Article contents
Culture and Decision Making in China, Japan, Russia, and the United States
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 13 June 2011
Abstract
Implicit in most recent social science explanations of human behavior is a conception of man as universal homo economicus. Although such a conception is capable of giving a powerful account of a great deal of human action, its account of the nature and variety of human values is inadequate. Cultural assumptions about the meaning of “self” and “others,” and about relations between human beings, are likely to vary from one society to another. These assumptions affect the collective decision processes of political elites under conditions of complexity, uncertainty, and ambiguity. The author first addresses the question of how to construct a compelling cultural explanation, and then offers evidence which suggests that, because Chinese, Japanese, and Russians tend to hold somewhat different conceptions of “self” and “others” than do Americans (the former tending to be more collectivist than the latter), these different conceptions have implications for collective decision making.
- Type
- Research Article
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Trustees of Princeton University 1986
References
1 The metaphor is borrowed from Geertz, Clifford, The Interpretation of Cultures (New York: Basic Books, 1973) 5Google Scholar.
2 See, for example, Almond, Gabriel A. and Verba, Sidney, The Civic Culture (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1963)CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Pye, Lucian and Verba, Sidney, eds., Political Culture and Political Development (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1965)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
3 See especially Elkins, David J. and Simeon, Richard E. B., “A Cause in Search of Its Effect, or What Does Political Culture Explain?” Comparative Politics 11 (January 1979), 127–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar; also Dittmer, Lowell, “Political Culture and Political Symbolism: Toward a Theoretical Synthesis,” World Politics 29 (July 1977), 552–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
4 DiFranceisco, Wayne and Gitelman, Zvi, “Soviet Political Culture and ‘Covert Participation' in Policy Implementation,” American Political Science Review 78 (September 1984), 603–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
5 Shirk, , Competitive Comrades: Career Incentives and Student Strategies in China (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1982) 4–7, 56–62.Google Scholar
6 Marshall, , Collective Decision Making in Rural Japan, Michigan Papers in Japanese Studies (Ann Arbor: Center for Japanese Studies, 1984) 145–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
7 Rochon, , “Electoral Systems and the Basis of the Vote: The Case of Japan,” in Campbell, John C., ed., Parties, Candidates, and Voters in Japan: Six Quantitative Studies, Michigan Papers in Japanese Studies (Ann Arbor: Center for Japanese Studies, 1981)Google Scholar.
8 Walder, , “Work and Authority in Chinese Industry: State Socialism and the Institutional Culture of Dependency,” Ph.D. diss. (University of Michigan, 1981), 157Google Scholar.
9 See Gitelman, Zvi, “Soviet Political Culture: Insights from Jewish Emigrés,” Soviet Studies 29 (October 1977), 543–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
10 See Whyte, Martin K. and Parish, William L., Urban Life in Contemporary China (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1984) 337Google Scholar; Mathews, Jay and Mathews, Linda, One Billion: A China Chronicle (New York: Random House, 1983) 161Google Scholar.
11 McClelland, David, The Achieving Society (New York: Free Press, 1961)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
12 Almond and Verba (fn. 2).
13 See, for example, Zander, Alvin, “The Psychology of Group Processes,” Annual Review of Psychology 30 (1979), 417–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar; McGrath, Joseph E., Groups: Interaction and Performance (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1984)Google Scholar.
14 See Nemeth, Charlan, “A Critical Analysis of Research Utilizing the Prisoner's Dilemma Paradigm for the Study of Bargaining,” in Berkowitz, Leonard, ed., Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, VI (New York: Academic Press, 1972) 203–34Google Scholar; Schlenker, Barry R. and Bonoma, Thomas V., “Fun and Games: The Validity of Games for the Study of Conflict,” Journal of Conflict Resolution 22 (March 1978), 7–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
15 Allison, Graham T., Essence of Decision: Explaining the Cuban Missile Crisis (Boston: Little, Brown, 1971) 4Google Scholar; Argyris, Chris, “Some Unintended Consequences of Rigorous Research,” Psychological Bulletin 70 (November 1968), 190CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
16 George, , Presidential Decisionmaking in Foreign Policy: The Effective Use of Information and Advice (Boulder, CO: Westview, 1980) 26–28Google Scholar.
17 Steinbruner, John D., The Cybernetic Theory of Decision: New Dimensions of Political Analysis (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1974) 121–22Google Scholar.
18 See George (fn. 16), 26–27; Steinbruner (fn. 17), 145.
19 Kanter, Rosabeth Moss, Men and Women of the Corporation (New York: Basic Books, 1977) 47–68Google Scholar.
20 See Putnam, Robert D., The Comparative Study of Political Elites (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1976) 33–36, 108–109Google Scholar; Kanter (fn. 19), 47–68.
21 See Janis, Irving L., Groupthink Psychological Studies of Policy Decision and Fiascoes (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1982)Google Scholar; Berman, Larry, Planning a Tragedy: The Americanization of the War in Vietnam (New York: Norton, 1982)Google Scholar; George (fn. 16). We should not dismiss the possibility, however, that the reminiscences of political elites are distorted by memory and by the desire to impose meaning on the past.
22 For greater detail, see Gaenslen, Fritz, “Fiction and Reality: A Case Study,” Sociological Methods and Research 10 (May 1982), 379–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Gaenslen, Fritz, “Culture and Decision Making: Social Influence in China, Japan, Soviet Russia, and the United States,” Ph.D. diss. (University of Michigan, 1984)Google Scholar.
23 See George, Alexander, “The Case for Multiple Advocacy in Making Foreign Policy,” American Political Science Review 66 (September 1972), 751–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar; MacCrimmon, K. R. and Taylor, R. N., “Decision-making and Problem-solving,” in Dunnette, Marvin, ed., Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1976), 1397–1453Google Scholar.
24 The government-controlled Chinese list consisted of all fiction appearing in the monthly journal Chinese Literature (Peking: Foreign Language Press), and also listed in Tsai's, MeishiContemporary Chinese Novels and Short Stories 1949–1974: An Annotated Bibliography (Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1979)CrossRefGoogle Scholar as having been available to mainland Chinese readers. The uncensored Chinese list consisted of all fiction mentioned in Gibbs, Donald A. and Li, Yun-chen, A Bibliography of Studies and Translations of Modern Chinese Literature 1918–1942 (Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1975)CrossRefGoogle Scholar. The government-controlled Soviet list consisted of all fiction appearing in the monthly journal Soviet Literature (Moscow: Writers Union of the U.S.S.R.) and set in the R.S.F.S.R. The Soviet samizdat list consisted of all appropriate fiction mentioned in Hayward, Max, “Literature in the Soviet Period 1917–1975,” in Auty, Robert and Obolensky, Dimitri, eds., Companion to Russian Studies 2: An Introduction to Russian Language and Literature (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1975), 185–230Google Scholar, or in Brown, Deming, Soviet Russian Literature Since Stalin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1978)CrossRefGoogle Scholar. The Japanese list consisted of all fiction mentioned in Fujino, Yukio, Modem Japanese Literature in Western Translations: A Bibliography, 1868 to the Present (Tokyo: International House of Japan Library, 1972)Google Scholar, or appearing in Japan Quarterly (Tokyo: Asahi Shimbum) between 1972 and 1977. The American list consisted of all fiction appearing in the annual anthology, Best American Short Stories, between 1915 and 1970 (Boston: Houghton Mifflin).
25 It is appropriate here to say something about the use of fiction in English translation. In a study concerned with the impact of interpersonal relations on how people behave in disagreements, the use of translations is less than ideal because speech often contains subtle clues as to the nature of the relationship between speaker and listener. The reader who relies on translations, then, may be left with fewer clues with which to work. In the present case, how-ever, the available clues were sufficient; if they had not been, we should have found much less agreement between the readers in their coding of the variables and much less correspondence between fiction and reality. Although elements of tone, idiom, and characterization may be lost in the move from one language to another, this is less likely to be the case with plot, with the who-says-what-to-whom-with-what-effect that is the stuff of disagreements. For present purposes, it is irrelevant to what extent the translated fiction is representative of fiction that has not been translated. The proper question is whether there is some basis for supposing that in this fiction relationships between variables will mirror those found in life. For the uncensored fiction, I have reported such a basis.
26 Lukes, Steven, Individualism (Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1973) 73Google Scholar.
27 Wood, Ellen Meiksins, Mind and Politics: An Approach to the Meaning of Liberal and Socialist Individualism (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1972) 14Google Scholar.
28 Pennock, J. Roland, Democratic Political Theory (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1979). 59Google Scholar.
29 I conceive of the pure collectivist as a good deal less egalitarian than some readers might deem appropriate; however, I am concerned with man as I think we have known him historically and not as we, or Marx, or Rousseau, might like him to be.
30 The statistical associations reported throughout this study are small, and they sometimes do not meet standard levels of formal significance. Are the results likely to be an artifact of chance? Since all of the nine comparisons made in this study (three hypotheses by three countries) are in the expected direction, an argument can be made against such a conclusion. Nevertheless, are the results of sufficient magnitude to warrant our attention? In answering this question, readers should consider the following: First, the data probably understate the differences in cultural assumptions about the meaning of “self and “others.” This is so because these assumptions are not measured directly, but inferred from associations between attitudes and behavior. Research shows that the magnitude of such associations is likely to be modest even under the best of circumstances. (See Schuman, Howard and Johnson, Michael P., “Attitudes and Behavior,” Annual Review of Sociology 2 [1976], 161–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar.) Second, the data can also be seen as understating the differential impact of cultural assumptions on small-group decision making among political elites. This is so because we expect this impact to be most noteworthy under conditions of complexity, uncertainty, and ambiguity. In fiction, decision makers often face complexity, but the competing values and interests to be reconciled are rarely those of more than two people. The fictional decision makers also commonly face un-certainty about the relation of means to ends, but rarely do they face complexity at the same time. Finally, in no case do fictional decision makers face ambiguity. Disputants are always able to envision the outcomes they prefer. In short, the situations described in the fiction are simpler than those commonly said to confront political elites. Finally, collective decision processes are likely to be relatively indeterminate in character; in view of such indeterminacy, even modest levels of “explained variance” may be the best we can hope for.
31 See, for example, Henderson, Gail E. and Cohen, Myron S., The Chinese Hospital: A Socialist Work Unit (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1984) 140–46Google Scholar; McLendon, Hiram James, “Sogo Shosha: Social Structure and Sociocultural Process and Change in a Japanese General Trading Company,” Ph.D. diss. (Harvard University, 1979), 215–18Google Scholar; Theodore Shabad, “Tedious Meetings Are Siberian's Target,” New York Times, August 14, 1983, p. 4.
32 Wish, Myron, Deutsch, Morton, and Kaplan, Susan J., “Perceived Dimensions of Inter£ personal Relations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 33 (April 1976), 409–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
33 Lerner, Allan W., The Politics of Decision-Making: Strategy, Cooperation, and Conflict (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, 1976) 38–81Google Scholar.
34 Shiflett, Samuel C. and Nealey, Stanley M., “The Effects of Changing Leader Power: A Test of 'Situational Engineering',” Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 17 (December 1972), 371–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
35 Kipnis, David, “Does Power Corrupt?” journal of Personality and Social Psychology 24 (October 1972), 33–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
36 See Mulder, Mauk, “Power Equalization Through Participation?” Administrative Science Quarterly 16 (March 1971), 31–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Milgram, Stanley, Obedience to Authority (New York: Harper & Row, 1974) 93Google Scholar; Webster, Murray and Driskell, James E., “Status Generalization: A Review and Some New Data,” American Sociological Review 43 (April 1978), 220–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
37 In the following discussion, I am indebted to Mohr, Laurence B., “Authority and Democracy in Organizations,” Human Relations 30 (October 1977), 919–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
38 Mulder, Mauk, “Reduction of Power Differences in Practice,” in Hofstede, Geert and Kassem, M. Sami, eds., European Contributions to Organization Theory (Assen, Netherlands: Van Gorcum, 1976) 79–94Google Scholar; Mohr (fn. 37).
39 Aberbach, Joel D., “Power Consciousness: A Comparative Analysis,” American Political Science Review 71 (December 1977), 553CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
40 Rubin, Jeffrey Z. and Brown, Bert R., The Social Psychology of Bargaining and Negotiation (New York: Academic Press, 1975) 172–75Google Scholar.
41 Ibid., 172–73.
42 Swap, Walter C. and Rubin, Jeffrey Z., “Measurement of Interpersonal Orientation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44 (January 1983), 211CrossRefGoogle Scholar. None of the differences found in the fiction is an artifact of different proportions of female disputants in the different sets of fiction.
43 Ibid., 218.
44 In this regard, see Jones, Ellen, “Committee Decision Making in the Soviet Union,” World Politics 36 (January 1984), 165–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Oksenberg, Michel, “Methods of Communication within the Chinese Bureaucracy,” The China Quarterly (No. 57, 1974), 1–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Lieberthal, Kenneth, Central Documents and Politburo Politics in China, Michigan Papers in Chinese Studies (Ann Arbor: Center for Chinese Studies, 1978)CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Vogel, Ezra F., ed., Modern Japanese Organization and Decision Making (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1975)Google Scholar.
45 Rogowski, Ronald, “Rationalist Theories of Politics: A Midterm Report,” World Politics 30 (January 1978), 296–323, at 297CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
46 See Nisbett, Richard and Ross, Lee, Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1980)Google Scholar; Kahneman, Daniel and Tversky, Amos, “The Psychology of Preferences,” Scientific American 246 (January 1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
47 See, for example, Anderson, Paul A., “Decision Making by Objection and the Cuban Missile Crisis,” Administrative Science Quarterly 28 (June 1983), 383–403CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
48 Axelrod, Robert, “Argumentation in Foreign Policy Settings: Britain in 1918, Munich in 1938, and Japan in 1970,” Journal of Conflict Resolution 21 (December 1977), 727–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
49 Gaenslen (fn. 22, 1984), 201.
50 This point is made by Child, John, “Culture, Contingency, and Capitalism in the Cross-National Study of Organizations,” in Cummings, L. L. and Staw, Barry N., eds., Research in Organizational Behavior, III (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1981) 307Google Scholar.
- 20
- Cited by