Tackling stigma and inequalities in health is a major UK government objective. 1,Reference Darzi2 Stigma is a social construction that devalues people because of a distinguishing characteristic or mark. Reference Biernat, Dovidio, Heatherton, Kleck and Hebl3 The World Health Organization (WHO) and World Psychiatric Association (WPA) recognise that the stigma attached to mental disorders is strongly associated with suffering, disability and poverty. Reference Corrigan, Markowitz, Watson, Rowan and Kubiak4 Stigma is also a major barrier to treatment-seeking behaviour. Reference Appleby5 Many studies show that negative attitudes towards the mentally ill are widespread. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Goddard and Meltzer6 Furthermore, the media generally depicts people who are mentally ill as violent, erratic and dangerous. Reference Granello, Pauley and Carmichael7 There have been several attempts to reduce the stigma of mental illness including the UK Royal College of Psychiatrists’ ‘Changing Minds’ campaign, 8 the current ‘See me’ campaign in Scotland (www.seemescotland.org.uk) and ‘Time to Change’ campaign in England (www.time-to-change.org.uk).
Society's neglect and ill treatment of people with intellectual disability (also known as learning disability in UK health services) is infamous. Reference Sperlinger, O'Hara and Sperlinger9,Reference O'Hara10 Various organisations report that people with intellectual disability encounter stigma, prejudice and suffer continued denial of their human rights. 11,12 However, very little research is published on the stigma of intellectual disability, although a recent paper describes a new instrument for measuring the stigma experienced by people with intellectual disability and describes the current situation. Reference Ali, Strydom, Hassiotis, Williams and King13 However, the proportion of research reports on intellectual disability was consistently less than any other diagnostic category in a survey of five high impact psychiatric journals. Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel14 In a Mencap survey of 5000 people with intellectual disability, over 80% had been bullied in the previous year, a third on a weekly basis, a half reported verbal abuse and a quarter reported physical assaults. 15 People with intellectual disability were twice as likely to be victims of crime. 16 Inequalities in healthcare were identified by an investigation conducted by the Disability Rights Commission in the UK and Mencap's Death by Indifference report. 11 The UK Government report, Valuing People, aims to counter these problems and improve the lives of people with intellectual disability by ensuring that services respect their rights, choices, independence and social inclusion, and ensuring their access to mainstream services. 17
There is a view that a disfigured or ‘dysmorphic’ facial appearance may further stigmatise people. Reference Houston and Bull18-Reference Kaney, Mason, Carlisle, Watkins and Whitehead20 By comparison, marketing strategies for commercial products invariably associate their product with positive images and avoid associating it with any negative images. Reference Atkinson, Atkinson, Smith, Bem and Nolen-Hoeksema21-Reference Lin and Yeh27 Hence our study looked at the effect of viewing pictures of people with intellectual disability and dysmorphic facial features on stigmatised attitudes.
Method
Participants
We recruited 250 participants from the UK general population using direct mail shots and advertisements in local newspapers as described in a previous study. Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher28 A total of 125 participants per group were approached with the aim of generating approximately 100 responses per group.
Instruments
The 5-item Attitude to Mental Illness Questionnaire (AMIQ) is a brief, self-completion questionnaire. Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher28,Reference Cunningham, Sobell and Chow29 Respondents read a short vignette describing an imaginary patient and answered five questions (Appendix). Individual questions were scored on a 5-point Likert scale (maximum +2, minimum −2) with blank questions, ‘neutral’ and ‘don't know’ scored zero. The total score for each vignette ranged between −10 and +10. The AMIQ has been shown to have good psychometric properties in a sample of over 800 members of the UK general public (one component accounted for 80.2% of the variance; test-retest reliability was 0.702 (Pearson's correlation coefficient); alternate test-reliability v. Corrigan's attribution questionnaire was 0.704 (Spearman's rank correlation Rho); Cronbach's alpha was 0.93). Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher28 Other research we have undertaken has shown a 2-unit difference between the stigma scores of pharmacists who were prepared to dispense methadone to people dependent on opiates and pharmacies who did not Reference Luty, Kumar and Stagias30 - the positive predictive value was 77% using a cut-off AMIQ score of 0. Hence the AMIQ scores are able to predict discrimination by people towards those with mental illness in a real-world situation.
Procedure
Participants were randomised using the randomisation function of the Stats Direct statistical package (version 2.4) for Windows. The ‘bad photo’ group were asked to look at a photograph of a man with intellectual disability from the cover of the Learning Disability Coalition leaflet entitled Tell it Like it is. 31 The photo shows the face of a man with dysmorphic features, a partial ptosis of one eye and a mild skin condition visible on his face and chin, wearing a casual outfit. Participants were then asked to complete the AMIQ stigma questionnaire with the following description: ‘Oliver has Down's syndrome. He is 32 years old and lives with his parents. He cannot read or write but he is happy and cheerful and keen to help people’ (Appendix). The ‘good photo’ group were asked to look at a photo of a man with intellectual disability who was smartly dressed in a shirt and tie apparently at work in an office (see August 2010 issue of The Psychiatrist). Participants were then asked to complete the AMIQ stigma questionnaire.
Data analysis
Randomisation, correlation coefficients and non-parametric (Mann-Whitney) tests were used to generate and compare differences in the two groups using the Stats Direct statistical software package (version 2.4) for Windows.
Results
Questionnaires were received from 187 participants (response rate 74%). Both groups were closely comparable on demographic data. For the bad photo group (n = 82), the mean age was 51 years (s.e. = 1.8), 46% were male and 59% in paid employment. For the good photo group (n = 105), the mean age was 54 years (s.e. = 1.5), 40% were male and 56% in paid employment. Over 90% of both groups described their ethnic group as White British.
The mean AMIQ stigma score for the bad photo group was 1.3 (s.e. = 0.3, median 1, interquartile range (IQR) = 0-3, n = 82). The mean AMIQ score for the good photo group was 2.8 (s.e. = 0.3, median 3, IQR = 1-5, n = 105). The difference in AMIQ stigma score was highly significant (two-sided P = 0.0001, median difference 2; Mann-Whitney U-test).
Discussion
This study shows that an attractive photograph of a person with intellectual disability significantly reduces stigmatised attitudes when compared with an unattractive photograph. Validation of the AMIQ shows that in practice the range of scores varied from −5 to +5. Furthermore, other research by our group has shown a 2-unit difference between the stigma scores of pharmacists who were prepared to dispense methadone to people dependent on opiates and pharmacies who did not. Reference Luty, Kumar and Stagias30 Hence the median difference (2 units) is likely to be worthwhile in practice.
In another study we found that a good (attractive) photo produced a mean AMIQ score of 2.43 (s.e. = 0.12, n = 174). Reference Varughese and Luty32 This is closely comparable with the good photo group in the current report (mean AMIQ score 2.8). A control group that had no photo attached produced an AMIQ score of 1.56 (s.e. = 0.21, n = 186). This is closely comparable to the bad photo result from the current report (mean AMIQ score 1.3). This suggests that a bad photo is better than no photo at all, whereas an attractive photo produces a highly significantly positive response.
Stigmatised attitudes are widely reported towards people as a result of disfigurement or dysmorphic facial appearance. Reference Houston and Bull18-Reference Kaney, Mason, Carlisle, Watkins and Whitehead20 By contrast, presenting individuals in a positive manner can significantly reduce the stigma of alcoholism, although the effect found for schizophrenia was negligible. Reference Luty, Rao, Arokiadass, Easow and Sarkhel33,Reference Corrigan and Penn34 Looking at an attractive picture of a man with Down syndrome actually reduced stigmatised attitudes and this is supported by most marketing strategies that invariably recommend associating a product with a successful, physically attractive individual rather than an unappealing image. Reference Atkinson, Atkinson, Smith, Bem and Nolen-Hoeksema21-Reference Lin and Yeh27
Methods to reduce the stigma of mental illness
The Royal College of Psychiatrists’ ‘Changing Minds’ campaign aimed to promote positive images of mental illness, challenge misrepresentations and discrimination and educate the public to the real nature and treatability of mental disorder. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Goddard and Meltzer6 Crisp et al's large survey showed that people with substance use disorders were the most stigmatised of all those with mental disorder. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Goddard and Meltzer6 More recently, national anti-stigma campaigns have been launched in Scotland (‘See me’) and England (‘Time to Change’), although unfortunately there have been reports that national anti-stigma campaigns are not particularly effective. Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel14,Reference Paykel, Hart and Priest35,Reference Mehta, Kassam, Leese, Butler and Thornicroft36 These reports discuss the disappointing results to date from the ‘Defeat Depression’, the ‘Changing Minds’ and the Scottish ‘See me’ campaign. Although a significant amount of work has been undertaken on the stigmatising effect of a diagnosis of schizophrenia, very little research has looked at the stigmatised attitudes directed against people with intellectual disability. Reference Corrigan, Markowitz, Watson, Rowan and Kubiak4,Reference O'Hara10,Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel14,Reference Penn, Kommana, Mansfield and Link37 For example, the stigma of intellectual disability is not addressed by either the ‘Changing Minds’ or ‘Time to Change’ campaigns or the UK government publication Action on Mental Health. 1 Methods of dealing with stigma are not addressed in the UK Department of Health report, Valuing People, which specifically concerns people with intellectual disability. 17
Action on Mental Health 1 provides 12 individual fact sheets to reduce stigma. This supplements the efforts of the ‘Changing Minds’ campaign. Both give practical advice to health agencies, employers and stakeholders to tackle stigma. Providing factual information in brief fact sheets Reference Penn, Guynan, Daily, Spaulding, Garbin and Sullivan38,Reference Thornton and Wahl39 or through extensive interventions such as educational courses has been reported to reduce stigma. Reference Corrigan and Penn34,Reference Mayville and Penn40-Reference Knox, Smith and Hereby42 Unfortunately, responses tend to be small, especially if negative consequences of mental illness are also disseminated. Knox et al showed that addressing stigmatised attitudes to mental illness among four million members of the US armed forces with mandatory training to recognise and treat mental illness significantly reduced suicide rates but not stigmatised attitudes. Reference Knox, Smith and Hereby42 Pinfold et al reported a project in which 472 English secondary school children attended mental health awareness workshops. Reference Pinfold, Toulmin, Thornicroft, Farmer, Huxley and Graham43 Overall, there was a small but positive shift in their understanding of mental illness. However, it was possible to argue that in both these settings participants engaged in anti-stigma training, whereas any involvement by the general public is entirely voluntary.
Penn et al reported a study of 163 US undergraduates who were assigned randomly to four groups: three watched a documentary about schizophrenia (represented realistically), polar bears or being overweight, and the fourth group was a ‘no video’ control group. Reference Penn, Chamberlin and Mueser44 The schizophrenia documentary did not change attitudes. Depicting the negative consequences of schizophrenia may be realistic but may not be the best way to reduce stigma. Depicting a success story may be more effective, although viewers may then classify this as an exception to the rule. Reference Corrigan and Penn34 By contrast another study showed that presenting individuals who had recovered from alcoholism in a positive manner can significantly reduce the stigma of alcoholism, however the effects for schizophrenia were negligible. Reference Luty, Rao, Arokiadass, Easow and Sarkhel33 Luty et al also found that a brief face-to-face intervention (motivational interviewing) helped to reduce the stigma of alcoholism, although the effect was modest. Reference Luty, Umoh and Nuamah45
Promoting direct interpersonal contact with people who are mentally ill may be an effective strategy, but the amount of contact required has not been established. Reference Corrigan and Penn34,Reference Penn, Guynan, Daily, Spaulding, Garbin and Sullivan38,Reference Pinfold, Toulmin, Thornicroft, Farmer, Huxley and Graham43,Reference Wolff, Pathare, Craig and Leff46 It would be difficult, in practice, to ensure that a significant proportion of the public had contact with people with a severe mental illness. Our report indicates that looking at pictures of people with intellectual disability and dysmorphic facial features reduces stigmatised attitudes significantly and this may act as an effective substitute for direct contact.
Strengths and limitations
The AMIQ was used in this project as it is convenient and has been well validated. Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher28,Reference Cunningham, Sobell and Chow29,Reference Luty, Rao, Arokiadass, Easow and Sarkhel33 Other instruments are available, although these tend to be much longer, involve interviews or tend to address the experience of stigma by people with mental illness (e.g. the Internalised Stigma of Mental Illness scale Reference Ritsher, Otilingam and Grajales47).Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher28
Although there was an excess of female respondents, the age and employment status of participants were reasonably matched to that from UK census surveys. Hence the sample appears to be a reasonable cross-section of the British public. However, it is self-selecting and may not generalise across the whole population. Ideally, interviews could be conducted using a quota survey of households with repeat visits for non-responders. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Goddard and Meltzer6 Unfortunately this is prohibitively expensive.
The study presented a hypothetical person with intellectual disability, as, it was not possible to measure stigmatised behaviour towards real people with intellectual disability. Moreover, the written views and expressed attitudes may not translate into any enduring behavioural change. Although there was no direct contact between participants and researchers, participants are likely to make some assumptions about the potentially liberal beliefs of researchers. Hence social desirability bias may affect the results. However, the results from other similar studies demonstrated a negative view of people with active substance use disorder and suggest that participants had little reservation about indicating their disapproval of these disorders. Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher28,Reference Luty, Rao, Arokiadass, Easow and Sarkhel33 This is confirmed in other reports. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Goddard and Meltzer6 This would indicate that social disability bias had only a modest effect. Furthermore, social desirability bias would affect both groups equally.
Funding
The research was supported by Southend MIND.
Appendix
Attitude to Mental Illness Questionnaire (AMIQ)Reference Luty, Fakuda, Umoh and Gallagher 28, Reference Cunningham, Sobell and Chow 29
Participants were presented with either photo A (good photo) or photo B (bad photo).
This is a fictitious report. This is Oliver.
‘Oliver has Down's syndrome. He is 32 years old and lives with his parents. He cannot read or write but he is happy and cheerful and keen to help people’. Please underline the answer which best reflects your views:
Do you think that this would damage Oliver's career?
Strongly agree-2/Agree-1/Neutral0/Disagree+1/Strongly disagree+2/Don't know0
I would be comfortable if Oliver was my colleague at work?
Strongly agree+2 / Agree+1 / Neutral0 / Disagree-1 / Strongly disagree-2 / Don't know0
I would be comfortable about inviting Oliver to a dinner party?
Strongly agree+2 / Agree+1 / Neutral0 / Disagree-1 / Strongly disagree-2 / Don't know0
How likely do you think it would be for Oliver's wife to leave him?
Very likely-2 / Quite likely-1 / Neutral0 / Unlikely+1 / Very unlikely+2 / Don't know0
How likely do you think it would be for Oliver to get in trouble with the law?
Very likely-2 / Quite likely-1 / Neutral0 / Unlikely+1 / Very unlikely+2 / Don't know0
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.