-analytic approach, which reduces the risk of obtaining a false positive result in the presence of a small number of studies (Inthout et al., Reference Inthout, Ioannidis and Borm2014). The possibility of small sample sizes within studies was addressed through the use of Hedges’ g, which allows for unbiased estimates of effect size even in the presence of small samples (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). A final limitation of this study was the absence of a second reviewer for initial screening of relevant literature, due to the large number of studies identified for screening. In order to minimise the potential impact of this limitation, any screening decisions that were unclear were discussed with the second and final authors until consensus was reached; a low threshold was used for initiating this discussion.

Directions for future research

Further research is required to determine whether mTBI patients report greater cognitive complaints than injured control groups, and if so, for what time period post-injury. This will have important implications from a rehabilitation perspective, as it would contribute to understanding the relative specificity of cognitive complaint in mTBI, which could assist with patient management.

Current measures of symptom reporting typically combine symptom frequency and symptom severity into a single score. It will be important for future research to examine cognitive complaints in a manner that allows for the evaluation of complaint frequency and severity as separate entities. The disentanglement of these components will allow for understanding of whether increased levels of cognitive complaint in mTBI are a result of increased complaint frequency, increased complaint severity or both.

Future research would also benefit from executive function difficulties being routinely included in symptom assessment, to enable comprehensive investigation of cognitive symptoms. Similarly, future research would profit from thorough reporting of study information including: age ranges of included participants, how exposure to mTBI was determined, rates of participant follow-up and recruitment setting and selection procedure for control groups. Future studies are encouraged to control for factors known to impact symptom reporting, including sex and psychological factors. Direct adherence to standardised definitions of mTBI, for example, the World Health Organization definition (Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, et al., Reference Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus and Coronado2004), would further improve comparability between studies.

Conclusions

This study has confirmed that cognitive complaints are consistently reported after mTBI. Findings have provided clear evidence to suggest that these complaints are reported to a greater extent, using a combined measure of frequency and severity, in mTBI patients than in control groups. Results suggest that this difference in symptom reporting may be greater when healthy controls, rather than injured controls, are employed. Given the importance of elevations in cognitive symptom reporting for outcome after mTBI, including in the context of return to work and healthcare use (Donovan et al., Reference Donovan, Cancelliere and Cassidy2014; Theadom et al., Reference Theadom, Barker-Collo, Jones, Kahan, Te Ao, McPherson and Te Ao2017), it is evident that cognitive complaints warrant investigation. It is clear from this review, however, that there has been limited research regarding the nature and time course of cognitive complaint after mTBI. Future research into cognitive complaint, including examination of the factors contributing to these complaints, will provide an evidence-based context for clinicians to consider these complaints with respect to management and intervention.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/BrImp.2022.19

Data availability

Data and analysis code is available at the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at: https://osf.io/ckjg6/?view_only=b582da431f6b4bd98ed9998eddd0cb5d.

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge: Jacquie Eyres, who acted as the second reviewer on this project; and Cameron Patrick (Melbourne Statistical Consulting Platform), who provided statistical consulting on this project.

Financial support

This work was supported by a Melbourne Research Scholarship from the University of Melbourne, awarded to the first author.

Conflicts of interest

Arielle M. Levy has no conflicts of interest to disclose. Michael M. Saling has no conflicts of interest to disclose. Jacqueline F. I. Anderson has no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Footnotes

Note: The protocol for this systematic review and meta-analysis was preregistered through PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42020151284). Data and analysis code has been made available on the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at: https://osf.io/ckjg6/?view_only=b582da431f6b4bd98ed9998eddd0cb5d.

References

Anderson, J. F. I. (2021). Cognitive complaint and objective cognition during the post-acute period after mild traumatic brain injury in pre-morbidly healthy adults. Brain Injury, 00(00), 111. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2020.1859613.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. F. I., & Fitzgerald, P. (2018). Associations between coping style, illness perceptions and self-reported symptoms after mild traumatic brain injury in prospectively studied pre-morbidly healthy individuals. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 2011, 114. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2018.1556706.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. F. I., & Jordan, A. S. (2021). Sex predicts post-concussion symptom reporting, independently of fatigue and subjective sleep disturbance, in premorbidly healthy adults after mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 0(0), 116. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2021.1993274.Google Scholar
Balduzzi, S., Rücker, G., & Schwarzer, G. (2019). How to perform a meta-analysis with R: A practical tutorial. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 22, 153160. doi: 10.1136/ebmental-2019-300117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchamp, F., Boucher, V., Neveu, X., Ouellet, V., Archambault, P., Berthelot, S., … Le Sage, N. (2021). Post-concussion symptoms in sports-related mild traumatic brain injury compared to non-sports-related mild traumatic brain injury. Canadian Journal of Emergency Medicine, 23(2), 223231. doi: 10.1007/s43678-020-00060-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cargin, J. W., Collie, A., Masters, C., & Maruff, P. (2008). The nature of cognitive complaints in healthy older adults with and without objective memory decline. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(2), 245257. doi: 10.1080/13803390701377829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, L. J., Cassidy, J. D., Holm, L., Kraus, J., & Coronado, V. G. (2004). Methodological issues and research recommendations for mild traumatic brain injury: The WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 36(Suppl. 43), 113125. doi: 10.1080/16501960410023877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, L. J., Cassidy, J. D., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., Von Holst, H., Holm, L.Pépin, M. (2004). Prognosis for mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 36(Suppl. 43), 84105. doi: 10.1080/16501960410023859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J. D., Boyle, E., & Carroll, L. J. (2014). Population-based, inception cohort study of the incidence, course, and prognosis of mild traumatic brain injury after motor vehicle collisions. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 95(3, Suppl. 2), S278S285. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.08.295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J. D., Cancelliere, C., Carroll, L. J., Côté, P., Hincapié, C. A., Holm, L. W., … Borg, J. (2014). Systematic review of self-reported prognosis in adults after mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the International Collaboration on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Prognosis. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 95(3, Suppl. 2), S132S151. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.08.299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J. D., Carroll, L. J., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., Von Holst, H., Holm, L., … Coronado, V. G. (2004). Incidence, risk factors and prevention of mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 36(Suppl. 43), 2860. doi:10.1080/16501960410023732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, J. C., & Diaz-Arrastia, R. (2014). Military traumatic brain injury: A review. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 10, S97S104. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, L. A., Genat, R. C., & Anderson, J. (2012). Long-term cognitive complaint and post-concussive symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury: The role of cognitive and affective factors. Brain Injury, 26(3), 298307. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2012.654588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cnossen, M. C., van der Naalt, J., Spikman, J. M., Nieboer, D., Yue, J. K., Winkler, E. A., … Lingsma, H. (2018). Prediction of persistent post-concussion symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 35(22), 26912698. doi: 10.1089/neu.2017.5486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dean, P. J. A., O’Neill, D., & Sterr, A. (2012). Post-concussion syndrome: Prevalence after mild traumatic brain injury in comparison with a sample without head injury. Brain Injury, 26(1), 1426. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2011.635354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deeks, J., Dinnes, J., D’Amico, R., Sowden, A., Sakarovitch, C., Song, F.Altman, D. (2003). Evaluating non-randomised intervention studies. Health Technology Assessment, 7(27), iii109.10.3310/hta7270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donovan, J., Cancelliere, C., & Cassidy, J. D. (2014). Summary of the findings of the International Collaboration on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Prognosis. Chiropractic & Manual Therapies, 22(1), 38. doi: 10.1186/s12998-014-0038-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, M. A., Meehan, W. P., & Mannix, R. (2014). Duration and course of post-concussive symptoms. Pediatrics, 133(6), 9991006. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-0158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frencham, K. A. R., Fox, A. M., & Maybery, M. T. (2005). Neuropsychological studies of mild traumatic brain injury: A meta-analytic review of research since 1995. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 334351. doi: 10.1080/13803390490520328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. & Ebert, D. D. (2019). dmetar: Companion R package for the guide “Doing Meta-Analysis in R”. R package version 0.0.9000. Retrieved from http://dmetar.protectlab.org.Google Scholar
Hartling, L., Hamm, M., Milne, A., Vandermeer, B., Santaguida, P. L., Ansari, M., … Dryden, D. M. (2012). Validity and inter-rater reliability testing of quality assessment instruments. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Retrieved form www.effectivehealthcare.ahrq.gov/reports/final.cfm.Google ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. Retrieved form www.handbook.cochrane.org.Google Scholar
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hou, R., Moss-Morris, R., Peveler, R., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Belli, A. (2012). When a minor head injury results in enduring symptoms: A prospective investigation of risk factors for postconcussional syndrome after mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 83(2), 217223. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-300767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, T., Hunt, C., & Ouchterlony, D. (2017). Is age associated with the severity of post–mild traumatic brain injury symptoms? Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 44(04), 384390. doi: 10.1017/cjn.2016.441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inthout, J., Ioannidis, J. P., & Borm, G. F. (2014). The Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman method for random effects meta-analysis is straightforward and considerably outperforms the standard DerSimonian-Laird method. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 14(1), 112. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, G. L., Karr, J. E., Gardner, A. J., Silverberg, N. D., & Terry, D. P. (2019). Results of scoping review do not support mild traumatic brain injury being associated with a high incidence of chronic cognitive impairment: Commentary on McInnes et al. 2017. PLoS ONE, 14(9), 120. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, G. L., & Lange, R. T. (2003). Examination of “postconcussion-like” symptoms in a healthy sample. Applied Neuropsychology, 10(3), 137144.10.1207/S15324826AN1003_02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, B., Beems, T., Stulemeijer, M., van Vugt, A. B., van der Vliet, T. M., Borm, G. F., & Vos, P. E. (2010). Outcome prediction in mild traumatic brain injury: Age and clinical variables are stronger predictors than CT abnormalities. Journal of Neurotrauma, 27(4), 655668. doi: 10.1089/neu.2009.1059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landre, N., Poppe, C. J., Davis, N., Schmaus, B., & Hobbs, S. E. (2006). Cognitive functioning and postconcussive symptoms in trauma patients with and without mild TBI. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(4), 255273. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.12.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawley, C. M., Lain, S. J., Algert, C. S., Ford, J. B., Figtree, G. A., & Roberts, C. L. (2015). Prosthetic heart valves in pregnancy, outcomes for women and their babies: A systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 122(11), 14461455. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.13491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, W., Risacher, S. L., McAllister, T. W., & Saykin, A. J. (2017). Age at injury is associated with the long-term cognitive outcome of traumatic brain injuries. Alzheimer’s and Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Disease Monitoring, 6, 196200. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.01.008.Google ScholarPubMed
Machamer, J., Temkin, N., Dikmen, S., Nelson, L. D., Barber, J., Hwang, P.Zafonte, R. (2022). Symptom frequency and persistence in the first year after traumatic brain injury: A TRACK-TBI study. Journal of Neurotrauma, 39(5–6), 358370. doi: 10.1089/neu.2021.0348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, N. V., & Smith, M. D. (1995). Post-concussion syndrome and the coping hypothesis. Brain Injury, 9(6), 553562. doi: 10.3109/02699059509008214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, A. R., Hanlon, F. M., Dodd, A. B., Ling, J. M., Klimaj, S. D., & Meier, T. B. (2015). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of cognitive control and neurosensory deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Human Brain Mapping, 36(11), 43944406. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrea, M., Iverson, G. L., McAllister, T. W., Hammeke, T. A., Powell, M. R., Barr, W. B., & Kelly, J. P. (2009). An integrated review of recovery after mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI): Implications for clinical management. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(8), 13681390. doi: 10.1080/13854040903074652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHugh, M. L. (2012). Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochemia Medica, 22(3), 276282. doi: 10.11613/bm.2012.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meares, S., Shores, E. A., Batchelor, J., Baguley, I. J., Chapman, J., Gurka, J., & Marosszeky, J. E. (2006). The relationship of psychological and cognitive factors and opioids in the development of the postconcussion syndrome in general trauma patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(6), 792801. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meares, S., Shores, E. A., Taylor, A. J., Batchelor, J., Bryant, R. A., Baguley, I. J., … Marosszeky, J. E. (2011). The prospective course of postconcussion syndrome: The role of mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 25(4), 454465. doi: 10.1037/a0022580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., Altman, D., Antes, G., … Tugwell, P. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7). doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murad, M. H., Chu, H., Lin, L., & Wang, Z. (2018). The effect of publication bias magnitude and direction on the certainty in evidence. BMJ Evidence-Based Medicine, 23(3), 8486. doi: 10.1136/bmjebm-2018-110891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murad, M. H., Sultan, S., Haffar, S., & Bazerbachi, F. (2018). Methodological quality and synthesis of case series and case reports. Evidence-Based Medicine, 23(2), 6063. doi: 10.1136/bmjebm-2017-110853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngwenya, L. B., Gardner, R. C., Yue, J. K., Burke, J. F., Ferguson, A. R., Huang, M. C., … Manley, G. T. (2018). Concordance of common data elements for assessment of subjective cognitive complaints after mild-traumatic brain injury: A TRACK-TBI Pilot Study. Brain Injury, 32(9), 10711078. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2018.1481527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, R. S., Shah, M. N., & Turkstra, L. S. (2019). Reaction time and cognitive-linguistic performance in adults with mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 33(9), 11731183. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2019.1632487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozen, L. J., & Fernandes, M. A. (2011). Effects of “diagnosis threat” on cognitive and affective functioning long after mild head injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 219229. doi: 10.1017/S135561771000144X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacella, M., Prabhu, A., Morley, J., Huang, S., & Suffoletto, B. (2018). Postconcussive symptoms over the first 14 days after mild traumatic brain injury: An experience sampling study. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 33(3), E31E39. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pertab, J. L., James, K. M., & Bigler, E. D. (2009). Limitations of mild traumatic brain injury meta-analyses. Brain Injury, 23(6), 498508. doi: 10.1080/02699050902927984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pullens, M. J. J., De Vries, J., & Roukema, J. A. (2010). Subjective cognitive dysfunction in breast cancer patients: A systematic review. Psycho-Oncology, 19(11), 11271138. doi: 10.1002/pon.1673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Rabinowitz, A. R., & Levin, H. S. (2014). Cognitive sequelae of traumatic brain injury. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 37(1), 111. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2013.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, E., Jensen, J. H., Ge, Y., Babb, J. S., Miles, L., Reaume, J.Inglese, M. (2011). Brain iron quantification in mild traumatic brain injury: A magnetic field correlation study. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 32, 18511856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, R. M., Iverson, G. L., Barth, J. T., Bush, S. S., & Broshek, D. K. (2009). Recommendations for diagnosing a mild traumatic brain injury: A National Academy of Neuropsychology education paper. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(1), 310. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, A. L. C., Huie, J. R., Boscardin, W. J., Nelson, L., Barber, J. K., Yaffe, K., … Gardner, R. C. (2022). Cognitive outcome 1 year after mild traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 98(12), e1248e1261. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000200041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schraa, J. C. (1995). Mild traumatic brain injury: Searching for the syndrome. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10(4), 2840. doi: 10.1097/00001199-199508000-00004.Google Scholar
Shumskaya, E., Andriessen, T. M. J. C., Norris, D. G., & Vos, P. E. (2012). Abnormal whole-brain functional networks in homogeneous acute mild traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 79(2), 175182. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31825f04fb.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stillman, A. M., Madigan, N., Torres, K., Swan, N., & Alexander, M. P. (2019). Subjective cognitive complaints in concussion. Journal of Neurotrauma, 311, 305311. doi: 10.1089/neu.2018.5925.Google Scholar
Stroup, D. F., Berlin, J. A., Morton, S. C., Olkin, I., Williamson, G. D., Rennie, D.Thacker, S. B. (2000). Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: A proposal for reporting. JAMA, 283(15), 20082012. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-3024-3_10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Studerus-Germann, A. M., Engel, D. C., Stienen, M. N., von Ow, D., Hildebrandt, G., & Gautschi, O. P. (2017). Three versus seven days to return-to-work after mild traumatic brain injury: A randomized parallel-group trial with neuropsychological assessment. International Journal of Neuroscience, 127(10), 900908. doi: 10.1080/00207454.2017.1278589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stulemeijer, M., Vos, P. E., Bleijenberg, G., & van der Werf, S. P. (2007). Cognitive complaints after mild traumatic brain injury: Things are not always what they seem. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63(6), 637645. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.06.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, K. A., Edmed, S. L., Greenslade, J. H., White, M., Chu, K., Lukin, B.Lurie, J. K. (2017). Psychological predictors of postconcussive symptoms following traumatic injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 33(4), 1. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000347.Google Scholar
Te Ao, B., Brown, P., Tobias, M., Ameratunga, S., Barker-Collo, S., Theadom, A.Feigin, V. L. (2014). Cost of traumatic brain injury in New Zealand: Evidence from a population-based study. Neurology, 83(18), 16451652. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theadom, A., Barker-Collo, S., Jones, K., Kahan, M., Te Ao, B., McPherson, K., … Te Ao, B. (2017). Work limitations 4 years after mild traumatic brain injury: A cohort study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 98(8), 15601566. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.01.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theadom, A., Starkey, N., Barker-Collo, S., Jones, K., Ameratunga, S., & Feigin, V. (2018). Population-based cohort study of the impacts of mild traumatic brain injury in adults four years post-injury. PLoS ONE, 13(1), 113. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veritas Health Innovation. (2019). Covidence systematic review software. Melbourne, Australia. Retrieved from www.covidence.org.Google Scholar
Veroniki, A. A., Jackson, D., Viechtbauer, W., Bender, R., Bowden, J., Knapp, G., Kuss, O.Salanti, G. (2016). Methods to estimate the between-study variance and its uncertainty in meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 7(1), 5579. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voormolen, D. C., Polinder, S., von Steinbuechel, N., Vos, P. E., Cnossen, M. C., & Haagsma, J. A. (2019). The association between post-concussion symptoms and health-related quality of life in patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Injury, 50(5), 10681074. doi: 10.1016/j.injury.2018.12.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vos, P. E., Battistin, L., Birbamer, G., Gerstenbrand, F., Potapov, A., Prevec, T.Von Wild, K. (2002). EFNS guideline on mild traumatic brain injury: Report of an EFNS task force. European Journal of Neurology, 9(3), 207219. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2002.00407.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wan, X., Wang, W., Liu, J., & Tong, T. (2014). Estimating the sample mean and standard deviation from the sample size, median, range and/or interquartile range. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 14(1), 113. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G. A., Shea, B., O’Connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., & Tugwell, P. (2000). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Retrieved from https://www.ohri.ca//programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.Asp.Google Scholar
Whittaker, R., Kemp, S., & House, A. (2007). Illness perceptions and outcome in mild head injury: A longitudinal study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 78(6), 644646. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.101105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L., François, R.Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43). doi: 10.21105/joss.01686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrightson, P., & Gronwall, D. (1981). Time off work and symptoms after minor head injury. Injury, 12(6), 445454. doi: 10.1016/0020-1383(81)90161-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yousefzadeh-Chabok, S., Kapourchali, F. R., & Ramezani, S. (2021). Determinants of long-term health-related quality of life in adult patients with mild traumatic brain injury. European Journal of Trauma and Emergency Surgery, 47(3), 839846. doi: 10.1007/s00068-019-01252-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeng, X., Zhang, Y., Kwong, J. S. W., Zhang, C., Li, S., Sun, F.Du, L. (2015). The methodological quality assessment tools for preclinical and clinical studies, systematic review and meta-analysis, and clinical practice guideline: A systematic review. Journal of Evidence-Based Medicine, 8(1), 210. doi: 10.1111/jebm.12141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for each study

Figure 1

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram. Modified from Moher et al. (2009).

Figure 2

Table 2. Star ratings on the modified NOS scale for each included study

Figure 3

Table 3. Characteristics of studies included in systematic review

Figure 4

Table 4. Definition of mTBI in studies included in systematic review

Figure 5

Table 5. Cognitive functions assessed by each scale

Figure 6

Figure 2. Results of the meta-analysis.

Supplementary material: File

Levy et al. supplementary material

Appendix

Download Levy et al. supplementary material(File)
File 121 KB