Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T11:24:38.697Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychophysical estimation of the best illumination for appreciation of Renaissance paintings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2006

PAULO D. PINTO
Affiliation:
Department of Physics, Campus de Gualtar, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
JOÃO M.M. LINHARES
Affiliation:
Department of Physics, Campus de Gualtar, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
JOÃO A. CARVALHAL
Affiliation:
Department of Physics, Campus de Gualtar, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
SÉRGIO M.C. NASCIMENTO
Affiliation:
Department of Physics, Campus de Gualtar, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal

Abstract

A variety of light sources are used in museum environments where the main concern is to prevent damaging effects of the light on paintings. Yet, the visual impression of an artistic painting is strongly influenced by the intensity and spectral profile of the illumination. The aim of this work was to determine psychophysically the spectral profile of the illumination preferred by observers when seeing paintings dated from the Renaissance époque and to investigate how their preferences correlate with the color temperature of the illumination and with the chromatic diversity of the paintings. Hyperspectral images of five oil paintings on wood were collected at the museum and the appearance of the paintings under five representative illuminants computed. Chromatic diversity was estimated by computing the representation of the paintings in the CIELAB color space and by counting the number of nonempty unit cubes occupied by the corresponding color volume. A paired-comparison experiment using precise cathode ray tube (CRT) reproductions of the paintings rendered with several illuminant pairs with different color temperatures was carried out to determine observers' preference. The illuminant with higher color temperature was always preferred except for one pair where no clear preference was expressed. The preferred illuminant produced the larger chromatic diversity, and for the condition where no specific illuminant was preferred the number of colors produced by the illuminant pair was very similar, a result suggesting that preference could have been influenced by chromatic diversity.

Type
TESTING AND METHODS
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Arend, L. & Reeves, A. (1986). Simultaneous color constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 3, 17431751.Google Scholar
Berns, R.S. (2001). The science of digitizing paintings for color-accurate image archives: A review. Journal of Imaging Science and Technology 45, 305325.Google Scholar
Brill, T.B. (1980). Light: Its Interactions with Art and Antiques. New York: Plenum Press.
Carvalhal, M.J.A.T.d. (2004). Digitalização de pintura artística com imagiografia hiperespectral. M.Sc. Thesis, Universidade do Minho, Braga, Portugal.
Davis, R.G. & Ginthner, D.N. (1990). Correlated color temperature, illuminance level, and the Kruithof curve. Journal of the Illuminating Engineering Society 19, 2738.Google Scholar
Fairchild, M.D. (2005). Color Appearance Models. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Fairchild, M.D. & Reniff, L. (1995). Time-course of chromatic adaptation for color-appearance judgments. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 12, 824833.Google Scholar
Foster, D.H., Nascimento, S.M.C., & Amano, K. (2004). Information limits on neural identification of colored surfaces in natural scenes. Visual Neuroscience 21, 331336.Google Scholar
Kemp, M. (1990). The Science of Art: Optical Themes in Western Art from Brunelleschi to Seurot. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Linhares, J.M.M., Nascimento, S.M.C., Foster, D.H., & Amano, K. (2004). Chromatic diversity of natural scenes. Perception 33, 6565.Google Scholar
Luo, M.R., Cui, G., & Rigg, B. (2001). The development of the CIE 2000 colour-difference formula: CIEDE2000. Color Research and Application 26, 340350.Google Scholar
Nassau, K. (1998). Color for Science, Art and Technology. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B.V.
Pointer, M.R. & Attridge, G.G. (1998). The number of discernible colours. Color Research and Application 23, 5254.Google Scholar
Scuello, M., Abramov, I., Gordon, J., & Weintraub, S. (2004a). Museum lighting: Why are some illuminants preferred? Journal of the Optical Society of America A 21, 306311.Google Scholar
Scuello, M., Abramov, I., Gordon, J., Weintraub, S., & Weintra, S. (2004b). Museum lighting: Optimizing the illuminant. Color Research and Application 29, 121127.Google Scholar
Taft, W.S., Mayer, J.W., Newman, R., Stulik, D., & Kuniholm, P. (2000). The Science of Paintings. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Thomson, G. (1986). The Museum Environment. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Turner, J. (1996). The Dictionary of Art. London, UK: Macmillan Publishers.
Uchikawa, K. & Ikeda, M. (1981). Temporal deterioration of wavelength discrimination with successive comparison method. Vision Research 21, 591595.Google Scholar
Werner, A., Sharpe, L.T., & Zrenner, E. (2000). Asymmetries in the time-course of chromatic adaptation and the significance of contrast. Vision Research 40, 11011113.Google Scholar
Wyszecki, G. & Stiles, W.S. (1982). Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae. New York: John Wiley and Sons.