Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by complex heterogeneous presentations and unknown cause. ASD is principally characterized by impaired social interaction and communication, and restrictive and repetitive behavior. ADHD is defined by symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 5 [DSM-5]; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). During the last two decades, the prevalence rate in children and adults with ASD or ADHD has dramatically risen. ASD is currently estimated to influence 1 in 68 children (Centers for Disease Control Prevention, 2014) and 1 in 100 adults (Brugha et al., Reference Brugha, McManus, Bankart, Scott, Purdon, Smith and Meltzer2011). For ADHD, more than 1 in 10 children (Visser et al., Reference Visser, Danielson, Bitsko, Holbrook, Kogan, Ghandour and Blumberg2014) and 1 in 30 adults (Fayyad et al., Reference Fayyad, De Graaf, Kessler, Alonso, Angermeyer, Demyttenaere and Lépine2007) are estimated to be affected. Both disorders are heritable, with heritability from external ratings ranging between 80% and 90% for ASD (Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Carlström, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2010; Losh et al., Reference Losh, Adolphs, Poe, Couture, Penn, Baranek and Piven2009) and about 80% for ADHD (Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Perlis, Doyle, Smoller, Goralnick, Holmgren and Sklar2005; Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Carlström, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2010) in children. In contrast, the heritability estimates of self-rated ASD and ADHD traits are about 57% (Hoekstra et al., Reference Hoekstra, Bartels, Verweij and Boomsma2007) and 30–40% (Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, Saviouk, Hottenga, Distel, De Moor, Vink and Willemsen2010; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Asherson, Chang, Ljung, Friedrichs, Larsson and Lichtenstein2013; Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008; van den Berg et al., Reference Van den Berg, Willemsen, de Geus and Boomsma2006), respectively, in adult samples.
Studies have further reported the frequent co-occurrence of ASD and ADHD (Rommelse et al., Reference Rommelse, Franke, Geurts, Hartman and Buitelaar2010). Approximately, 28–44% of the adult population diagnosed with ASD also meets Diagnostic and Statistical Manual IV (DSM-IV) criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) for ADHD (Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, Dittner, Bramham, Murphy, Knight and Russell2013; Lai et al. Reference Lai, Lombardo and Baron-Cohen2014). While ASD and ADHD negatively impact on daily functioning, this impact is particularly severe when these conditions occur together (Rao & Landa, Reference Rao and Landa2014). The estimated annual economic cost for individuals with ASD and ADHD were U.S. $268 billion (Leigh & Du, Reference Leigh and Du2015) and U.S. $143–266 billion (Doshi et al., Reference Doshi, Hodgkins, Kahle, Sikirica, Cangelosi, Setyawan and Neumann2012) in the United States, respectively.
Despite the high rates of comorbidity between ASD and ADHD, the underlying cause of their co-occurrence remains poorly understood. So far, there have been only four adult twin studies investigating the etiology of co-occurrence of ASD and ADHD traits. Two studies (Lundstrom et al., Reference Lundström, Chang, Kerekes, Gumpert, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2011; Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008), suggested that the co-occurrence of these disorders might be due to substantial genetic overlap between these two traits (genetic correlation; r g = ~0.60). Polderman and colleagues, further investigated the association between ASD and ADHD traits using the five trait-based dimensional measures of ASD (social skills, routine, attentional switching, imagination, and patterns) and two dimensions of ADHD (inattention and hyperactive/impulsive problems). Their study reported that this association would be the result of attention-related problems (Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Hoekstra, Vinkhuyzen, Sullivan, van der Sluis and Posthuma2013). Another study conducted by the same author (Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Hoekstra, Posthuma and Larsson2014) reported a substantial genetic overlap between restrictive-repetitive behaviors/interests and ADHD dimensions (r g = 0.61–0.64) using measures based on the DSM-5; however, the genetic overlap between social communication difficulties and ADHD dimensions was weak (r g = 0.20–0.22). Although our understanding of the genetic structure of these disorders is changing rapidly, molecular studies have failed to show shared genetic etiology for ADHD and ASD, reflecting (a) lack of power to detect common associated variants of small effect (Bulik-Sullivan et al., Reference Bulik-Sullivan, Finucane, Anttila, Gusev, Day, Loh and Neale2015; Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2013), and possibly (b) the confounding effects of unmeasured cross-traits in case-control study designs, that is, failure to control for ADHD traits in comparison groups in molecular genetic studies of ASD, and conversely, failure to control for ASD traits in comparison groups in molecular genetic studies of ADHD, which the effect of either would be to erode the ability to detect overlapping genetic variance.
Increasing evidence suggests that the continua of behavioral traits, whose extremes reflect clinical neuropsychiatric disorders are contributed to by variations in what has historically been construed as personality, and that the symptom structure of psychopathology in general may be more parsimonious than has been traditionally appreciated. Specifically, neuroticism, defined as the tendency to experience negative emotions (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Deary and Whiteman2003), may share causal origins with the drivers of comorbidity between ASD and ADHD. Neuroticism is a moderately heritable personality trait with heritability estimates ranging from 49% to 56% (Hansell et al., Reference Hansell, Wright, Medland, Davenport, Wray, Martin and Hickie2012; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner, Gatz and Pedersen2007) and characterized by proneness to negative emotions, such as anxiety, sadness, guilt, fear, anger, and stress (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Deary and Whiteman2003; Zuckerman & Stelmack, Reference Zuckerman and Stelmack2004). Some of these characteristics are noted as frequently elevated in both ASD (Duvekot et al, Reference Duvekot, van der Ende, Constantino, Verhulst and Greaves-Lord2016; Gillott et al., Reference Gillott, Furniss and Walter2001; Gillott & Standen, Reference Gillott and Standen2007; Hurtig et al., Reference Hurtig, Kuusikko, Mattila, Haapsamo, Ebeling, Jussila and Moilanen2009) and ADHD (Alexander & Harrison, Reference Alexander and Harrison2013; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Alexander and Armstrong2013) population. Moreover, clinic-based (Schriber et al., Reference Schriber, Robins and Solomon2014; Schwartzman et al., Reference Schwartzman, Wood and Kapp2016) and population-based (Austin, Reference Austin2005; Wakabayashi et al., Reference Wakabayashi, Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright2006) studies have indicated that higher neuroticism is associated with higher levels of autistic traits. Several lines of evidence also suggest that higher neuroticism is linked with the severity of ADHD (Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Romanos, Dempfle, Heine, Windemuth-Kieselbach, Kruse and Brocke2007; Knouse et al., Reference Knouse, Traeger, O'Cleirigh and Safren2013; Michielsen et al., Reference Michielsen, Comijs, Semeijn, Beekman, Deeg and Kooij2014; Nigg et al., Reference Nigg, John, Blaskey, Huang-Pollock, Willicut, Hinshaw and Pennington2002; Parker et al., Reference Parker, Majeski and Collin2004). The co-occurrence of ASD and anxiety (Duvekot et al., Reference Duvekot, van der Ende, Constantino, Verhulst and Greaves-Lord2016) and ASD and ADHD (Musser et al., Reference Musser, Hawkey, Kachan-Liu, Lees, Roullet, Goddard and Nigg2014; van Steijn et al., Reference van Steijn, Richards, Oerlemans, de Ruiter, van Aken, Franke and Rommelse2012) may be partly explained by their shared familial transmission, indicating overlap in familial risk. On the whole, neuroticism is the only Big Five trait to be consistently associated with both ASD and ADHD traits, while conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness to experience showed inconsistent results. Moreover, neuroticism (or negative emotionality) has been proposed to represent a non-specific vulnerability factor that underlies and shares genetic overlap with multiple dimensions of psychopathology (Tackett, et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013). Neuroticism could represent an example of general predisposition to psychopathology, influencing the development and severity of ASD, ADHD, and other psychological and psychiatric outcomes (Constantino, Reference Constantino2017) and so be a non-trait or non-diagnostic specific susceptibility factor for general psychopathology. At present, molecular studies have shown no evidence of pleiotropy between neuroticism and the ADHD (Gale et al., Reference Gale, Hagenaars, Davies, Hill, Liewald, Cullen and Smith2016)—and there is no data available about neuroticism and ASD. The contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the comorbidity of neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD has not been investigated.
A previous study conducted with a sample partially overlapping with the one used in the present study (Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008) suggested that in young adults, a substantial overlap of the genetic influences on autistic traits and self-reported inattention symptoms of ADHD may lead to their frequent co-occurrence. However, this study was conducted in a small sample size (N = 674). Even though they were not detected, we were unable to draw conclusions about common environmental effects, genetic dominance, sex-limitation effects, and sibling-interaction effects because of limited power. In addition to this, it is still unclear whether these findings on the high genetic correlation between self-rated ASD and ADHD traits (r g = 0.72) is largely driven by neuroticism.
The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationship between neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD traits (inattentive and impulsive symptoms). Using a large sample (N = 3,170), we sought to: (1) replicate our prior work (Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008) showing the evidence for substantial genetic correlation between ASD and ADHD traits, using a larger sample; and (2) examine the degree to which the co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD traits could be explained by shared genetic and environmental risk factors in common with neuroticism.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and Procedure
The data reported here included an unselected population-based sample of 3,202 twins born between 1962 and 1987 enrolled in the Australian Twin Registry as part of a study focused on cannabis and other substance use (Lynskey et al., Reference Lynskey, Heath, Nelson, Bucholz, Madden, Slutske and Martin2002).
After excluding individual participants with missing items (n = 10) or unknown zygosity (n = 22), the sample comprised a total of 3,170 twins (1,081 complete pairs), including 371 monozygotic (MZ) female, 154 MZ male, 278 dizygotic (DZ) female, 95 DZ males, and 183 opposite-sex (OS) twin pairs. However, single twins from incomplete pairs were used in the Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) analysis to better estimate the sample distribution of means, variances, and thresholds. Zygosity was ascertained by questionnaire. Mean age at completion was 32.31 (SD = 2.5). Informed consent was obtained from participants. This study was approved by the QIMR Berghofer Human Research Ethics Committee, the Institutional Review Board at Washington University School of Medicine, and the Australian Twin Registry Ethics Committee.
Measures
Participants completed an online or paper-and-pencil survey on health and well-being, including the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992), for the measure of personality, a modified short version of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino & Gruber, Reference Constantino and Gruber2005), and the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Adler, Ames, Demler, Faraone, Hiripi and Ustun2005), which were designed to minimize subject burden and improve data quality.
Neo Five-Factor Inventory-Neuroticism (NEO-FFI-N)
Neuroticism was measured by summing 12 items of the Neuroticism domain of the short form of the Neo-FFI, which consists of a total of 60 items (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992). Each NEO-FFI-N item was scored on a 5-point scale (0 = strongly disagree; 1 = disagree; 2 = neutral; 3 = agree; 4 = strongly agree) with total scores ranging from 0 to 48. Cronbach's alpha for this scale was 0.89, indicating high inter-item reliability.
The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS)
The SRS (Constantino & Gruber, Reference Constantino and Gruber2005) is a 65-item questionnaire based on the DSM-IV and designed to assess autistic traits. Each SRS item is scored on a 4-point scale (0 = false, not at all true; 1 = slightly true; 2 = mainly true; 3 = very true). In the present study, the total number of items was limited to 11 items, with total sum scores ranging from 0 to 33. The modified version of SRS included the items that measure the autistic symptoms the three DSM-IV autism domains: Social Cognition (SRS items 15, 42, and 58), Social Communication (SRS items 16, 18, 35, and 37), Social Motivation (SRS item 6), and Autistic Mannerisms (SRS items 24, 29, and 39). These 11 items had strong loadings on the first unrotated factor of a principal components analysis of the SRS in a pediatric sample (Constantino et al., Reference Constantino, Przybeck, Friesen and Todd2000). Cronbach's alpha for SRS was 0.83, indicating high inter-item reliability at least with child psychiatric patients.
Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS)
The ASRS (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Adler, Ames, Demler, Faraone, Hiripi and Ustun2005) is a widely used short screening tool for ADHD. It includes 18 items that references the DSM-IV ADHD criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The modified version of ADHD included the 12 items that seemed most applicable to this age group (9 inattentive and 3 impulsive symptoms). We did not include the ASRS items related to the DSM-IV hyperactive symptoms, because these symptoms tended to have less stability than inattentive symptoms in this age group (Todd et al., Reference Todd, Huang, Todorov, Neuman, Reiersen, Henderson and Reich2008). Similar to SRS items, each ASRS item is scored on a 4-point scale (0 = false, not at all true; 1 = slightly true; 2 = mainly true; 3 = very true) with total sum scores ranging from 0 to 36. This questionnaire also demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.84).
Total raw scores were calculated for each questionnaire by summing the scores of the relevant items. Higher scores indicated greater severity. Examination of the distribution revealed that the SRS and ASRS data were highly skewed (skewness = 1.97 for SRS and 1.22. for ASRS). Hence, we used a threshold model under the assumption of an underlying continuous liability distribution with the thresholds defining categories. A simulation study by Derks and colleagues (Reference Derks, Dolan and Boomsma2004) showed that this is the best approach when analyzing L-shaped distributed phenotypic data as it produced unbiased parameter estimates. Otherwise, the skewness in the data leads to biases in parameter estimates, including underestimation of the shared environmental factors and overestimation of the unique environmental factors.
These continuous scores were clustered in rank-ordered groups of individuals. The number of categories was chosen in such way that the number of subjects in each category was roughly similar, in order to avoid small cell size. NEO-FFI-N scores had only modest skew (skewness = 0.44) and, therefore, did not require transformation. Figure 1 shows the distributions by sex for the three variables.
Statistical Analyses
Initially, a saturated model in which all the parameters are allowed to be free was used to test hypotheses concerning means, variances (or thresholds), and covariance in the analysis of individual observations for twin pairs for the three variables (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992).
The results from the saturated models were used in designing a univariate modeling. In the present study, we applied the classical twin design (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992) and implemented structural equation modeling using OpenMx (Boker et al., Reference Boker, Neale, Maes, Wilde, Spiegel, Brick and Mehta2011) to estimate the amount of the variances in scores due to additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), dominant genetic (D), and unique environmental (E) factors. However, these components cannot be estimated simultaneously, since dominant genetic and shared environmental components are negatively confounded (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Eaves, Kearsey and Davies1978). Therefore, we chose to model either an ACE or an ADE, based on the pattern of twin correlations. MZ twins share 100% of their genes, and DZ twins share 50% of their genes. If the MZ correlation is more, than twice the DZ twin correlation, we used the ADE model as the MZ correlation indicates the presence of the genetic effects due to dominance over additive genetic influences. For MZ correlations less than twice the DZ correlations, we used the ACE model as the presence of both A and C components are indicated.
We obtained the nested sub-models by dropping A or C (or D) or both parameters. The fit of each sub-model was assessed by the difference in log likelihood between the sub and full models. If sub-model fit was significantly worse than the full model, the full model was accepted. Otherwise, the sub-model was used as the most parsimonious model.
Next, a trivariate Cholesky decomposition model was fitted to estimate the magnitude of genetic and environmental influences that mediate the phenotypic covariation between NEO-FFI-N, SRS, and ASRS scores. The choice of parameters for this trivariate Cholesky decomposition model was based on the results from the univariate modeling.
Results
First, a saturated model was used to check whether means and variances or thresholds are equal across different zygosity groups, and for the presence of genetic and sex-limitation effects as well as age and sex effect differences. No differences in means, variances, or thresholds were found across birth order and zygosity in any of the variables, except for NEO-FFI-N, where there was an apparent difference of means across like-sex MZ (24.8 for males, 20.8 for females) and DZ twins (23.6 for males and 22.8 for females) in the assumption testing (Δχ2 = 9.49, Δdf = 2, p < .009). In the structural equation modeling, we allowed the means to vary across zygosity groups (MZ and DZ, including opposite sex twins) in the full model and tested if there was a significant difference of fit with a model where the means were equated. Since there were no significant differences, we assumed equal means across zygosity for the rest of the analyses. For SRS and ASRS, difference in thresholds were found for sex (Δχ2 = 15.47, 25.35, Δdf = 3, p < .001). Therefore, sex was modeled as a covariate for these two variables. In addition, the effect of age was also controlled on SRS (Δχ2 = 6.58, Δdf = 1, p < .01). There was a significant difference (Δχ2 = 78.82, 80.51, 74.01, Δdf = 1, p = <.001, <.001, <.001), when we constrained all twin covariances to zero, indicating for all three variables the presence of familial aggregation. For all three variables, we found significant differences in MZ and DZ twin correlations (Δχ2 = 8.99, 6.03, 7.95, Δdf = 1, p < .003, <.01, <.005), indicating that NEO-FFI-N, SRS, and ASRS are influenced by genetic factors. There was no indication for a sex-limitation model for any of the variables.
FIML estimates of twin correlations are presented in Table 1. The correlations between MZ twins are significantly greater than DZ twins (see above). However, the MZ correlations were less than twice as high as the DZ correlations, suggesting that the covariance between them might be influenced by genes and by shared environmental influences. Thus, an ACE model was fitted to the data in two zygosity groups (MZ and DZ).
Note: NEO-FFI-N = Neo Five-Factor Inventory-Neuroticism; SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale; ASRS = Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale.
We fitted univariate models to disentangle the sources of variance of the three phenotypes. As shown in Table 2, our univariate analysis for each score suggested that the most parsimonious univariate models were those with the additive genetic (A) and environmental (E) components. Additive genetic factors accounted for 45% (95% CI [37, 51]) of the variance of SRS scores, 41% (95% CIs [33, 48]) of ASRS scores, and 41% (95% CI [34, 47]) of the variance of NEO-FFI-N scores, whereas unique environmental influences accounted for the remaining variance in each score. The common environmental factor (C) could be dropped from our ACE model with no significant deterioration in fit. Hence, we only included A and E in our trivariate Cholesky decomposition model (Figure 2).
Note: Bold values indicate best-fitting models. A = additive genetic factors; C = common environmental factors; E = unique environmental factors; -2LL = twice negative log-likelihood; Δ-2LL = difference in -2LL; df = degrees of freedom; Δdf = difference in degrees of freedom. Models compared with either the ACE or ADE model where the model with the smallest AIC value selected.
Figure 2 shows the parameter estimates from the trivariate Cholesky decomposition model with standardized path coefficients. Following the order of the variables, a common genetic factor shared (A1) accounted for all the genetic variance (41% of the total variance) in NEO-FFI-N, half of the genetic variance (22% of the total variance) in SRS, and one-third of the genetic variance (13% of the total variance) in ASRS. A further 50% of the genetic variance (22% of the total variance) in SRS and 10% of the genetic variance (4% of the total variance) in ASRS can be explained by the genetic factor (A2) shared with SRS and ASRS. The genetic factor not shared with NEO-FFI-N and SRS accounted for the remaining genetic variance (25% of the total variance) in ASRS. On the other hand, all the unique environmental variance (59% of the total variance) in NEO-FFI-N, 30% of the unique environmental variance (17% of the total variance) in SRS, and 14% of the unique environmental variance (8% of the total variance) in ASRS could be explained by overlapping unique environmental factor (E1) shared with NEO-FFI-N, SRS, and ASRS. In addition to this, 70% of the unique environmental variance (38% of the total variance) in SRS and 14% of the unique environmental variance (8% of the total variance) in ASRS can be further explained by shared unique environmental factors (E2) between with SRS and ASRS. Over 70% of the environmental influence (42% of the total variance) on ASRS scores were not shared with NEO-FFI-N and SRS scores (E3).
As shown in Table 3, the phenotypic correlation between NEO-FFI-N and SRS scores was moderate (r p = 0.62, 95% CI [0.62, 0.64]). About 49% of this phenotypic correlation was driven by strong shared genetic influence (r g = 0.71, 95% CI [0.62, 0.74]), with the remaining covariance explained by unique environmental factors (r e = 0.55, 95% CI [0.49, 0.61]). There was also a weak to moderate phenotypic correlation between NEO-FFI-N and ASRS (r p = 0.45, 95% CI [0.43, 0.49]) and a moderate phenotypic correlation between SRS and ASRS (r p = 0.56, 95% CI [0.55, 0.58]). Similar to the phenotypic correlation between NEO-FFI-N and SRS scores, about 52% of the correlation between NEO-FFI-N and ASRS was explained by genetic influences (r g = 0.56, 95% CI [0.53, 0.60]), with the remaining covariance explained by unique environment (r e = 0.37, 95% CI [0.30, 0.44]). For the phenotypic correlation between SRS and ASRS, about 49% of this correlation was accounted for by a moderate degree of overlapping genetic and unique environmental influences (r g = 0.63, 95% CIs [0.62, 0.72]; r e = 0.51, 95% CI [0.50, 0.58]).
Discussion
In the present study, we sought to examine the role of neuroticism in the correlation between ASD and ADHD symptoms by decomposing the variance of ADHD into influences shared with neuroticism and ASD and those shared only with ASD. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that has explored the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to neuroticism and the causal overlap between ASD and ADHD traits using a twin study. Consistent with previous twin studies in adults (Hansell et al., Reference Hansell, Wright, Medland, Davenport, Wray, Martin and Hickie2012; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner, Gatz and Pedersen2007; Lundstrom et al., Reference Lundström, Chang, Kerekes, Gumpert, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2011; Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Hoekstra, Posthuma and Larsson2014; Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008), our heritability estimates for neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD traits were moderate. Moreover, we did not find any significant evidence for common environment effects, genetic dominance, or sex limitation effects. We observed a low correlation between male DZ twins for ADHD traits (r = 0.02; 95% CI [-0.21, 0.24]). This finding concurs with previous observations consistent with the presence of sibling contrast effects that might inflate heritability estimates (Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008), although we could not test for sibling contrasts, and dominance or stochastic effects cannot be discarded.
The results of the trivariate twin analyses showed that the genetic factor whose primary effect is on neuroticism (A1 in Figure 2) also has a significant effect on the variance of ASD and ADHD. This factor may reflect a susceptibility to negative emotional states (i.e., anger, fear, anxiety, depression, and stress). It accounted for 41% of the variance in neuroticism, 22% of the variance in the ASD traits, and 13% of the variance in the ADHD traits. The second genetic factor (A2 in Figure 2), which is independent of neuroticism, also accounted for 22% of the variance in ASD and 4% of the ADHD. The third genetic factor (A3 in Figure 2) that is specific to ADHD accounted for 25% of the variance of its variance. Interestingly, the first genetic factor (A1) shared by neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD traits showed a similar degree of genetic contribution to ASD traits as the second genetic factor (A2) that is independent of neuroticism. This factor also showed a higher degree of genetic contribution to ADHD traits, while the second genetic factor (A2) accounted for only 4% of the variance in ADHD traits, indicating that the common genetic factor is the largest contributor to the comorbidity between the co-occurrence of ASD and ADHD traits.
In contrast, the first unique environmental factor (E1 in Figure 2) shared by neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD traits (A1 in Figure 2) contributed little to the variance (8%) in ADHD traits while they accounted for 59% of the variance in neuroticism and 17% of the variance in ASD traits. The second unique environmental factor (E2 in Figure 2) that is independent of neuroticism, also accounted for 38% of the variance in ASD traits and 8% of the variance in ADHD traits. Most of the variance (42%) of ADHD traits was influenced by unique environmental factor (E3 in Figure 2) specific to ADHD traits. The moderate cross-loading is also consistent with most of E being measurement error.
The genetic correlation (r g) between ASD and ADHD traits reported by previous studies (Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008, using a sample partially overlapping with the sample used in the current study (Lundstrom et al., Reference Lundström, Chang, Kerekes, Gumpert, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2011; Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Hoekstra, Posthuma and Larsson2014) were also very similar to ours (r g ~0.60). Their studies also reported a similar unique environmental correlation between ASD and ADHD traits (approximately half the variance of r p). A recent large study (Gale et al, Reference Gale, Hagenaars, Davies, Hill, Liewald, Cullen and Smith2016) investigated the evidence of pleiotropy between neuroticism and physical and mental health related traits and found no single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) genetic correlation between neuroticism and ADHD, indicating insufficient power to detect correlation between common genetic variants or that rare genetic variants shared between family members may contribute more to the variance in neuroticism and ADHD traits.
Apart from a genetic correlation, we found slightly higher unique environmental correlations between ASD and ADHD traits (r e = 0.51; 95% CI [0.50, 0.58]) than those previously reported unique environmental correlations (r e) of 0.22–0.40 between ASD and ADHD traits (Lundstrom et al., Reference Lundström, Chang, Kerekes, Gumpert, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2011; Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Hoekstra, Posthuma and Larsson2014; Reiersen et al., Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008). We also identified overlapping unique environmental factors related to all traits or only common to ASD and ADHD traits, independent of neuroticism. This suggests that environmental factors might be almost as important as genetic factors to understand the development of comorbidity among neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD traits. It could also reflect the lack of insight, the under-reporting of symptomatology, and the lower accuracy of self-report associated with ADHD (Barkley et al., Reference Barkley, Fischer, Smallish and Fletcher2002; Sibley et al., Reference Sibley, Pelham, Molina, Gnagy, Waxmonsky, Waschbusch and Kuriyan2012). It is, however, important to note that unique environmental factors may also include measurement error biases.
This study has a number of limitations. First, our measures were based solely on self-reports commonly used in adult population studies. Several lines of evidence reported that adult self-report scales may produce lower heritability estimates in comparison to those typically used in child population studies, such as parental, teacher, or other outside observer ratings (Franke et al., Reference Franke, Faraone, Asherson, Buitelaar, Bau, Ramos-Quiroga and Lesch2012). Second, we used the shortened version of SRS and ASRS questionnaires to minimize subject burden and improve compliance. As a result, autistic and ADHD traits constructed in this present study may be different to those constructed in other studies. Reduction in the number of items used from the SRS is known to diminish the ability to distinguish ASD from general psychopathology (Constantino & Gruber, Reference Constantino and Gruber2012; Constantino et al., Reference Constantino, Hudziak and Todd2003; Frazier et al., Reference Frazier, Ratliff, Gruber, Zhang, Law and Constantino2014). In addition, our ADHD symptom measure did not include DSM-IV hyperactive symptoms, thus only reflecting inattention and impulsiveness. Third, the distributions of our SRS and ADHD scores were skewed. Derks and colleagues (Reference Derks, Dolan and Boomsma2004) reported that skewed data could lead to overestimation of non-shared environmental effects and underestimation of shared environmental effects. However, as suggested in their study, we employed the liability threshold model to minimize these parameter estimation biases. Fourth, we obtained our data from a non-clinical population in Australia to increase the representative of our findings and avoid biases inherent in clinic-based studies, such as greater comorbidity and biased gender ratio. However, caution is required as our findings may be different in clinical populations or populations in other countries.
Overall, our findings indicate shared genetic and unique environmental influences among neuroticism, ASD, and ADHD symptoms. These findings not only support evidence of a moderate genetic association between ASD and ADHD traits but indicate that this correlation could be partially explained by etiological factors that these conditions have in common with neuroticism. Our results could reflect that non-disease-specific risk can compound disease-specific risk in the accumulation of susceptibility to a specific psychiatric disease or variation (Constantino, Reference Constantino2017; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013).
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to the Australian twin participants and project managers for data preparation.
Financial Support
This research was funded by National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) grant: DA18267 (ML) and by NHMRC grants to NGM. Analysis was partially supported by the NHMRC project grant (1043664) to AJG. The research was facilitated through access to the Australian Twin Registry. Twins Research Australia receives support from the National Health and Medical Research Council through a Centre of Research Excellence Grant, which is administered by the University of Melbourne. AJG is funded by the NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (APP1061922). LCC was supported by the QIMR Berghofer Fellowship.
Conflict of Interest
None.