Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:43:14.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Religious Attendance Moderates the Environmental Effect on Prosocial Behavior in Nigerian Adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2019

Yoon-Mi Hur*
Affiliation:
Research Institute for Welfare Society, Mokpo National University, Jeonnam, South Korea
Hoe-Uk Jeong
Affiliation:
Department of Education, Mokpo National University, Jeonnam, South Korea
Frances Ajose
Affiliation:
Clinical Sciences, Lagos State University College of Medicine, Lagos, Nigeria
Ariel Knafo-Noam
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
*
Author for correspondence: Yoon-Mi Hur, Email: [email protected]

Abstract

There is a growing body of literature linking religious attendance to prosocial behavior (PB). The main purposes of the present study were to estimate genetic and environmental influences on the frequency of religious attendance (FRA) and to explore whether and how FRA moderates genetic and/or environmental influences on PB. As part of the Nigerian Twin and Sibling Study, 2860 (280 monozygotic male, 417 monozygotic female, 544 dizygotic male, 699 dizygotic female, and 920 opposite-sex dizygotic) twins (mean age = 14.2 years; SD = 1.7 years; age range = 12–18 years) completed a questionnaire regarding FRA and a PB scale. Similar to the findings from western twin samples, FRA showed substantial shared environmental influences of 74% (95% CI = 69%, 78%), with absence of genetic effects. The phenotypic correlation between FRA and PB was modest but positive and significant (r = .12; p < .01), suggesting that PB is higher among more frequent attenders than among less frequent attenders. The results of gene–environment (G × E) interaction model-fitting analysis revealed that FRA changed individual environmental experiences rather than genetic effects on PB such that while genetic variance was stable, non-shared environmental variance declined, leading the total phenotypic variance of PB to decrease with increasing levels of religious attendance.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

The ‘religious prosociality hypothesis’ states that religious belief or concept facilitates prosocial behavior (PB) such as giving to charity, volunteering or helping strangers (Norenzayen & Shariff, Reference Norenzayan and Shariff2008). Since the publication of the religious prosociality hypothesis, the relationship between religiousness and PB has been hotly debated in psychology and the general public. Although a few researchers have found negative relationships between religiousness and PB (e.g., Decety et al., Reference Decety, Cowell, Lee, Mahasneh, Malcolm-Smith, Selcuk and Zhou2015), most correlational studies have shown modest but positive relationships between the two (Benson et al., Reference Benson, Scales, Sesma and Roehlkepartain2006; Furrow et al., Reference Furrow, King and White2004). Experimental research that induced religious thinking and subsequently measured PB of subjects yielded positive relationships as well. For example, a recent meta-analysis of religious priming studies has shown that religious reminders increased PB, with an average effect size of Hedges’ g = 0.27 (95% CI = 0.15, 0.40) (Shariff et al., Reference Shariff, Willard, Andersen and Norenzayan2016).

While the relationship between religiousness and PB has been relatively well established, how genetic and environmental effects combine to influence this relationship remains poorly understood. In an attempt to understand the causal mechanism of the relationship, Koenig et al. (Reference Koenig, McGue, Krueger and Bouchard2007) have applied a bivariate genetic model to adult male twin data for altruistic behavior and religiousness and found that both common genetic and shared environmental variances explained the association.

Twin studies of PB on the basis of western and Asian samples have shown that about 30–50% of the variance in children and adolescents were attributable to genetic influences, with the remaining variance being predominantly due to non-shared environmental influences and measurement error (Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, Light-Häusermann, Rijsdijk and Eley2009; Hur & Rushton, Reference Hur and Rushton2007; Knafo & Plomin, Reference Knafo and Plomin2006; Scourfield et al., Reference Scourfield, John, Martin and McGuffin2004). In contrast, most twin studies of religious attendance have indicated substantial influence of shared environmental factors with the absence of genetic factors in childhood and adolescence (Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, de Geus, van Baal and Koopmans1999; Bradshaw & Ellison, Reference Bradshaw and Ellison2008; Truett et al., Reference Truett, Eaves, Meyer, Heath and Martin1992), although some studies have found reverse patterns in adulthood (Hvidtjørn et al., Reference Hvidtjørn, Petersen, Hjelmborg, Skytthe, Christensen and Hvidt2013; Kendler & Myers, Reference Kendler and Myers2009; Kirk et al., Reference Kirk, Maes, Neale, Heath, Martin and Eaves1999; Koenig et al., Reference Koenig, McGue, Krueger and Bouchard2005). For example, in the Kendler and Myers study (Reference Kendler and Myers2009), shared environmental factors explained about 50% of the variance of frequency of church attendance in childhood and adolescence. In adulthood, however, these factors declined dramatically to a statistically non-significant amount. In contrast, genetic and non-shared environmental factors progressively increased with increasing age, accounting for about 60% and 40%, respectively, of the variance in adulthood. These age-related changes may be attributable to a decreasing family influence as children become older, and there are increasing influences from individual life events (e.g., marriage/divorce, birth of own child, accidents, illness). Results on sex difference in genetic and environmental influences on the frequency of church attendance are mixed: while some studies (e.g., Truett et al., Reference Truett, Eaves, Meyer, Heath and Martin1992, Reference Truett, Eaves, Walters, Heath, Hewitt, Meyer and Kendler1994) have reported that females had greater genetic and smaller shared environmental influences on church attendance than did males, others have found the opposite pattern (e.g., Eaves et al., Reference Eaves, Hatemi, Prom-Womley and Murrelle2008; Winter et al., Reference Winter, Kaprio, Viken, Karvonen and Rose1999).

Twin studies of PB and religious attendance have rarely been undertaken among Africans. Recently, using a sample of Nigerian public school twins as used in the present study, we demonstrated that the magnitudes of genetic and environmental influences on PB were within the range of those found in European and East Asian samples. Specifically, genetic and non-shared environmental influences on PB were 0.38 (95% CI = 0.31, 0.46) and 0.62 (95% CI = 0.54, 0.69), respectively (Hur et al., Reference Hur, Taylor, Jeong, Park and Haberstick2017). These findings were surprising given the large differences in environments and genetic variations between African and non-African populations (Campbell & Tishkoff, Reference Campbell and Tishkoff2008).

Previous studies have demonstrated that religiousness modified the genetic and/or environmental effects on adolescents’ behaviors. For example, using the gene–environment (G × E) interaction model-fitting approach (Purcell, Reference Purcell2002), Button et al. (Reference Button, Hewitt, Rhee, Corley and Stallings2010) have shown that genetic variance in problem alcohol use decreased significantly with increasing levels of religiosity in both males and females during adolescence. The authors suggested that reduced genetic variance in problem alcohol use in adolescence might be the consequence of large social control in adolescence. Consistent with the Button et al. study, Koopmans et al. (Reference Koopmans, Slutske, van Baal and Boomsma1999) have also found higher heritability (40%) in alcohol use initiation among females without a religious upbringing compared with females with a religious upbringing (0%). Based on the sample that Koopmans et al. have used, Boomsma et al. (Reference Boomsma, de Geus, van Baal and Koopmans1999) have shown that religious upbringing reduced the influence of genetic factors on disinhibition in males but not in females, suggesting that religious upbringing can moderate the genetic effects on disinhibition at least in males. However, all of these studies have focused on the moderating role of religiousness in problem behaviors and related traits, with little consideration of the moderating role in positive character traits such as PB. Frequent exposures to religious teachings that emphasize the value of PB may serve to activate specific genetic mechanisms underlying PB or restrict environmental variabilities in religious followers due to the influence that religious organizations may exert. Using the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) susceptibility variants, Sasaki et al. (Reference Sasaki, Kim, Mojaverian, Kelley, Park and Janusonis2013) have investigated whether religion priming has G × E interaction effects to influence PB. The authors have found that participants with DRD4 susceptibility variants were more prosocial when primed with religion than when not primed with religion, whereas participants without DRD4 susceptibility variants were not affected by religious priming. Although the Sasaki et al. study suffered from a small sample size, the results suggested that there may be a G × E interaction in the relationship between religiousness and PB.

Using a G × E interaction modeling (Purcell, Reference Purcell2002), the present study aimed to examine whether and how the frequency of religious attendance (FRA) moderates genetic and environmental influences on PB in Nigerian adolescents. Nigeria is a very religious nation. Over 98% of the Nigerian population is known to be either Christian or Muslim, with the remainder having African traditional religions or no religion (Pew Research Center, 2012). It has been estimated that whereas 16.4% of the US population and 42.1% of the Dutch population are religiously unaffiliated, only about 0.4% of the Nigerian population are unaffiliated (Pew Research Center, 2012). Thus, the results of the present study may have an implication in understanding the processes of how religious attendance contributes to the development of PB, especially in a highly religious environment.

Methods

Sample

The sample included 2860 adolescent twins aged 12–18 years. These were 280 (128 pairs and 24 individuals) monozygotic males (MZM), 417 (197 pairs and 23 individuals) monozygotic females (MZF), 544 (246 pairs and 52 individuals) dizygotic males (DZM), 699 (328 pairs and 43 individuals) dizygotic females (DZF), and 920 (418 pairs and 84 individuals) opposite-sex dizygotic (OSDZ) twins drawn from the Nigerian Twin and Sibling Registry (Hur et al., Reference Hur, Kim, Chung, Shin, Jeong and Auta2013). The mean (SD) age of these twins was 14.2 (1.7) years. In terms of religious affiliation, 64.3% of our twins identified themselves as Christian, 35.6% as Muslim and 0.1% as others. An excess of Christians found in the present sample was in line with the demographic characteristics of the population in Lagos state in Nigeria (Pew Research Center, 2012).

Twins were recruited from 272 public junior and senior secondary schools in Lagos state. The details of the recruitment procedures of the present sample can be found in Hur et al. (Reference Hur, Kim, Chung, Shin, Jeong and Auta2013, Reference Hur, Taylor, Jeong, Park and Haberstick2017). Briefly, we first obtained approvals to conduct this research from the Ministry of Education in Lagos state and the Health Research and Ethics Committee of the Lagos State University Teaching Hospital in Nigeria. Then, we visited each of the 272 schools and administered the questionnaires to the twins in a library or a special classroom. During the testing session, a saliva sample was taken and analyzed to determine twins’ zygosity. Eighteen microsatellite markers of DNA, including amelogenin, were analyzed to test zygosity of twins. A much larger number of dizygotic than monozygotic twins in the present sample likely reflected twin birth rates in Nigeria (MacGillivray, Reference MacGillivray1986).

Measures

Prosocial behavior (PB) scale

The PB scale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, Reference Goodman1997) was used to assess PB. The PB scale includes five prosocial items regarding sharing, helping and being kind and considerate of others. The PB scale of the SDQ has consistently emerged from factor analysis of the SDQ items across several countries in Europe, South Asia and Africa (Stevanovic et al., Reference Stevanovic, Urbán, Atilola, Vostanis, Singh Balhara, Avicenna and Petrov2015). Twins were instructed to rate themselves on a three-point scale (0 = not true to 2 = certainly true) for each of the five items. The responses to the five items were summed to create the score of the PB scale, where higher scores represent higher levels of PB. Cronbach α reliability of the five items was 0.63 for the present twin sample.

Frequency of religious attendance

Self-report data on the FRA were obtained from a single question: ‘How often do you attend religious service?’ Twins were asked to choose one of the six response options: (1) not at all, (2) 1–2 times a year, (3) about once a month, (4) 2–3 times a month, (5) about once a week, (6) more than once a week. Frequencies of responses of the six options were 2.5%, 2.9%, 3.4%, 7.1%, 13.4% and 70.8%, respectively. As the distribution of FRA was very skewed and kurtotic with a skewness of −2.2 and a kurtosis of 4.1, FRA was operationalized as a categorical variable. We combined the first four options to one category, which resulted in three categories: non-frequent attenders (2–3 times per month or less; 15.8%), frequent attenders (about once a week; 13.4%) and very frequent attenders (more than once a week; 70.8%) (Figure 1).

Fig. 1. Proportions of religious attendance among three categories in the study sample.

Statistical Analysis

Polychoric correlations of FRA were estimated separately for MZM, DZM, MZF, DZF and OSDZ twins by using the raw ordinal data option in Mx (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2003). The general sex limitation model-fitting analyses were conducted to determine additive genetic (A), shared environment (C) and non-shared environment plus measurement error (E) for FRA.

As there were no significant shared genetic effects in FRA and PB, the univariate G × E interaction model (Figure 2; Purcell, Reference Purcell2002) was applied to the data to determine whether genetic and environmental contributions to PB vary as a function of the moderator, FRA. The univariate G × E interaction model includes the standard paths, that is, additive genetic (a), shared environment (c), non-shared environmental influences and measurement error (e) and the magnitudes of moderation effects related to A, C and E, respectively, that are allowed to vary across the levels of the moderator (M). Thus, the phenotypic variance (Vp) in this model can be expressed as: Vp = (a + βαM)2 + (c + βcM)2 + (e + βeM)2. In this equation, βα, βc and βe represent the magnitude of the moderating effects associated with A, C and E, respectively. Additionally, we allowed µ + βmM to represent the moderating effect of FRA on the mean of PB. To determine the best-fitting, most parsimonious model, we used the likelihood ratio test (LRT) and Akaike’s information criterion (AIC = −2LL — 2df ). Models having lower AIC are considered more parsimonious and thus preferred (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987).

Fig. 2. Additive genetic (A), shared environment (C) and non-shared environment and error (E) effects on the scores of prosocial behavior (PB). a, c and e = unmoderated genetic, shared environment and non-shared environment and error components; βa, βc and βe = moderated components of a, c and e, respectively. βm = main effect of moderator; M = moderator; μ = grand mean. M = moderator (frequency of religious attendance), β = regression coefficient for M.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Twin Correlations

There were modest but significant age and sex effects on PB (older > younger; female > male). PB was significantly higher in Christians than in Muslims (8.1 vs. 7.8). However, the mean difference was trivial (Cohen’s d = 0.15) and appeared to attain statistical significance due to a large sample. The details of the descriptive statistics for PB can be found in Hur et al. (Reference Hur, Taylor, Jeong, Park and Haberstick2017). FRA was not significantly different between males and females (Δχ22 = 5.0; p = .08) in our sample. We divided the total sample into junior and senior secondary school students to examine the main effect of age on FRA. However, no significant difference was found between the two age groups (Δχ22 = 4.2; p = .12).

Maximum likelihood correlations for PB were 0.28 (95% CI = 0.11, 0.44) for MZM, 0.26 (95% CI = 0.13, 0.38) for DZM, 0.45 (95% CI = 0.32, 0.56) for MZF, 0.14 (95% CI = 0.02, 0.26) for DZF and 0.19 (95% CI = 0.09, 0.29) for OSDZ twins. The difference between monozygotic and dizygotic correlations for PB was larger in females than in males, suggesting that genetic effects on PB may be larger in females than in males. However, model-fitting analysis indicated that the magnitudes of genetic and environmental effects were not significantly different between the two sexes (Hur et al., Reference Hur, Taylor, Jeong, Park and Haberstick2017). Polychoric correlations for FRA were 0.78 (95% CI = 0.63, 0.88) for MZM, 0.64 (95% CI = 0.51, 0.74) for DZM, 0.75 (95% CI = 0.64, 0.84) for MZF, 0.76 (95% CI = 0.67, 0.83) for DZF and 0.76 (95% CI = 0.68, 0.82) for OSDZ twins. In both sex groups, twin correlations were very high and similar across monozygotic and dizygotic twins, suggesting the presence of substantial shared environmental influences on FRA. The OSDZ twin correlation was not lower than the correlation for DZM or DZF, providing little evidence for the presence of sex-specific genetic effects on FRA.

General Sex Limitation Model-Fitting Analysis for FRA

The general sex limitation model-fitting analysis for FRA showed that sex-specific genetic effects were not significant (Δχ12 = 2.2; p = .14). Furthermore, the magnitudes of additive genetic and shared and non-shared environmental influences were not significantly different between the two sexes (Δχ42 = 3.9; p = .42). While dropping shared environmental effects from the full model significantly worsened the model fit (Δχ52 = 95.6; p = .00), dropping additive genetic effects did not (Δχ52 = 4.4; p = .50). From these model comparisons, we concluded that the model that included shared and non-shared environmental influences alone was the best. In the best-fitting model, shared and non-shared environmental influences were 74% (95% CI = 69%, 78%) and 26% (95% CI = 22%, 31%), respectively, for both sexes.

G × E Model-Fitting Analysis

The phenotypic correlation between FRA and PB was modest but positive (r = .12; p < .01), suggesting that PB is higher in more frequent attenders than in less frequent attenders. This association was not significantly different between Muslims and Christians (r = .13 in Christians; r = .12 in Muslims).

Table 1 presents the results of univariate G × E interaction analysis. When all of the moderation effects were removed from the full model simultaneously, a significant reduction in model fit occurred (model 3: Δχ32 = 17.4; p = .001), indicating the presence of the effects of moderation. When we removed the effect of moderation individually from the full model (models 4–6), only moderation on A (βa) was not significant, suggesting that the genetic effects on PB were stable across all levels of FRA. While removing C and moderation on C simultaneously produced a non-significant change in chi-square (model 8: Δχ22 = 4.1; p = .13), removing A and moderation on A yielded a significant difference in chi-square (model 7: Δχ22 = 13.3; p = .00). From model 8, we further eliminated moderation on A (βa; model 9) and on E (βe; model 10), respectively. These procedures yielded a significant chi-square change in model 10 but not in model 9, providing evidence for the presence of moderation on E. From these model comparisons using LRT, we concluded that model 9 was the best fitting one. AIC also suggested that model 9 was the best because it showed the lowest AIC.

Table 1. Results of the univariate G × E interaction models testing for the moderating effects of the frequency of religious attendance on genetic and environmental influences on prosocial behavior

Note: −2LL = −2 log-likelihood. A = additive genetic variance, C = shared environmental variance, E = non-shared environmental variance including measurement error. The best-fitting model is indicated in bold.

Figure 3 depicts the unstandardized additive genetic, non-shared environmental and total variances, and Figure 4 depicts the standardized variances of the corresponding components. Figure 3 shows that while additive genetic variance was stable across the levels of FRA, the non-shared environmental and total variances declined with increasing levels of FRA. These results suggest that a decrease in the total variance of PB with increasing FRA was due to a reduction in non-shared environmental rather than additive genetic variance. Among non-frequent attenders, non-shared environmental variances accounted for most of the total variance of PB, which resulted in a small heritability of PB in Figure 4. Among very frequent attenders, additive genetic variance explained a significant amount of the total variance, leading to a relatively high heritability of PB, as indicated in Figure 4. Figure 4 shows that heritability and non-shared environmental influences on PB were 29% (95% CI = 27%, 30%) and 71% (95% CI = 70%, 73%), respectively, among non-frequent attenders, while the corresponding estimates were 38% (95% CI = 33%, 44%) and 62% (95% CI = 56%, 67%) among very frequent attenders.

Fig. 3. Unstandardized additive genetic (A) and non-shared environment including measurement error (E) variances, and the total variance of prosocial behavior (PB) as a function of the frequency of religious attendance (FRA) in the best-fitting model (95% CIs are in parentheses).

Fig. 4. Standardized additive genetic (A) and non-shared environment including measurement error variances (E) of prosocial behavior (PB) as a function of the frequency of religious attendance (FRA) in the best-fitting model (95% CIs are in parentheses).

Discussion

Using a genetically informative design, the present study explored the role of religious attendance in the development of PB in public school twins in Nigeria. We first examined genetic and environmental influences on religious attendance in Nigerian adolescents. While genetic influence on FRA was not significant, the estimate of shared environmental influence was substantial (74%), replicating the findings from the western adolescent twin samples (Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, de Geus, van Baal and Koopmans1999; Bradshaw & Ellison, Reference Bradshaw and Ellison2008; Truett et al., Reference Truett, Eaves, Meyer, Heath and Martin1992). However, the finding that there was no significant sex difference in FRA in the present sample was not consistent with the general finding that females are more religious than males (Trzebiatowska & Bruce, Reference Trzebiatowska and Bruce2012). In the questionnaire given to twins, we included an item asking their parents’ religious affiliation. Over 98% of our twins reported that they had the same religious affiliation that their parents had. Thus, the sources of shared environment in religious attendance in the present sample were likely to be parental factors such as parents’ religious attendance, expectations, values and assortative mating for religious attendance (Truett et al., Reference Truett, Eaves, Walters, Heath, Hewitt, Meyer and Kendler1994).

The phenotypic correlation between religious attendance and PB in the present sample was significant but modest (r = .12), which was in line with the findings from Stavrova and Siegers (Reference Stavrova and Siegers2014). This correlation was also close to the finding from the Donahue and Benson study (Reference Donahue and Benson1995) where the authors have reported a correlation of .11 (p < .01) between altruism and church attendance in a large national sample of US adolescents. The result of G × E model-fitting analysis using FRA as an environmental moderator showed environment–environment (E × E) rather than G × E interaction: environmental experiences explained more of the variance in PB in less frequent religious attenders. Adolescents at the low end of the FRA were likely from non-religious households and learned PB from various sources rather than religious organizations. A large non-shared environmental variance found in adolescents at the low end of the FRA suggests that there were great variabilities in the prosocial characteristics of the peers and experiences of socialization, perhaps because these adolescents were relatively free from religious constraints. Due to the large non-shared environmental influences, the relative influence of genetic factors on PB was small among adolescents at the low end of the FRA. In contrast, adolescents at the high end of the FRA were from very religious families and were likely associated with religious peers who share and promote prosocial values. These adolescents were very frequently exposed to religious teachings that encourage practices of PB. We speculate that these religious atmospheres substantially attenuated the variation of the non-shared environment among adolescents at the high end of the FRA. Due to the reduction in non-shared environment, the relative influence of genetic factors in this group was high. The implication of our finding is that frequent religious attendance can change individual environmental experiences rather than genetic effects on PB, which may result in an increase in the mean level and a decrease in the total variability of PB.

The lack of a significant moderating effect on genetic variation in PB in our study is in contrast to the results of the Sasaki et al.’s study (Reference Sasaki, Kim, Mojaverian, Kelley, Park and Janusonis2013) and previous quantitative G × E studies that have examined the moderating role of religiousness (Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, de Geus, van Baal and Koopmans1999; Button et al., Reference Button, Hewitt, Rhee, Corley and Stallings2010; Koopmans et al., Reference Koopmans, Slutske, van Baal and Boomsma1999). However, the Sasaki et al. study was based on the candidate–gene approach, which has been criticized for a high rate of false-positives (Sullivan, Reference Sullivan2007). Prior quantitative G × E studies have focused on problem behaviors such as substance abuse or the initiation of substance use in adolescents, while our study examined a positive trait. Thus, whether or not religiousness can moderate genetic variations in positive traits such as PB awaits replications in other samples.

The present study has several limitations. First, we used FRA as a proxy measure of religiousness. However, it has been suggested that the measure of FRA underestimates the complexity of the religiousness construct (Hills & Pargament, Reference Hills and Pargament2003). It is, therefore, important in future studies to investigate whether and how other aspects of religiousness interact with genetic and environmental influences on PB. Prior studies have shown that personal forms of religiousness such as praying to God showed higher genetic influences than do social forms of religiousness such as church attendance (Hvidtjørn et al., Reference Hvidtjørn, Petersen, Hjelmborg, Skytthe, Christensen and Hvidt2013). Thus, the pattern of G × E may be different from our findings if we employ measures of a personal form of religiousness. Second, as our measures of FRA and PB are both self-reported, twins’ responses may have been contaminated by social desirability because more religious individuals tend to inflate self-ratings of prosocial characteristics (Galen, Reference Galen2012). However, several researchers have indicated that the relationship between religiousness and PB remained significant even when social desirability was controlled (e.g., Lewis, Reference Lewis1999, Reference Lewis2000; Saroglou et al., Reference Saroglou, Pichon, Trompette, Verschueren and Dernelle2005). Third, the present sample consisted of adolescents aged 12–18 years. As prior studies have shown that genetic and environmental influences on religious attendance change with age, the patterns of G × E interaction found in the present study may differ in adulthood. For instance, given an increase in genetic influences on religious attendance with age, gene–environment correlation between religious attendance and PB may be found to be significant in adulthood. Finally, our sample only comprised adolescent twins attending public schools in Lagos state, Nigeria. Thus, it remains to be seen whether or not the present findings extend to other ethnic groups or regions in Nigeria.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Charles Darwin Research Institute (USA) and the Ulster Institute for Social Research (UK) to the first author (Y.M.H.). I would like to thank the twins who participated in this study, and school teachers and principals in the public schools in Lagos state, the staff members in the Ministry of Education in Lagos state and many research assistants who kindly assisted this study. Special thanks are given to Human Pass, Korea, who assisted us with twins’ zygosity testing.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Sesma, A., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2006). Adolescent spirituality. Adolescent and Family Health, 4, 4151.Google Scholar
Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J., van Baal, G. C., & Koopmans, J. R. (1999). A religious upbringing reduces the influence of genetic factors on disinhibition: Evidence for interaction between genotype and environment. Twin Research, 2, 115125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, M., & Ellison, C. G. (2008). Do genetic factors influence religious life? Findings from a behavior genetic analysis of twin siblings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 47, 529544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Button, T. M., Hewitt, J. K., Rhee, S. H., Corley, R. P., & Stallings, M. C. (2010). The moderating effect of religiosity on the genetic variance of problem alcohol use. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 34, 16191624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, M. C., & Tishkoff, S. A. (2008). African genetic diversity: Implications for human demographic history, modern human origins, and complex disease mapping. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 9, 403433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Cowell, J. M., Lee, K., Mahasneh, R., Malcolm-Smith, S., Selcuk, B., & Zhou, X. (2015). The negative association between religiousness and children’s altruism across the world. Current Biology, 25, 29512955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donahue, M. J., & Benson, P. I. (1995). Religion and the well-being of adolescents. Journal of Social Issues, 51, 145160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eaves, L. J., Hatemi, P. K., Prom-Womley, E. C., & Murrelle, L. (2008). Social and genetic influences on adolescent religious attitudes and practices. Social Forces, 86, 16211646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furrow, J. L., King, P. E., & White, K. (2004). Religion and positive youth development: Identity, meaning, and prosocial concerns. Applied Developmental Science, 8, 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galen, L. W. (2012). Does religious belief promote prosociality? A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 876906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, A. M., Light-Häusermann, J. H., Rijsdijk, F., & Eley, T. C. (2009). Behavioral genetic analyses of prosocial behavior in adolescents. Developmental Science, 12, 165174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, P. C., & Pargament, K. I. (2003). Advances in the conceptualization and measurement of religion and spirituality. American Psychologist, 58, 6474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hur, Y.-M., Kim, J. W., Chung, K. W., Shin, J. S., Jeong, H.-U., & Auta, E. (2013). The Nigerian twin and sibling registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 282284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hur, Y.-M., & Rushton, J. P. (2007). Genetic and environmental contributions to prosocial behaviour in 2- to 9-year-old South Korean twins. Biological Letters, 22, 664666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hur, Y. M., Taylor, J., Jeong, H. U., Park, M. S., & Haberstick, B. C. (2017). Perceived family cohesion moderates environmental influences on prosocial behavior in Nigerian adolescent twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 20, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hvidtjørn, D., Petersen, P., Hjelmborg, J., Skytthe, A., Christensen, K., & Hvidt, N. C. (2013). Familial resemblance in religiousness in a secular society: A twin study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 544553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Myers, J. (2009). A developmental twin study of church attendance and alcohol and nicotine consumption: A model for analyzing the changing impact of genes and environment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 11501155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirk, K. M., Maes, H. H., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., & Eaves, L. J. (1999). Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: Models of family resemblance. Twin Research, 2, 99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knafo, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Parental discipline and affection and children’s prosocial behavior: Genetic and environmental links. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 147164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, L. B., McGue, M., Krueger, R. F., & Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2005). Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness: Findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings. Journal of Personality, 73, 471488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, L. B., McGue, M., Krueger, R. F., & Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2007). Religiousness, antisocial behavior, and altruism: Genetic and environmental mediation. Journal of Personality, 75, 265290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koopmans, J. R., Slutske, W. S., van Baal, G. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (1999). The influence of religion on alcohol use initiation: Evidence for genotype × environment interaction. Behavior Genetics, 29, 445453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, C. A. (1999). Is the relationship between religiosity and personality ‘contaminated’ by social desirability as assessed by the Lie Scale? A methodological reply to Michael W. Eysenck (1998). Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 2, 105114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, C. A. (2000). The religiosity-psychoticism relationship and the two factors of social desirability: A response to Michael W. Eysenck (1999). Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 3, 3945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacGillivray, I. (1986). Epidemiology of twin pregnancy. Seminars in Perinatology, 10, 48.Google ScholarPubMed
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical modeling. Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University Medical School.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 5862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pew Research Center. (2012). The global religious landscape: Religious composition by country, in numbers and percentage. Retrieved from http://www.pewforum.org/2012/12/18/table-religious-composition-by-country-in-percentagesGoogle Scholar
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saroglou, V., Pichon, I., Trompette, L., Verschueren, M., & Dernelle, R. (2005). Prosocial behavior and religion: New evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44, 323348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, J. Y., Kim, S. H., Mojaverian, T., Kelley, L. D. S., Park, I. Y., & Janusonis, S. (2013). Religion priming differentially increases prosocial behavior among variants of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene. Scan, 8, 209215.Google ScholarPubMed
Scourfield, J., John, B., Martin, N., & McGuffin, P. (2004). The development of prosocial behavior in children and adolescents: A twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 927935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming: A meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 2748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stavrova, O., & Siegers, P. (2014). Religious prosociality and morality across cultures: How social enforcement of religion shapes the effects of personal religiosity on prosocial and moral attitudes and behaviors. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 315333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevanovic, D., Urbán, R., Atilola, O., Vostanis, P., Singh Balhara, Y. P., Avicenna, M., … Petrov, P. (2015). Does the strengths and difficulties questionnaire — Self report yield invariant measurements across different nations? Data from the International Child Mental Health Study Group. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24, 323334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, P. F. (2007). Spurious genetic associations. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 11211126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Truett, K. R., Eaves, L. J., Meyer, J. M., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (1992). Religion and education as mediators of attitudes: A multivariate analysis. Behavior Genetics, 22, 4362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Truett, K. R., Eaves, L. J., Walters, E. E., Heath, A. C., Hewitt, J. K., Meyer, J. M., … Kendler, K. S. (1994). A model system for the analysis of family resemblance in extended kinships of twins. Behavior Genetics, 24, 3549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trzebiatowska, M., & Bruce, S. (2012). Why are women more religious than men? Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, T., Kaprio, J., Viken, R. J., Karvonen, S., & Rose, R. (1999). Individual differences in adolescent religiosity in Finland: Familial effects are modified by sex and region of residence. Twin Research, 2, 108114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Proportions of religious attendance among three categories in the study sample.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Additive genetic (A), shared environment (C) and non-shared environment and error (E) effects on the scores of prosocial behavior (PB). a, c and e = unmoderated genetic, shared environment and non-shared environment and error components; βa, βc and βe = moderated components of a, c and e, respectively. βm = main effect of moderator; M = moderator; μ = grand mean. M = moderator (frequency of religious attendance), β = regression coefficient for M.

Figure 2

Table 1. Results of the univariate G × E interaction models testing for the moderating effects of the frequency of religious attendance on genetic and environmental influences on prosocial behavior

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Unstandardized additive genetic (A) and non-shared environment including measurement error (E) variances, and the total variance of prosocial behavior (PB) as a function of the frequency of religious attendance (FRA) in the best-fitting model (95% CIs are in parentheses).

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Standardized additive genetic (A) and non-shared environment including measurement error variances (E) of prosocial behavior (PB) as a function of the frequency of religious attendance (FRA) in the best-fitting model (95% CIs are in parentheses).