Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T14:13:14.523Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

POV: me, an empath, sensing the linguistic urge . . . to study the forms and functions of text-memes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 January 2024

Jacqueline Hirsh Greene*
Affiliation:
Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany
Hans–Jörg Schmid
Affiliation:
Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Jacqueline Hirsh Greene; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

A catapult into the world of social media, the title of our paper may be incoherent to some readers. What on earth does POV: me, an empath, sensing the linguistic urge ( . . . ) mean, and when and why would speakers say it? The title contains a jumble of three so-called text-memes – (1) POV, (2) me, an empath, VERB-ing X, and (3) the X urge to Y – all of which will be encountered in this paper. What we are calling ‘text-memes’ are memes that, in fairly neutral terms, have a purely textual form, as opposed to a pictorial one. Similar phenomena, which differ regarding their forms and functions, however, have been labelled ‘snowclones’ or ‘phrasal templates’ (cf. Pullum & Whitman, 2004; Know Your Meme, 2007–).

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

A catapult into the world of social media, the title of our paper may be incoherent to some readers. What on earth does POV: me, an empath, sensing the linguistic urge ( . . . ) mean, and when and why would speakers say it? The title contains a jumble of three so-called text-memes – (1) POV, (2) me, an empath, VERB-ing X, and (3) the X urge to Y – all of which will be encountered in this paper. What we are calling ‘text-memes’ are memes that, in fairly neutral terms, have a purely textual form, as opposed to a pictorial one. Similar phenomena, which differ regarding their forms and functions, however, have been labelled ‘snowclones’ or ‘phrasal templates’ (cf. Pullum & Whitman, Reference Pullum and Whitman2004; Know Your Meme, 2007–).

Text-memes, as we identify in the sections to come, are ubiquitous in certain corners of the internet, particularly on the algorithmic ‘feeds’ of millennial and generation-Z internet users. They seem to constitute some kind of linguistic register in their own right. A message that could be conveyed using other words is instead said in, or through, a text-meme. Let us consider a couple instances of text-memes posted on Twitter:

(1) me an empath avoiding people in emotional distress because im barely keeping it together myself

(az [@1111azzzz], 2022)

(2) me, an empath, noticing everyone at the funeral is crying and sad [GIF]

(pudding person [@JUNLPER], 2022)

In example 1, the speaker expresses what seems to be a genuine concern that attests to their heightened sense of empathy. This is in contrast to example 2, in which a genuine claim to be an empath seems to be subverted, the irony laying in how plain the observation is (that one does not need to be an empath to realize that crying and being sad are exactly what goes on at a funeral).

To our knowledge, the phenomenon of text-memes as recurrent utterance types in the form of distinct types of internet meme has not received due attention by linguists so far. In this paper we investigate text-memes with regard to their forms and functions. Our main questions are:

  1. a. What actually are text-memes and what are their formal and semantic characteristics?

  2. b. What are the main functions of text-memes? What might text-memes ‘do’, so-to-pragmatically-speak?

2. What are text-memes?

2.1 Background: Genetics, memetics, or semiotics?

The notion of the meme emerged as an attempt to draw an analogy between cultural studies and Darwinian evolutionary biology (Dawkins, Reference Dawkins1976). Richard Dawkins originally suggested that memes – i.e. trends, catchphrases, social norms, beliefs, and so on – are cultural material that transfer across generations and among populations (the comparison being that memes are encoded units of information which evolve and are societally transmitted by way of repetition and mimicry). The analogy, namely how it reduces the agency of the creators and spreaders of memes to subjects of automated transmission and evolution, has remained widely criticized by cultural anthropologists, biologists, and semioticians, among others (see Fomin, Reference Fomin2019: 331). Memes do not spread themselves, nor do ‘brains’ spread them indiscriminately, as memetics would suggest. Instead, socially-aware agents decide to create, compare, select, package and circulate them (cf. ‘hosting’ a meme ‘infra-psychically’, cf. Castelfranchi, Reference Castelfranchi2001: 21).

Taking consideration from all arenas with stake in the study of memes, Shifman's (Reference Shifman2014: 14) three-part criteria of internet memes, as a subject of cultural and media studies in their own right, are a starting point for understanding text-memes: memes are ‘(a) a group of digital items sharing common characteristics of content, form, and/or stance, which (b) were created with awareness of each other, and (c) were circulated, imitated, and/or transformed via the Internet by many users.’ Of course, when we talk specifically about internet memes, which the field of memetics precedes, the process of this exact diffusion is reinforced and amplified by the affordances of social media (Schmid, Reference Schmid2020: 181, 314).

Literature in the field of linguistics seems to focus on the ‘image macro’ type of meme. Image macros are composed of viral images that are de- or re-contextualized by help of superimposed or adjacent linguistic elements (Bülow, Reference Bülow, Bülow, Koch, Krieg–Holz and Trost2023: 260). They have been studied in relation to their form-meaning correspondence, as pragmatic significations, and as realizations of collective conventions entrenched in (some) speakers’ minds (cf. Shifman, Reference Shifman2014; Bülow, Merten & Johann, Reference Bülow, Merten and Johann2018; Zenner & Geeraerts, Reference Zenner, Geeraerts, Winter–Froemel and Thaler2018; Denisova, Reference Denisova2019; Wiggins, Reference Wiggins2019; Zappavigna, Reference Zappavigna, Stöckl, Caple and Pflaeging2020; Bülow, Reference Bülow, Bülow, Koch, Krieg–Holz and Trost2023). The way in which image macros package a stable image with variable or semi-variable text (e.g. one does not simply X, Bülow et al., Reference Bülow, Merten and Johann2018: 21) is similar to the packaging of a stable text with a variable or semi-variable text in the case of text-memes (e.g. me, an empath, VERB-ing X). The variable text portion (e.g. avoiding people in emotional distress because I'm barely keeping it together myself or noticing everyone at the funeral is crying and sad in examples 1 and 2) is referred to here as the co-text. ‘Co-text’ in this case draws from Schmid's (Reference Schmid2020: 19) notion, as the “utterances preceding and following a given utterance or part thereof.”

The closest formal description of text-memes has been elsewhere assigned the term ‘snowclones’ or ‘phrasal templates’ (cf. Pullum & Whitman, Reference Pullum and Whitman2004; Know Your Meme, 2007–). So-called snowclones would be template-like phrases such as ‘X is the new Y’ or ‘to X or not to X’, for example. The term ‘phrasal template’ is also quite general; it could refer to really any phrase which includes variable slots, such as those above, or anything such as ‘X(adj) as a Y(animal)’ as in ‘blind as a bat’ or ‘stubborn as a mule’, which we argue are not the same kind of linguistic patterns or phrasal forms as text-memes. ‘Phrasal templates’ as well as ‘snowclones’ differ from text-memes with regard to both forms and functions, which is noticeable in how typically simpler, less abundant in text, less narrative-driven, less obscure, and more constrained snowclones and phrasal templates are in comparison to text-memes. In addition, text-memes are products of social media creation and spread. The original purpose and connotation attributed to ‘snowclones’ are also at odds with our notion of text-memes. In an entry to Language Log, Pullum and Whitman (Reference Pullum and Whitman2004) coined the term as a way of describing ‘cliché frames for lazy journalists’. Whereas snowclone emphasizes the imitative, ‘cloned’ property of meaning-making, text-meme emphasizes the creative, ‘text’ property of meaning-making.

2.2 Definition and form: Meme + co-text = text-meme package

Text-memes can be characterized as bipartite units, consisting of a somewhat fixed and a somewhat variable part (see Figure 1), despite that meme-users may likely grasp the entire construction holistically. Otherwise, i.e. if they are interpreted compositionally, it is unlikely that language users are able to recognize them as having a ‘special significance’ such as in their pragmatics or idiomatic-like expression. In the text-meme me, an empath, VERB-ing X, we would identify the fixed meme portion me an empath and the co-text VERB-ing X.

Figure 1. Visualization of the bipartite text-meme as ‘meme’ plus ‘co-text’

Memes and their co-text are complementary. Co-texts are the core of a speaker's communicative point in terms of propositional content. Memes in turn make a strong contribution to perspectivizing this content, e.g. by obscuring, enriching or questioning it and, most importantly, by placing it in the interpersonal relationship between writer/speaker and reader/hearer. Memes cushion the co-text, which also means that certain co-texts call for certain memes.

Co-texts are mostly variable, in that they can take on the topic of really anything. But in order for text-memes to cohere and have a maximal effect and reception as text-memes, co-texts must stick to formal and semantic conventions imposed by the meme. For example, it would be unconventional if the aforementioned text-memes were to instead have the following co-texts:

(3) [Me, an empath, a person who avoids other people's emotional distress considering I am barely keeping it together myself.]

(4) [me, an empath, attending a funeral where everyone is crying and sad.]

These two fabricated reconstructions of examples 1 and 2 illustrate how co-texts are only variable to an extent. Example 3 deviates from the convention imposed by the meme, in that me, an empath is followed by a relative clause modifying me. One would usually expect the meme to be followed by a verb phrase where the head verb takes the progressive ing-form. A particular semantic type of verb is also likely expected, typically either a psych verb such as sensing, seeing, or feeling, or at least some verb related to a scenario of having feelings akin to empathy (see avoiding in example 1). Although formally typical, example 4 is unconventional regarding the semantics of attending. There is no direct or obvious relation between (attending an event) → (being an empath), but maybe only when (attending a particular event) + (perceiving it in a particular way) → (being an empath). We could however hypothesize that speakers give less effort to ticking certain grammatical and even semantic boxes of the co-text than they give to reporting a meme somewhat uniformly. Again this expectation is based on the assumption that some degree of holistic processing is required for successful understanding of text-memes.

The meme portion itself is formally constrained as well, but also flexible to some degree. Speakers have leeway in how they report the meme portion. As is also the case with co-texts, limitations to this creativity are imposed by recognizability. After all, memes have to be repeated uniformly enough to be recognized as a meme in the first place. One often finds ambiguous meme usage though:

(5) As an empath, when somebody passes away it just makes me SO sad. Especially seeing their loved ones going through it. Shit sucks

(Aja Lou [@XO_Tamar], 2022)

(6) The empath in me has had a really really REALLY tough week. To the point of physical exhaustion.

(Brittaney Shanae [@bshanae_mua], 2022)

In examples 5 and 6, speakers identify themselves as empaths given some explanation, e.g., feeling upset by death, or having a particularly difficult week. One question here may be if other meme users would analyze these examples, despite variation in the stable meme portion, as evoking the me, an empath VERB-ing X text-meme. It could also be that empath is a current buzzword, and the phenomenon of calling oneself an empath is on-trend, apart from the text-meme construction. Such examples might be considered highly modified allusions to the meme, which rest on the key notion of empath and references made to the speaker. As an empath, when X, then Y (example 5) and the empath in me VERBs (example 6) deviate quite a bit formally from me, an empath, VERB-ing X. The question lies in the flexibility of readers to accept the meme in various altered states: e.g., if omitting commas, as in me an empath VERB-ing, is OK, and other changes in the noun phrase, as in me, as the empath I am, VERB-ing, are also OK, but I am an empath because X is not OK?

The issue of variability is probably quite functionalized, however, meaning that deviations from the norm would be recognized and more likely to be found acceptable by the ‘meme-literate,’ but not necessarily by the meme-non-literate (Procházka, Reference Procházka2014; Merten, Bülow & Johann, Reference Merten, Bülow and Johannforthcoming). We can therefore perhaps settle on understanding text-memes as formally ambiguous; as forms that must nod to both to variability and stability. Yet to deviate from the norm and still be recognized as a text-meme requires that the particular text-meme has gained enough traction as a conventionalized ‘utterance type’ (Schmid, Reference Schmid2020: 19–28). Speakers have to know the formal, semantic, and pragmatic conventions of co-texts and memes, which are established and reinforced by usage and familiarity with that particular text-meme. And in order to become ‘proficient’ in text-meme usage, speakers must internalize specific formal and contextual characteristics of not just that one instance of a text-meme, but of an entire cache of other ones.

3. Functions

Coherent systems of functions in language have been proposed, for example, by Bühler (Reference Bühler1934), Jakobson (Reference Jakobson and Sebeok1960) or Halliday (Reference Halliday1994) (see Schmid, Reference Schmid2020: 18 for a survey). Instead of sticking to one of these, we will discuss functions of text-memes in an eclectic but tailor-made way. As the paper is programmatic and opens up a new field of inquiry, we focus on three different hand-picked text-meme types, having considered approximately 100 tokens per type extracted from the Twitter spotlight-search function. We are aware that text-memes are also ubiquitous on other social-media platforms, but will restrict our attention to Twitter in view of space limitations. For this reason, this paper only includes a small number of examples which are useful for illustrating the functional patterns highlighted below. Based on this selection, we put forward a division into six dominant configurations of sociopragmatic functions, in the vague sense of ‘motivations for using text-memes and intended effects’ (Table 1):

Table 1. Functions of text-memes

These six functions fall into two groups. The first group applies to memes and text-memes, which are both: phatic; indexical to the speaker, yet both addressee- and addresser-centered; referential to other memes, the genre, and to other pop-culture references; and social codes.

The remaining two functions, i.e. the narrative and therapeutic ones, seem to be more specific to text-memes. Text-memes often, though not always, exhibit the narrative function, characteristic of their predominant feature, text. ‘Text’ is not meant in the semiotic sense, where images also qualify as text or textual artifacts, but in a more narrow sense of the term, as a ‘verbal record of a communicative act’ (Brown & Yule, Reference Brown and Yule1983: 6, our emphasis). Text under these terms does not only mean that there is a communicative act in some recorded form (Brown & Yule, Reference Brown and Yule1983: 6); in addition, the notion of ‘text’ entails an element of coherence, or felt-coherence at the least. The narrative function explains how speakers tell coherent stories and self-mythologize, which is a more extreme and overt form of the kind of self-indexation present in all memes. The therapeutic function ties into the narrative function, in how speakers create distance from their message put forth in the co-text by using a meme as a hedge. The narrative and therapeutic functions elucidate how text-memes and image macros differ regarding their communicative capabilities or goals. Text-memes are intended to be easy to consume to those who get them, as are image macros, but key to these functions and to text itself, text-memes are generally more capable of conveying complex, content-driven messages.

3.1 Phatic, indexical/interpersonal, intertextual/poetic, and social functions

Initially our interest in memes and their communicative functions was sparked by the suggestion that memes mainly, or even only, communicate phatically, among an internet with increasingly ‘empty’ media (cf. Miller, Reference Miller2008; Varis & Blommaert, Reference Varis and Blommaert2015). Jakobson's (Reference Jakobson and Sebeok1960) phatic function describes speech as serving the purpose to ‘establish, to prolong, or to discontinue communication’ (Jakobson, Reference Jakobson and Sebeok1960: 355), i.e. it is language for the sake of contact. But if the point of posting online is most simply to make contact, then any post on social media, memes included, have an inherent phatic function.

When considering the examples 7 and 8 below which use the POV text-meme, we can see why a ‘merely phatic’ conclusion might be tempting. The POV text-meme is meant to provide a ‘point of view’, inserting the reader into the speaker's narrative about an event. The speaker rhetorically asks of the reader to consider that this propositional information is their own point of view; the text-meme as such originated as one that necessitated a video or an image that served as the POV. Examples 7 and 8 are however two extremely cryptic tokens of the POV text-meme. The hasty formulation in 7 suggests the speaker might have been drunkenly stating they are drunk. Example 8 is even more cryptic: what exactly is the point of view, with a co-text of just peepee poopoo? We can only speculate as to its intended meaning. Yet, even examples 7 and 8 go beyond the phatic function.

(7) Pov drunk

(Amber [@gentlearthquake], 2022)

(8) pov: peepee poopoo

(dead account. [@MarianneFESimp], 2021)

These examples still exhibit the indexical function, despite being not nearly as overt as other text-meme tokens in their indexicality, nor in what Jakobson called the emotive function of communication, which is language for the sake of speaker-centered expression. These text-meme tokens hardly reveal anything about the speaker or the context, and yet, the speakers index themselves as meme-users. This in itself reveals a particular membership belonging of the speaker. Zenner and Geeraerts (Reference Zenner, Geeraerts, Winter–Froemel and Thaler2018: 173) similarly describe image macro usage as marking an ‘ingroup code of the digitally literate’ (see also Bülow, Reference Bülow, Bülow, Koch, Krieg–Holz and Trost2023: 261).

Producing and sharing a meme is a highly performative act, even if practically a reflex. On the flipside of the indexical function is the interpersonal function, related to what Jakobson referred to as the conative function of communication, which is language for the sake of appeal. Memes constantly rely on an interface between addressers and addressees. They are self-referential and ultimately only meaningful by way of reception. That is, if reception is possible: the addressee has to be just as much in the know as the addresser. And if so, memes are then identified as intertextual references over word-for-word utterances, i.e., they are meant to be read as holistic chunks. For a meme to be considered a meme, prior and current usages are intertextually linked. Memes network with other internet material, sources of pop culture, and most importantly, with other memes (Laineste & Voolaid, Reference Laineste and Voolaid2017; Zenner & Geeraerts, Reference Zenner, Geeraerts, Winter–Froemel and Thaler2018; Wiggins, Reference Wiggins2019). This intertextual function is not at all specific to memes but rather to genres in general (De Beaugrande & Dressler, Reference De Beaugrande and Dressler1981). Example 9 from Twitter demonstrates the intertextual function by exaggeration:

(9) Me (an empath) sensing that the feminine urge that my toxic trait is that we are not the same me n who??

(A [@gaywarlord], 2021)

This example of the me, an empath VERB-ing X text-meme is composed entirely of other memes as the co-text: the X urge to Y; my toxic trait is X; you/’re X, I/’m Y, we are not the same; and X: me and who? The speaker jumbles together these popular text-memes to make one seemingly nonsensical chain of text-memes (perhaps criticizing the overuse of the text-meme types). If these intertextual references to the text-memes were unknown, then the indexical and interpersonal point of example 9 would be missed. The meaning of example 9 also lays in the poetic function. Using Jakobson's terminology, the poetic function describes how the form of the message reflects ‘on itself.’ That is, when the message design makes the meaning, and when the form of the message is the point - apart from the meaning attached to the forms. Speakers do not only refer to the entities they talk about, but inevitably to the text-memes themselves (they are self-referential).

The notion of ‘message makes the meaning’ could also be explained in terms of the social function. This last function that we attribute to all meme types might be able to answer to a few special qualities of text-memes, such as their prolificity and tendency to change. Once memes are mimicked enough, they compete with other, newer, possibly even better memes. At that point, for a meme to survive rather than die, there is the potential to remix into something alike, but different (cf. ‘memetic drive’ in Blackmore, Reference Blackmore2000; or ‘intermemetic referentiality’ in Esteves & Meikle, Reference Esteves, Meikle and Atton2015; Wiggins, Reference Wiggins2019). In the case of text-memes, it is common for either the form of the meme or the conventions of the co-text to easily and quickly change. For instance, a well-established text-meme, the feminine urge to X, has been significantly revamped over time. On its so-called ‘adaptability’, the text-meme even got a feature in the New York Times in an article titled ‘The Timesian Urge to Explain a Meme (Kambhampaty, Reference Kambhampaty2021). Know Your Meme, a vast and up-to-date encyclopedic web source on meme tracking, estimates the first instance of what became a text-meme to have been the following Tumblr post (Know Your Meme, 2007-):

(10) the feminine urge to stab <3

([coffeeheaux], 2021)

In early instances of the text-meme, the co-text would usually pair a transitive verb with no subsequent object. Semantically, the transitive verb would have little or nothing to do with what one associates with femininity, or ‘feminine urges’ nonetheless (e.g. stabbing). When in full throttle, there likely comes a split in imaginations of a fitting co-text of a text-meme type, and more variation in form too. Some interpret and use the text-meme ironically (example 11), and some more so at its face value (example 12). The feminine urge to X eventually allowed for an open slot to all kinds of urges, the modified construction being the X urge to Y (examples 13 and 14).

(11) the feminine urge to get a lobotomy

(Maria Is Online [@averyonlinegirl], 2021)

(12) the feminine urge to understand him in the deepest way possible

(Angélica García [@angelicaswild], 2021)

(13) The nonbinary urge to crush the cockroach

(Lin Biao School of Aeronautics [@Pondershevik], 2021)

(14) The Marxist urge to die

(Astrologically Oppressed [@PD8800], 2021)

Inevitably, some meme-users would deem even the revamped constructions ‘dead memes’, despite whether some still use it (‘Dead meme: A meme that has become irrelevant or unfunny, often due to age or overly cringeworthy use’ [Urban Dictionary, 2017). The following speakers took to Twitter their meta-level complaints about the text-meme:

(15) Oh lord now politicians and brands making “urge” tweets. It is dead. Welp, time for the next trend lmao

(trudy [@thetrudz], 2021)

(16) The male urge to beat a meme until it's completely unfunny and dead

(B [@Bguth__], 2021)

(17) I don't CARE if the meme is dead I have the urge so I will EXPRESS it dammit

(Jessica [@greyharuka], 2022)

Divergent opinions on the repute of a meme seems unavoidable. Memes are like codes, in that they must balance being encoded enough (i.e. they have to be obscure enough, for only the in-group to get them), yet not too decoded (i.e. they should not be too obscure, in order for the in-group to get them). Text-memes necessitating obscurity may seem counterintuitive, if the argument is that text-memes are communicative utterances above all else. Why should text be obscure if functioning predominantly to communicate? A certain level of obscurity is critical for memes to ‘stick’. This is also a criterion that differentiates text-memes from snowclones or phrasal templates. For as much as memes function to be widely used and ‘mimicked,’ they also function to die, so that newer and cooler memes can be born. Relevance theory (Sperber & Wilson, Reference Sperber and Wilson1996) could give some kind of explanation for text-memes’ quick, successful and curious rise and fall as communicative forms among some internet users. In this approach, the notion of relevance is understood as being a trade-off between the positive cognitive effects to be gained from processing an utterance and the energy required for doing so. Memes are not superficially ‘relevant’, by means of that terminology, but are rather only relevant on a deeper level, like in their obscurity and humor. The process of working out meme's relevance is rewarding, in that the reader can rejoice once having worked it out, gotten the point of the intertextual references, and can be satisfied with being in the know. As soon as a meme is overused, there is no longer a reward in store. This is also a property typical of expressions that leave a gap between what is said and what is meant (cf. ‘euphemism treadmill’, Keller, Reference Keller1994; Pinker, Reference Pinker2002).

3.2 Narrative and therapeutic functions

An apt introduction to the distinct functionality of text-memes is to consider them stories, and speakers storytellers. It has been suggested that memes at large are analogous to storytelling and lore in how they retell a story repeatedly over time, spun by the speaker and their context (Esteves & Meikle, Reference Esteves, Meikle and Atton2015). This analogy remarks more on how memes as a genre work. What we suggest here is that text-memes in particular often a particular narrative function. Narrative, most simply put, is the record of someone telling someone else that something that happened, (for a pragmatic purpose). And, as is typical of narratives, there is always some kind of complication (Labov & Waletzky, Reference Labov, Waletzky and Helm1967).

Each instance of a text-meme entails two indexical jobs related to narrative concerns: speakers index themselves in relation to some piece of propositional content (expressed in their co-text), and speakers index themselves as meme-users, to express the former. In this way, text-memes can serve as self-mythologies, in the sense that the speaker – the narrator – recounts on an event, real or not, pinning themselves as the main character in and against the world. These self-mythologies tend to represent the complication component around which the structure of narratives revolves. Maybe for this reason, an initial impression of text-memes is that they come across self-centered. This is a legitimate impression, because text-memes, even when the subject is not the speaker, are often collections of personal anecdotes and morals related to the experiences of speakers (remember examples 1 and 2, where speakers attempt to set the record straight that they are ‘empaths’). And very often, the topics of such text-memes are overshares or taboos:

(18) pov both ur cats died within 5 days :) i'm actually very mentally stable rn i'm not gonna kms at all

(osh [@oshiewott], 2022)

(19) pov ur breaking up with me but glee is on and i'm silently humming the songs to myself

(tiana [@gagamilkies], 2022

Examples 18 and 19 from Twitter demonstrate what is meant by text-memes as mini-narratives: they set up a scene and describe an event representing the complication. In 18, the literal narrative is how the speaker reflects on the death of their pet cats and sarcastically attests they are currently ‘very stable’ despite also contemplating death; in 19 the speaker narrates humming while being broken up with. Beneath the hypothetic and hyperbolic tone common of POV text-memes, 18 and 19 likely reveal actual sentiments felt by the speakers: feeling devasted, or, being indifferent to something usually considered devastating. We mentioned that all meme usage indexes speakers as meme-users, but the narrative quality of text-memes takes self-indexation one step further. The therapeutic function might explain the question as to why this is. Why tell the story, and why use a meme to tell it?

We suggest that text-memes very often exhibit a therapeutic function, in which speakers use them to cope or engage with a wide-range of emotions:

(20) Pov: you feel alone even though you live with seven billion people

(Idkbincool [@Idkbincool13], 2021)

(21) pov ur 9 years old droppin it low to london bridge by fergie at the roller rink. ur mom gave u $20 for arcade games n bad hot dogs. all of ur friends are here. they're about to play dead bug. all is well.

(queef urban [@creature_cal], 2022)

The POV affords speakers a particularly easy, indirect outlet to speak from the ‘you’ rather than the ‘I’ perspective. It is a fixed formula to out oneself, own up to or open up about, for instance, feeling alone or basking in childhood nostalgia.

Global, collectively-felt events are also a favorite topic among text-memes, which play into the therapeutic function. A study on 9/11 and Iraq war pictorial memes or image macros, such as the Mission Accomplished motif (Silvestri, Reference Silvestri2018), brought to light how memes can contribute to past or current social and political events by synthesizing them and raising awareness for them, and by doing so, take on immense responsibility for how online communities come to view the events (cf. Crovitz & Moran, Reference Crovitz and Moran2020). How on one hand, it is hopeful that memes are segues to activism, and on the other, there is concern that memes offer a lightheartedness and humor which can make it a challenge to discern serious matters from the silly or simplified messages typical of image macros. It would be interesting to see how text-memes compare in this sense; for instance on the topic of the SCOTUS ruling to overturn Roe V. Wade:

(22) POV: roe v wade is overturned you live in Texas and just found out you're caring an ectopic pregnancy and you can not terminate pregnancy because people think a fetus is more important than your own life.

(Geraldine [@_geraldine_84], 2022)

(23) Pov: its 2023. Abortion is illegal, condoms illegal and birth control is illegal . . .. I bet homosexuality dont sound so bad now do it?

(KNTY [@Mrkezzz], 2022)

Inherent to the more expansive, textual form of text-memes and the difference in their communicative abilities, we could at least initially suggest that compared to political image macros, text-memes could by nature allow for easier, more thoughtful argumentation and activism. Example 22 demonstrates this ability of text-memes well. Example 23, quite crudely, reminds us that text-memes are still memes, and that the extra creative license of text-memes is somewhat of a double-edged sword, just like with image macros. Meme-users can speak out against injustice just as much they can make light of it. Humor is of course an important feature in memes as entertainment devices, also as coping mechanisms for the speakers themselves, as might be the case in the examples above. To Silvestri's (Reference Silvestri2018) point, meme users indeed become stakeholders in important geo-political affairs. While this notion seems promising on some fronts, for instance in countering political apathy among young people, the boldness and comedic delivery which has potential to be insensitive is an issue of concern when it comes to serious, unfunny issues. In their efforts to act or cope with current events, it is the individual meme-user's responsibility to decide how they set the tone.

4. Conclusion

What we have done so far is preliminarily sort through the most pressing criteria of our subject as a distinct, novel linguistic phenomenon. We introduced text-memes as types of internet memes. We have decided for a bipartite approach to the form of text-memes, despite that they are likely interpreted wholistically and dynamically. As illustrated by the questionable reconstructions in examples 3 through 6, memes are not entirely fixed, and co-text is not entirely variable. Constructing both requires a calculated, though probably implicit, pairing of formal and semantic expectations. Lastly on form, we emphasized that text-memes are text-driven, an emphasis that profiles how they function as complex communicative utterances.

Our focus was mainly on functionality. We suggested a total of six functions that can account for how speakers ‘do stuff,’ knowingly and unknowingly, with text-memes. Four of the six functions are more or less applicable functions among all kinds of memes. We began with the phatic function. Text-memes can be plain silly or even seemingly nonsensical. Nonetheless, we maintain that text-memes are never merely phatic, but rather always phatic. Regardless of how expressive and speaker-centered the content of a meme is, text-memes first and foremost index speakers as meme-users, which is at the core of the indexical function.

All memes function to appeal as well: memes are made to be received. Central to the interpersonal function is the medium, that memes are meant (and need) to be public, widely shared phenomena. Memes also possess self-referentiality, which we discussed in the context of the intertextual and poetic functions. Intertextuality is the basis for why memes are memes from the start. Through their mimicry, memes are in constant reference to a network of other memes. The meaning of memes and text-memes is always at least partly, or even entirely, made by the message itself (recall example 9), hence their poetic function.

The social function highlights the role of memes as codes, whose obscurity facilitates their social importance. Memes are only codes as long as they leave a large enough residuum of society to stand outside ‘the know’.

The other two functions we discussed are more specific to how text-memes often pattern, and may be what distinguish them as their own type. The narrative function explains how speakers position themselves in and against the world. The content of these self-mythologies can handle everything from nonsense to candid, unfiltered thoughts, overshares, or social taboos, all of which can be hedged by a meme. The therapeutic function considers how and why text-memes would be useful in hedging this kind of content. Text-memes afford speakers distance between themselves and their claims – a space that allows for the chance to cope or engage with their audience and context. Both narrative and therapeutic functions emphasize the expressive, indexical, and personalized quality of text-memes, especially when compared to image macros.

This paper sought to open up a conversation on the text-meme type of meme as formally and functionally meaningful. Understanding the lifespans of text-memes, which might very well be shorter-lived than other memes such as image macros, requires a look into a confluence of socio-cognitive elements that account for new and changed language. Having a closer and more detailed look at these elements has to be the topic of another study.

JACQUELINE HIRSH GREENE is an MA student of English Linguistics at Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich and of ‘Mind and Brain’ at the Humboldt University in Berlin. Greene obtained a bachelor's degree in 2019 in Linguistics and German as a double major at the University of New Mexico. Her research interests generally lie in the fields of semantics, pragmatics, psycholinguistics, and in internet language and memes. Email:

HANS–JÖRG SCHMID is Full Professor and Chair of Modern English Linguistics at LMU Munich, Germany. His research has been devoted to a wide range of fields in linguistics including linguistic theory, cognitive linguistics, lexical semantics, syntax, word-formation, pragmatics, sociolinguistics and language change. In his recent book, entitled The Dynamics of the Linguistic System. Usage, Conventionalization and Entrenchment (2020, OUP), he develops a unified model of how language works, integrating cognitive, social and pragmatic aspects, to explain linguistic structure, variation and change. Email:

References

A [@gaywarlord]. 2021, April 5. Me (an empath) sensing that the feminine urge that my toxic trait is that we are not the same me n who?? [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/gaywarlord> [Tweets from this user are protected].+[Tweets+from+this+user+are+protected].>Google Scholar
Aja Lou [@XO_Tamar]. 2022, June 28. As an empath, when somebody passes away it just makes me SO sad. Especially seeing their loved ones going through it. Shit sucks ☺ [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/XO_Tamar/status/1541798722509602816>..>Google Scholar
Amber [@gentlearthquake]. 2022, May 21. Pov drunk [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/gentlearthquake/status/1528126200660516873>..>Google Scholar
Angélica García [@angelicaswild]. 2021, November 21. the feminine urge to take care of him and understand him in the deepest way possible [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/angelicaswild/status/1462511704856662017>..>Google Scholar
Astrologically Oppressed [@PD8800]. 2021, November 20. The Marxist urge to die [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/PD8800/status/1461874506327216130>..>Google Scholar
az [@1111azzzz]. 2022, March 15. me an empath avoiding people in emotional distress because im barely keeping it together myself [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/1111azzzz/status/1503864358979448839>..>Google Scholar
Blackmore, S. 2000. The Meme Machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brittaney Shanae [@bshanae_mua]. 2022, June 24. The empath in me has had a really really REALLY tough week. To the point of physical exhaustion. [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/bshanae_mua/status/1540406889145683970>..>Google Scholar
Brown, G. & Yule, G. 1983. Discourse Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bühler, K. 1934. Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache. Jena: G. Fischer.Google Scholar
Bülow, L. 2023. ‘Image macros als ressource für sprachliche verstärkungsprozess.’ In Bülow, L., Koch, G., Krieg–Holz, U. & Trost, I. (eds.), Remotivierung in der Sprache. Auf der Suche nach Form und Bedeutung. Berlin: Springer, 257280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bülow, L., Merten, M. L. & Johann, M. 2018. ‘Internet-memes als zugang zu multimodalen konstruktionen.’ Zeitschrift für Angewandte Linguistik, 69, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castelfranchi, C. 2001. ‘Towards a cognitive memetics: Socio-cognitive mechanisms for memes selection and spreading.’ Journal of Memetics, 5, 120.Google Scholar
[coffeeheaux]. 2021, April 14. the feminine urge to stab <3 [Tumblr Post]. Tumblr. <https://waltenfiles.tumblr.com/post/654739431049297920>..>Google Scholar
Crovitz, D. & Moran, C. 2020. ‘Analyzing disruptive memes in an age of international interference.’ English Journal, 109(4), 6269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. 1976. The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
dead account. [@MarianneFESimp]. 2021, March 10. pov: peepee poopoo [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/MarianneFESimp/status/1369733553815236614>..>Google Scholar
De Beaugrande, R. & Dressler, W. 1981. Introduction to Text Linguistics. London & New York: Longman.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denisova, A. 2019. Internet Memes and Society. Social, Cultural, and Political Contexts. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esteves, V. & Meikle, G. 2015. ‘“Look @ this fukken doge”: Internet memes and remix cultures.’ In Atton, C. (ed.), The Routledge Companion to Alternative and Community Media. New York: Routledge, 561570.Google Scholar
Fomin, I. 2019. ‘Memes, genes, and signs: Semiotics in the conceptual interface of evolutionary biology and memetics.’ Semiotica, 230, 327340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geraldine [@_geraldine_84]. 2022, June 26. POV: roe v wade is overturned you live in Texas and just found out you're caring an ectopic pregnancy and you can not terminate pregnancy because people think a fetus is more important than your own life [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/_geraldine_84/status/1540927119361880064>..' href=https://scholar.google.com/scholar?q=Geraldine+[@_geraldine_84].+2022,+June+26.+POV:+roe+v+wade+is+overturned+you+live+in+Texas+and+just+found+out+you're+caring+an+ectopic+pregnancy+and+you+can+not+terminate+pregnancy+because+people+think+a+fetus+is+more+important+than+your+own+life+[Tweet].+Twitter.+.>Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. 1994. An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: E. Arnold.Google Scholar
Idkbincool [@Idkbincool13]. 2021, August 14. Pov: you feel alone even though you live with seven billion people [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/Idkbincool13/status/1426356733065277443>..>Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. 1960. ‘Closing statement: Linguistics and poetics.’ In Sebeok, T. A. (ed.), Style in Language. Cambridge: MIT Press, 350377.Google Scholar
Kambhampaty, A.P. 2021. ‘The Timesian urge to explain a meme.’ New York Times, December, 6.Google Scholar
Keller, R. 1994. Sprachwandel. Von der Unsichtbaren Hand in der Sprache. Tübingen: Francke.Google Scholar
Know Your Meme. 2007–. Online at <https://www.knowyourmeme.com> (Accessed December 13, 2022).+(Accessed+December+13,+2022).>Google Scholar
KNTY [@Mrkezzz]. 2022, May 9. Pov: its 2023. Abortion is illegal, condoms illegal and birth control is illegal . . . . I bet homosexuality dont sound so bad now do it? ☺ [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/Mrkezzz/status/1523652232532881409>..>Google Scholar
Labov, W. & Waletzky, J. 1967. ‘Narrative analysis.’ In Helm, J. (ed.), Essays on the Verbal and Visual Arts. Seattle: University of Washington Press, 1244.Google Scholar
Laineste, L. & Voolaid, P. 2017. ‘Laughing across borders: Intertextuality of internet memes.’ European Journal of Humour Research, 4(4), 2649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin Biao School of Aeronautics [@Pondershevik]. 2021, November 15. The nonbinary urge to crush the cockroach [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/Pondershevik/status/1460134425757458432>..>Google Scholar
Maria Is Online [@averyonlinegirl]. 2021, October 26. the feminine urge to get a lobotomy [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/averyonlinegirl/status/1452793199001493504>..>Google Scholar
Merten, M. L., Bülow, L. & Johann, M. Forthcoming. ‘Image macros as multimodal patterns: Image-language relations and norm-related adaption.’ Pattern Theory in Language and Communication.Google Scholar
Miller, V. 2008. ‘New media, networking and phatic culture.’ Convergence, 14, 387400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. 2002. The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Procházka, O. 2014. ‘Internet memes – A new literacy?Ostrava Journal of English Philology, 6(1), 5374.Google Scholar
pudding person [@JUNLPER]. 2022, January 3. me, an empath, noticing everyone at the funeral is crying and sad [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/JUNlPER/status/1477777123448344580>..>Google Scholar
Pullum, G. K. & Whitman, G. 2004. ‘Snowclones: Lexicographical dating to the second.’ Online at <http://itre.cis.upenn.edu/~myl/languagelog/archives/000350.html> (Accessed December 14, 2022)+(Accessed+December+14,+2022)>Google Scholar
queef urban [@creature_cal]. 2022, February 15. pov ur 9 years old droppin it low to london bridge by fergie at the roller rink. ur mom gave u $20 for arcade games n bad hot dogs. all of ur friends are here. they're about to play dead bug. all is well [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/creature_cal/status/1493641371252662283>' href=https://scholar.google.com/scholar?q=queef+urban+[@creature_cal].+2022,+February+15.+pov+ur+9+years+old+droppin+it+low+to+london+bridge+by+fergie+at+the+roller+rink.+ur+mom+gave+u+$20+for+arcade+games+n+bad+hot+dogs.+all+of+ur+friends+are+here.+they're+about+to+play+dead+bug.+all+is+well+[Tweet].+Twitter.+>Google Scholar
Schmid, H. J. 2020. The Dynamics of the Linguistic System. Usage, Conventionalization, and Entrenchment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shifman, L. 2014. Memes in Digital Culture. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Silvestri, L. E. 2018. ‘Memeingful memories and the art of resistance.’ New Media & Society, 20(11), 39974016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. 1996. Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford and Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
trudy [@thetrudz]. 2021, December 1. Oh lord now politicians and brands making “urge” tweets. It is dead. Welp, time for the next trend lmao [Tweet]. Twitter. <https://twitter.com/thetrudz/status/1465833393522987012>..>Google Scholar
Urban Dictionary. 2017. Dead meme. Retrieved from <https://www.urbandictionary.com/define.php?term=Dead%20Meme> (Accessed Mar. 5, 2022).+(Accessed+Mar.+5,+2022).>Google Scholar
Varis, P. & Blommaert, J. 2015. ‘Conviviality and collectives on social media: Virality, memes, and new social structures.’ Multilingual Margins, 2(1), 3145.Google Scholar
Wiggins, B. E. 2019. The Discursive Power of Memes in Digital Culture Ideology, Semiotics, and Intertextuality. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zappavigna, Michele. 2020. ‘“And then he said . . . No one has more respect for women than I do”: Intermodal relations and intersubjectivity in image macros.’ In Stöckl, H., Caple, H. & Pflaeging, J. (eds.), Shifts toward Image-Centricity in Contemporary Multimodal Practices. New York and London: Routledge, 204225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zenner, E & Geeraerts, D. 2018. ‘One does not simply process memes: Image macros as multimodal constructions.’ In Winter–Froemel, E. & Thaler, V. (eds.), Cultures and Traditions of Wordplay and Wordplay Research. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter, 167194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Visualization of the bipartite text-meme as ‘meme’ plus ‘co-text’

Figure 1

Table 1. Functions of text-memes