Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T18:02:01.987Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spatial zones of binocular rivalry in central and peripheral vision

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2009

Robert P. O'Shea
Affiliation:
Cresap Neuroscience Laboratory, Northwestern University, Evanston
T. J. Mueller
Affiliation:
Cresap Neuroscience Laboratory, Northwestern University, Evanston

Abstract

This paper presents results from psychophysical experiments on human binocular rivalry in central and peripheral vision. Results show that the incidence of periods of exclusive visibility of a given eye's rival target increased with decreasing target size, and for a given sized target exclusive visibility increased with retinal eccentricity. Control measures confirmed that these results were not attributable solely to reduced peripheral acuity, to Troxler's effect, or to spatial frequency. We computed the minimum-sized stimulus that would lead to a criterion level of exclusive visibility of one or the other eye; this we term the spatial zone of binocular rivalry. The change in estimated size of spatial zones of rivalry with eccentricity compares favorably with estimates of human cortical magnification. We propose a model that assumes concentrically organized zones of rivalry. These zones do not function independently, but instead exhibit a high degree of mutual excitatory cooperativity. The model has multiple solutions for the foveal zone size, but the best fits predict a diameter of 5.3 or 7.3 min of visual angle; these values dovetail nicely with our empirical estimates of the foveal zone size.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Blake, R. (1981). Binocular rivalry and perceptual inference. Perception and Psychophysics 29, 7778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R. & Wilson, H.R.Neural models of stereopsis. Trends in Neuroscience 14, 445452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R., O'Shea, R.P. & Halpern, D.L. (1988). Spatial extent of binocular rivalry suppression. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 29, 411.Google Scholar
Breese, B.B. (1899). On inhibition. Psychological Review Monograph Supplement 3, (1, Whole No. 11).Google Scholar
Breese, B.B. (1909). Binocular rivalry. Psychological Review 16, 410415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowey, A. & Rolls, E.T. (1974). Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity. Experimental Brain Research 21, 447454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, P.M. & Whitteridge, D. (1961). The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys. Journal of Physiology 159, 203221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, A.K., Konig, P., Kreiter, A.K. & Singer, W. (1991). Interhemispheric synchronization of oscillatory neuronal responses in cat visual cortex. Science 252, 11771179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fahle, M. (1982). Binocular rivalry: Suppression depends on orientation and spatial frequency. Vision Research 22, 787800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fahle, M. (1987) Naso-temporal asymmetry of binocular inhibition. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 28, 10161017.Google ScholarPubMed
Gattass, R., Sousa, A.P.B. & Gross, C.G. (1988). Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience 8, 18311845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossberg, S. & Marshall, J.A. (1989). Stereo boundary fusion by cortical complex cells; a system of maps, filters, and feedback networks for multiplexing distributed data. Neural Networks 2, 2951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1964). Contralateral suppressive fields of binocular combination. Psychonomic Science 1, 157158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollins, M. (1980). The effect of contrast on the completeness of binocular rivalry suppression. Perception and Psychophysics 27, 550556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, G. (1944). The organization of the visual cortex in man. Proceedings of the Royal Society B (London) 132, 348361.Google Scholar
Hubel, D.H. & Wiesel, T.N. (1974). Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: A parallel relationship between field size, scatter, and magnification factor. Journal of Comparative Neurology 158, 295305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, L. (1963). On the spread of suppression and binocular rivalry. Vision Research 3, 401415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S.A. & Levi, D.M. (1987). Position sense of the peripheral retina. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 4, 15431554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehky, S. (1988). An astable multivibrator model of binocular rivalry. Perception 17, 215228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W.J.M. (1965). On Binocular Rivalry. Soesterberg, The Netherlands: Institute for Perception RVO-TNO.Google Scholar
Levi, D.M., Klein, S.A. & Aitsebaomo, A.P. (1985). Vernier acuity, crowding and cortical magnification. Vision Research 25, 963977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, T.J. (1990). A physiological model of binocular rivalry. Visual Neuroscience 4, 6374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, T.J. & Blake, R. (1990). Estimating the size of a zone of suppression for binocular rivalry. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 31, 525.Google Scholar
Ogle, K.N. (1964). Researchers in Binocular Vision. New York: Hafner.Google Scholar
O'Shea, R.P. (1987). Chronometric analysis supports fusion rather than suppression theory of binocular vision. Vision Research 27, 781791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pointer, J.S. (1986). The cortical magnification factor and photopic vision. Biological Review 61, 97119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Press, W.H., Flannery, B.P., Teukolsky, S.A. & Vetterling, W.T. (1989). Numerical Recipes in Pascal. The Art of Scientific Computing. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Richards, W. (1970). Oculomotor effects upon binocular rivalry. Psychologiste Forschung 33, 135154.Google ScholarPubMed
Rovamo, J. & Virsu, V. (1979). An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factor. Experimental Brain Research 37, 495510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schein, S.J. & De Monasterio, F.M. (1987). Mapping of retinal and geniculate neurons onto striate cortex of macaque. Journal of Neuroscience 7, 9961009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schor, C., Wesson, M. & Robertson, K.M. (1986). Combined effects of spatial frequency and retinal eccentricity upon fixation disparity. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 63, 619626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thibos, L.N., Cheney, F.E. & Walsh, D.J. (1987). Retinal limits to the detection and resolution of gratings. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 4, 15241529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vijn, P.C.M., Van Dijk, B.W. & Sperkreijse, H. (1991). Visual stimulation reduces EEG activity in man. Brain Research 550, 4953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, P. (1978). Binocular rivalry: central or peripheral selective processes? Psychological Bulletin 85, 376389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, M.J. (1987). Spatiotemporal properties of grating motion detection in the center and the periphery of the visual field. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 4, 16271633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yager, D. & Davis, E.T. (1987). Variations of visual functions across the visual field: Feature issue. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 4,14811703.Google Scholar