Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T03:48:54.777Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How do supervisee's learn during supervision? A Grounded Theory study of the perceived developmental process

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 March 2012

Lynne H. Johnston*
Affiliation:
Halley Johnston Associates Ltd, Tyne & Wear, UK
Derek L. Milne
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Ridley Building, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
*
*Author for correspondence: L. H. Johnston, Principal Psychologist, Sunderland Royal Hospital, City Hospitals Sunderland NHS Foundation Trust, Sunderland SR4 7TP, UK. (email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To contribute to a model of CBT supervision, we interpreted supervisees' understanding of the processes involved in their receipt of supervision. Second, we assessed the utility of a Grounded Theory Methodology (GTM) to study supervision. Supervisees were interviewed about their experiences of supervision, within a cross-sectional, qualitative design. In-depth, face-to-face individual interviews were conducted with seven trainee clinical psychologists. Their perceptions of supervision were analysed by means of a constructivist revision of GTM. A conceptual model is presented, to show the learning process from the perspective of the supervisees. This suggests that the receipt of supervision was experienced against a developmental backdrop involving a progression along two continua: competency and awareness. A set of core processes (Reflection, Socratic Information Exchange, Scaffolding, Supervisory Alliance) were thought to interact, enabling appropriate learning across developmental stages. This was thought to facilitate movement through individualized Zones of Proximal Development (ZPD). The fidelity construct of ‘receipt’ is complex and does not lend itself to quantification and measurement using a positivistic approach. By contrast, GTM was a useful methodology to use in this context. Further research using a similar methodology may further contribute to a model of CBT supervision.

Type
Original Research
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2012

Introduction

Supervision in cognitive-behavioural therapy (CBT) has largely been based on ‘the same processes and methods that characterize the therapy’, such as goal-setting, collaboration, and guided discovery (Padesky, Reference Padesky and Salkovskis1996, p. 289). This affords supervisors a clear and internally consistent (reflexive) guide to supervision, one that is also associated with high levels of supervisee satisfaction (Townend et al. Reference Townend, Iannetta and Freeston2002). But, like supervision in general, CBT supervision lacks adequate conceptualization (Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, Ladany, Krengel and Schult1996). This pragmatic reliance on reflexivity restricts our ability to organize data, understand phenomena, make decisions, adapt interventions, train supervisors, conduct research, etc. (Bernard & Goodyear, Reference Bernard and Goodyear2004). In short, ‘without theory or a conceptual model, one does not really understand the process of supervision’ (Hart, Reference Hart1982, p. 27).

What is therefore required is a specific model for CBT supervision, in the same way that Bennett-Levy (Reference Bennett-Levy2006) has proposed a CBT model for therapist skill development, the ‘declarative-procedural-reflective’ model (DPR), to address what he referred to as a ‘theoretical vacuum’ (p. 58). A sound model is comprehensive, drawing on a wide range of relevant data. By contrast, CBT supervision has been criticized as limited, in terms of ignoring potentially valuable ideas from parallel literatures for being purely reflexive (Milne, Reference Milne2007, Reference Milne2008). For example, it ‘neglects . . . modern educational theory’ (Pretorius, Reference Pretorius2006, p. 418). Additionally, a sound model is precise, removing vagueness or ambiguity. But CBT supervision has failed to detail the micro-skills of CBT supervision (James et al. Reference James, Milne and Morse2008), such as ‘scaffolding’ and ‘responsivity’. A further, related criterion is ‘operationality’, i.e. that a model should be specified in measurable terms – Popper's (Reference Popper1972) ‘falsifiability’ criterion. Until recently, there was no psychometrically valid, published instrument specifically designed to evaluate the CBT supervisor's competence (Milne et al. Reference Milne, Reiser, Cliffe and Raine2011). Without an instrument of this kind, it is impossible to falsify any assumptions made about CBT supervision. As detailed above, these conceptual weaknesses create numerous practical problems for CBT supervisors.

To begin to fill this vacuum, some work has been done on developing a sound model of CBT supervision. Just as Bennett-Levy (Reference Bennett-Levy2006) drew on general theory to create his model of therapist skill acquisition (i.e. information-processing theory), so James et al. (Reference James, Milne, Blackburn and Armstrong2006) drew on expert consensus to piece together an integrative model drawing on seven inter-linked concepts, such as the ‘zone of proximal development’ (Vygotsky, Reference Vygotsky1978) and the theory of experiential learning (Kolb, Reference Kolb1984). This theoretical review by James et al. (Reference James, Milne, Blackburn and Armstrong2006), has been complemented by a systematic review of the best-available evidence, designed to construct inductively a basic supervision model (Milne et al. Reference Milne, Aylott, Fitzpatrick and Ellis2008). This clarified the moderators, mediators and mechanisms of effective supervision, offering support for the James et al. (Reference James, Milne, Blackburn and Armstrong2006) model. For example, 82% of the mechanisms reported in the sampled empirical studies (mostly CBT) were consistent with Kolb's (Reference Kolb1984) theory of experiential learning. A further part of the theoretical jigsaw has been generated by experienced CBT therapists, who contributed their perceptions of what was effective in the development of CBT skills within training and supervision (Bennett-Levy et al. Reference Bennett-Levy, McManus, Westling and Fennell2009). According to their sample of 120 therapists, a combination of educational methods was required to enable skill acquisition to occur, especially modelling and role-play. This is consistent with the foregoing, although Bennett-Levy et al. (Reference Bennett-Levy, McManus, Westling and Fennell2009) organized these therapists’ perceptions within the DPR model. Similarly, an expert consensus has helped to define the general and CBT-specific competences (Roth & Pilling, Reference Roth and Pilling2008).

Explicit attempts to model CBT supervision have included at least two studies utilising the qualitative method of Grounded Theory (GT; Glaser & Strauss, Reference Glaser and Strauss1967). In the first of these, Milne et al. (Reference Milne, Pilkington, Gracie and James2003) studied 10 video tapes of CBT supervision within an informal GT approach, noting 14 themes (e.g. collaboration and conceptualization), mapping closely onto the Cognitive Therapy Scale – Revised (Blackburn et al. Reference Blackburn, James, Milne, Baker, Standart and Garland2001). In a more recent, formal GT analysis, Townend (Reference Townend2008) interviewed 16 programme directors of CBT training courses in the UK, asking for their beliefs about what made their own supervision effective and appropriate. Eight themes were extracted from these interviews, including moderating factors (e.g. organizational structure), cognitive processes (e.g. reflection and self-awareness), and supervisory skills (e.g. using techniques to develop clinical skills, such as educational role-play). Echoing the reflexive approach, Townend (Reference Townend2008, p. 9) noted that these skills ‘were the same as those at the heart of cognitive behavioural psychotherapy’, like goal-setting and guided discovery. In summary, these two GT studies indicated that CBT supervision consisted of multiple factors, ones that interacted in a dynamic way to foster the supervisee's development.

Conceptual and methodological justification

In the absence of a specific model for CBT supervision, Milne (Reference Milne2006) suggested drawing parallels between processes-outcome relationships in supervision and in therapy. One comparatively recent development within the therapy literature is the concept of treatment fidelity (e.g. Bellg et al. Reference Bellg, Borrelli, Resnick, Hecht, Minicucci, Ory, Ogedegbe, Orwig, Ernst and Czajkowski2004; Resnick et al. Reference Resnick, Bellg, Borrelli, DeFrancesco, Breger, Hecht, Sharp, Levesque, Orwig, Ernst, Ogedegbe and Czajkowski2005). Given that clinical supervision has itself been conceptualized as an intervention (Wampold & Holloway, Reference Wampold, Holloway and Watkins1997), an original and yet logical development is therefore to examine the applicability of fidelity components in supervision. Johnston et al. (Reference Johnston, Breckon, Hutchison, Dugdill, Crone and Graham2009) have emphasized intervention fidelity testing as a key methodological requirement in applied psychology interventions, while Culloty et al. (Reference Culloty, Milne and Sheikh2010) have illustrated its quantitative application within supervisor training. A first step in developing a rich qualitative understanding of each fidelity component, within the context of clinical supervision, is to explore and define each element from a variety of perspectives. The current study focuses on one component (receipt) and is restricted to one perspective (final-year trainee psychologist). Following Bellg et al. (Reference Bellg, Borrelli, Resnick, Hecht, Minicucci, Ory, Ogedegbe, Orwig, Ernst and Czajkowski2004), receipt is defined as the supervisee's learning during supervision (i.e. the mini-impacts or immediate outcomes of supervision, such as improved understanding of a patient's presenting problem). This can be distinguished from what the supervisor may be doing to facilitate that learning (termed ‘training’, such as reformulating a case); and with any subsequent transfer of the learning (‘enactment’ or generalization, such as drawing on this reformulation at the next session).

The majority of clinical psychology supervision research has adopted an objectivist standpoint (e.g. Watkins, Reference Watkins1997), carrying various conceptual and methodological limitations (e.g. Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, Ladany, Krengel and Schult1996). It may be more productive to also study supervision from a constructionist stance (Charmaz, Reference Charmaz2006). This standpoint may help to clarify whether clinical supervision is the dynamic, complex, interpersonal process some believe it to be (Holloway, Reference Holloway1984; Holloway & Neufeldt, Reference Holloway and Neufeldt1995; Wampold & Holloway, Reference Wampold, Holloway and Watkins1997). Several theoretical perspectives are linked to a constructionist epistemology (see Denzin & Lincoln, Reference Denzin and Lincoln2008). The perspective which informed the current study is interpretivism (Schwandt, Reference Schwandt, Denzin and Lincoln2003). The principle investigator (PI) was critically engaged in the receipt of her own clinical supervision. Thus, the co-construction of knowledge was grounded in the experiences of both the subject and object. Grounded Theory Methodology (GTM; Charmaz, Reference Charmaz2006) was considered an appropriate approach, due to the focus on process factors (Richards & Morse, Reference Richards and Morse2007).

Aim and objectives

The aim of this study was to conceptualize, from the perspective of the supervisee, the receipt of clinical supervision. This was divided into two objectives:

  • To interpret supervisees' understanding of the processes involved in the receipt of supervision.

  • To assess the utility of a Grounded Theory Methodology (GTM) to study the receipt of supervision.

The central research question was: ‘What are the key processes involved in the receipt of clinical supervision, as defined through GTM’? In terms of novelty, the present study builds on the two identified GTM studies of CBT supervision, as noted above, by drawing on the first author's position as a supervisee (i.e. peer of the participants) and as an experienced qualitative researcher; by contrasting several supervision approaches (CBT; psychodynamic, etc.); by the use of the best-available software to facilitate interview coding; and by adhering to robust quality criteria governing the methodology (Johnston et al. Reference Johnston, Corban, Clarke and Bull1999, Reference Johnston, Breckon, Hutchison, Dugdill, Crone and Graham2009).

Method

Design

Supervisees were interviewed about their experiences of supervision within a cross-sectional, qualitative design. At the time of the interview they had received supervision within four consecutive placements over a continuous 2-year period. This supervision reflected their supervisors’ theoretical orientations, with the majority using a CBT approach (i.e. either pure CBT or ‘eclectic’ supervision), but there were also psychodynamic, systemic, cognitive-analytic and eclectic supervisors (see Table 1 for details).

Table 1. Participant's age and supervision experience at time of interview

CAT, Cognitive Analytic Therapy; MDT, Multi-Disciplinary Team.

Sampling strategy

In line with GTM, an iterative approach to data collection and analysis was followed (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. The iterative process of data collection and analysis in Grounded Theory Methodology (adapted from Johnston et al. Reference Johnston, Corban, Clarke and Bull1999, p. 268).

Participants were purposively sampled (Coyne, Reference Coyne1997) from the final year of a doctorate course in clinical psychology from a UK university. This was to ensure that the receipt phenomenon would be present in the sample. Theoretical sampling (Coyne, Reference Coyne1997) was then employed, based on the emergent thematic information.

Data collection methods

Ethical approval was gained from both the local NHS Trust and the host university. All participants signed a voluntary informed consent form and agreed to take part in one face-to-face interview. Participants (n = 7) were all female, their mean age was 26.71 years (s.d. = 2.06 years), and they had received supervision from a minimum of four clinical supervisors (see Table 1).

Data management and analysis

All interviews were digitally recorded using an Olympus WS-321M digital voice recorder. The data comprised 470.46 (χ = 67.21, s.d. = 6.86) minutes of digital recordings. Each recording was reviewed at least three times prior to each subsequent interview. This allowed modifications to the interview guide to be made in accordance with GTM. All interviews were transcribed verbatim and transcripts were returned to each participant to check for accuracy. This resulted in 66657 (χ = 9522, s.d. = 1341) text-based words for analysis. The software programme QSR-NVIVO version 8 (QSR International, 2009) was used to manage and organize the data and to facilitate the analytical process (Hutchison et al. Reference Hutchison, Johnston and Breckon2010). All digital interviews were imported into NVIVO8 with the accompanying verbatim transcripts. This helped to identify multiple levels of meaning (i.e. use of humour, sarcasm) as well as times where participants emphasized particular points (i.e. tone, pitch of voice).

The interpretation of the data involved the constant comparative method (Charmaz, Reference Charmaz2006). This involved generating initial Free Nodes in NVIVO8 and using the Coding Stripes function to aid the comparative process. Each Node had a unique name, description and memo attached. As the analysis progressed, additional Free Nodes (n = 57) were created and further Memo writing undertaken. The Memo content moved from a descriptive summary, with reflective questions, to a broader conceptual development of the node and its relationship to other nodes. The extensive use of See Also Links enabled the integration of the secondary literature within memos (Hutchison et al. Reference Hutchison, Johnston and Breckon2010). This included online articles (i.e. PDFs) and reading notes from book chapters and offline articles, which were recorded as Externals within NVIVO8 (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. NVIVO8 screen print showing the Socratic Method free node, memo and external.

As the analysis process developed, the Modeler tool was utilized to dimensionalize the data and visually represent potential relationships and sub-categories. Tree Nodes were used to represent axial coding (fracturing the data into properties and dimensions). As the central themes became clearer, selective coding focused on making specific comparisons. This involved examining Coding Stripes to identify potential relationships, and then using the Query tool to ask specific questions (see Fig. 3). The end of a GTM journey is marked by theoretical saturation (Charmaz, Reference Charmaz2006). Theoretical memos and models provided a clear audit trail of the analytical process and in documenting the decision to stop collecting further data after the seventh interview. The research diary was contained within NVIVO8; this acted as a conceptual management tool where data and literature were integrated. Critically, the research diary provided a clear record of prior assumptions, decisions made, procedures followed, questions (queries) asked, results found, and models built (see Bringer et al. Reference Bringer, Johnston and Brackenridge2004, Reference Bringer, Johnston, Brackenridge and Bryman2007). The aforementioned methodological procedures diligently followed the approach outlined in previous published examples of the use of NVIVO software within a GTM (see Bringer et al. Reference Bringer, Johnston and Brackenridge2006a, Reference Bringer, Johnston and Brackenridgeb; Hutchison et al. Reference Hutchison, Johnston and Breckon2010).

Fig. 3. NVIVO8 screen print showing the results node ‘autonomy and developmental’ with coding stripes.

Results

Figure 4 shows receipt against a developmental backdrop (Contextual Background) progressing along two continua: Competency and Awareness. A set of core processes (Reflection, Socratic Information Exchange, Scaffolding, Supervisory Alliance) interact to enable appropriate processing of receipt across developmental stages. This facilitates movement through trainees’ individualized Zones of Proximal Development (ZPD) from ‘Blissful Ignorance’ to ‘Just do it’. Figure 4 cannot fully convey the complexity of the processes involved. Therefore, the following narrative illuminates these processes and quotations are used to exemplify key points. Relevant themes (NODES) are italicized in the Results section.

Fig. 4. The developmental progression of the receipt of clinical supervision.

The importance of the supervisory alliance

Trainees talked about the importance of the supervisory alliance throughout the developmental process. It was especially significant during earlier stages, where trainees reported that they would elicit a deeper level of supervision, if they were honest and open in their reflections about their clinical practice. Notably, trainees reported they were more able to do this when their supervisors also adopted an open, honest and inquisitive stance, because this facilitated the development of mutual trust and empathy:

I think, having someone else saying ‘if I was working with them I'd find it hard’, and you feel confident enough to open up say ‘that was a nightmare session’. And then you take more from it; 'cos they can tell you more about it and then you've got more to go away with.   (Participant 2)

The supervisees reported that they were not open and honest about their needs when they perceived the supervisory alliance to be weak and/or when they feel unsafe. They felt unsafe when their supervisors were inconsistent and when feedback was either missing or communicated in an overly critical manner. Poor communication skills, personality factors and the imposition of an inflexible therapeutic style were all construed as factors which could damage the alliance. A weak alliance was perceived to be exacerbated by the inherent power differential associated with evaluation:

It's intrinsically quite a threatening process to go into; to be honest and open so that you can benefit, but doing that with the person that's your judge and executioner.   (Participant 5)

Several factors influenced the development of a strong alliance. Trainees needed to feel there was a minimum level of mutual respect; agreement regarding a joint responsibility for the relationship, and clear roles. This facilitated a team approach which engendered mutual understanding, and shared goals and responsibilities. Several factors influenced the respect trainees had for their supervisors. Trainees expressed a clear preference to be supervised by clinicians who they perceived as professionally and personally credible. Personal credibility included a positive evaluation of supervisors’ human qualities. This included being approachable, honest, warm, empathetic, and supervisee-centered. Professional credibility was discussed in terms of ability (i.e. as a supervisor, colleague and clinician). Critically, trainees emphasized the importance of having the opportunity to directly observe their supervisors work:

You're looking at these people to develop you, to show you, for instruction on how to do the job; and if those people can't do the job then ‘how are you going to develop me into what I want to be?’   (Participant 3)

Trainees expressed a need for professional boundaries and expressed a preference for clearly delineated supervision time and space. Supervisors who were either unboundaried or excessively rigid were perceived as lacking in professional credibility. Further, trainees described high levels of anxiety and a need to put extra time, effort and energy into trying to understand such supervisors. Notably, in each case cited it was a supervisor who had imposed a psychodynamic model (inflexibly) onto trainees:

With this placement it has been very boundaried in the sense that you can contact me if there is risk, an emergency, but supervision is one hour, a regular time, and really that's supervision, and we will stop bang on the hour, regardless of whether I'm in mid-thought, mid-conversation . . . the practical stuff I want to know, kind of sometimes gets left behind.   (Participant 4)

A Socratic approach to information exchange and scaffolding

In their earlier stages of training, trainees had less confidence, felt more anxious, needed more containment and less direct challenge. They wanted more practical and informational support and a greater amount of educational scaffolding. As they developed there was a movement away from discussions about specific skills and techniques (what to do) to a deeper collaborative discussion about inter-personal processes (how to do it). Trainees were able to look back on these earlier stages and understand their need for an increasing level of autonomy:

In my first placement, after the assessment session it was ‘what do I do next?’, and if I didn't get to see my supervisor before seeing someone again I didn't know what I was meant to be going on to. Whereas this isn't such a priority anymore, it's more about updating the supervisor after a few sessions.   (Participant 6)

A key pedagogic method discussed by all participants (irrespective of the supervisors’ theoretical orientation) was the skilful use of Socratic dialogue. In earlier developmental stages trainees reported that the Socratic approach had a guiding or eliciting tone; in later stages the Socratic dialogue felt more collaborative. Socratic dialogue was perceived to be important for several reasons. First, to enable adequate time, space, and educational scaffolding; these helped trainees to remember and process information at a deeper level because the learning process was inherently more meaningful. Second, it had a clear impact on trainees’ sense of ownership (i.e. they had made their own discoveries) and thus they were more motivated. Third it was viewed as a helpful way of regulating the trainees’ emotions within their individualized ZPD. For example, when trainees felt appropriately anxious they were able to function well; when they were too anxious they were unable to process the information being ‘transmitted’. The Socratic Method highlighted this and supervisors were able to respond and adapt accordingly.

When trainees answered questions but then received a lack of feedback this was not considered helpful or Socratic (although the supervisor may have believed they were being Socratic via their use of open questions). A compounding factor appeared to be that some supervisors failed to make things explicit. Thus, trainees were unable to ‘know what they didn't know’ or to be goal-directed in bringing what they knew into their awareness. Again the examples cited were all related to psychodynamic supervision:

If I hadn't have come with an agenda we would have sat in silence . . . So I didn't receive information, I was actively told not to read books, 'cos that was my defence to get things right . . . I do think that I learned a lot from the placement but I can't really tell you what I've learned!   (Participant 6)

The centrality of the reflective process

Trainees cited reflection as a key factor in aiding their receipt of supervision; however, this was not done in a structured, strategic, or active way:

Supervision will come back to me when I'm sitting thinking about a case formulation or I'm worried about a particular client, and I'm out walking the dog, and there will be that kind of flash of ‘ooohhh’ and it's like the pennies kind of been filtering through and reached a place where it makes sense and fits.   (Participant 5)

Nevertheless, reflection was enhanced when certain things were in place. First, the adoption of a Socratic approach with appropriate educational scaffolding facilitated their ability to understand and remember supervision because they felt more engaged in the process. Second, when trainees allowed themselves time and space immediately after their supervision session they were able to think about and process the material more effectively. Third, the emotional climate engendered within the supervisory alliance had a strong impact upon what was received and reflected upon. Trainee reflection appeared to depend on the way the information was transmitted, their ability (and willingness) to process and understand it, and the perceived salience (value judgment) attached to the information:

[CBT-based supervision] is not too infantilizing, or patronizing, it's about being collaborative, and helping you realize what you don't know; the Socratic questions and stuff like that. Whereas if it's someone who is psychodynamic and started trying to turn it into a weird therapy session. I would go along with for the [Doctoral] course but inside I'd be like, ‘what are you going on about’!   (Participant 1)

Interestingly, while the use of audio or video recordings was perceived to be useful with clients, none of the supervisees recorded their supervision sessions and therefore they were highly reliant on their supervision notes. Supervisees' supervision notes changed developmentally in several ways. First, there was a movement from practical, task-focused (aide-mémoire) notes to notes about therapeutic processes. Second, notes became more focused and concise. Third, there was an increasing recognition of the need to allow adequate reflection time to write supervision notes. Fourth, in the later developmental stages there was increasing evidence of reflection taking place within supervision sessions, as well as afterwards. Interestingly, some trainees reported that they had been specifically told by psychodynamic supervisors not to take notes. This was not perceived as helpful.

Discussion

The current study supports and extends the literature detailing an emergent model of CBT supervision. As previously reported, we found that participants’ perceived that carefully structured, collaborative, experiential work fostered their learning. Our findings also extend this small theoretical literature by emphasizing the view that the developmental context is integral to the receipt of clinical supervision (e.g. Watkins, Reference Watkins1997), and by illuminating the developmental process, especially the role of awareness in guiding experiential learning. All developmental models aim to demonstrate a progression from relative novice to accomplished therapist, and the most prominent is the Integrated Developmental Model (IDM) (Stoltenberg & Delworth, Reference Stoltenberg and Delworth1987; Stoltenberg & McNeil, Reference Stoltenberg, McNeil and Watkins1997). In line with the IDM, the current work supports the idea that trainees are able to reflect upon a developing sense of self and others; and that they work with an increased level of autonomy as they move through their relative ZPD. The dimensions in Figure 4 were initially conceptualized across two dimensions representing a relative ‘knowing’ and ‘not knowing’ position. Green (Reference Green, Fleming and Steen2004) has also proposed this sequence of skill acquisition. The four quadrants depicted in Figure 4 represent movement across these developmental stages. Quadrants 1 and 4 are easy to differentiate because they lie at opposing ends of the process dimension (represented by the central spiral of movement through the trainees ZPD). However, Quadrants 2 and 3 are more difficult to differentiate and in accordance with IDM it may be that for different tasks trainees are relatively more or less aware of their relative incompetence. The following discussion offers an explanation as to why trainees’ development may be dependent upon the complex interplay of four key constructs that allow movement to take place (Supervisory Alliance, Scaffolding, Socratic Information Exchange, Reflection).

The supervisory alliance

The importance of the supervisory alliance has been discussed by numerous authors (Bordin, Reference Bordin1983; Holloway, Reference Holloway1995; Worthen & McNeil, Reference Worthen and McNeil1996; Weaks, Reference Weaks2002; Beinart, Reference Beinart, Fleming and Steen2004). The current study also emphasizes the centrality of the supervisory alliance; particularly in earlier developmental stages, to maximize the educative and restorative potential of supervision. The supervisees explained that without a strong alliance they held back on disclosures. Previous studies have noted a relationship between non-disclosure and a weak supervisory alliance (e.g. Webb & Wheeler, Reference Webb and Wheeler1998; Ladany & Melincoff, Reference Ladany and Melincoff1999; Ladany et al. Reference Ladany, Ellis and Friedlander1999). Further, Webb & Wheeler (Reference Webb and Wheeler1998) found supervisees were less likely to make disclosures to supervisors who were imposed and those who held an assessment function. In the current study, constructs which were implicated in the development of a strong alliance were the supervisors’ professional and personal credibility; the need for strong boundaries and clear roles; and the mutuality of the relationship. This supports existing research (e.g. Palomo et al. Reference Palomo, Beinart and Cooper2010).

Participants in the current study felt that the imposition of a psychodynamic model onto the supervisory dynamic had a negative impact upon the alliance. Their main complaints related to the inflexibility of the approach, rigid boundaries, and a lack of practical guidance. This was perceived to have impeded learning due to high levels of anxiety. This is somewhat at odds with psychoanalytical authors, who have emphasized the centrality of the alliance and the need to be supervisee-centred, open and safe (e.g. Dewald, Reference Dewald and Watkins1997). One factor which may help to explain these differences is the use of explicit contracting at the beginning of the supervisory relationship (e.g. Scaife, Reference Scaife2001; Hawkins & Shohet, Reference Hawkins and Shohet2007).

Scaffolding and Socratic Method

Trainees found the Socratic Method was beneficial because it was viewed as inherently supervisee-centred and developmentally sensitive. This appeared to aid their processing of receipt and ensured that the learning process was motivating. Learning in this way is likely to enhance self-efficacy (Bandura, Reference Bandura1997), positive affect and intrinsic motivation (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2000). Notably, the use of the Socratic Method is implicit within an approach to therapy which places the development of intrinsic motivation at its core: Motivational Interviewing (MI; Miller & Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick1991, Reference Miller and Rollnick2002). The relationship between the Socratic Method and educational scaffolding emerged as an important dynamic. This changed developmentally from task-focused scaffolding to process-focused scaffolding. Later stages were also associated with a more collaborative Socratic approach. Recently, James et al. (Reference James, Milne and Morse2008) highlighted the value of scaffolding as a key micro-skill within cognitive behavioural supervision. A similar level of micro-analysis has been pivotal to developments within MI and subsequent training initiatives (e.g. Amrhein et al. Reference Amrhein, Miller, Yahne, Palmer and Fulcher2003, Amrhein, Reference Amrhein2004). These types of micro-analytical studies are arguably much needed and are likely to yield useful practical teaching and supervision materials within clinical process research.

One question which emerges from the current study is why trainees perceived the receipt of supervision from psychodynamic supervisors to be non-Socratic and lacking in terms educational scaffolding and explicit feedback? Dewald (Reference Dewald and Watkins1997) offers an explanation in terms of differences in pedagogic theory within psychoanalytical supervision. While some psychoanalytical supervisors see feedback and encouragement as part of their role, others do not and espouse a position where ‘judgments about what is good or bad are essentially reserved for quiet, silent reflection in the mind of the analyst or supervisor’ (p. 38). Clearly trainees in the current study found this to be unhelpful and anxiety-provoking. Recent research within educational psychology would seriously question the rationale for minimal guidance and a lack of feedback (e.g. Kirschner et al. Reference Kirschner, Sweller and Clark2006).

The role of reflection

While reflection has been highlighted as a ‘blind spot’ within psychology (e.g. Bennett-Levy, Reference Bennett-Levy2006), its importance is recognized within the adult learning literature (e.g. Kolb, Reference Kolb1984; Boud et al. Reference Boud, Keogh and Walker1985; Schon, Reference Schon1986) and latterly within CBT supervision (e.g. Bennett-Levy et al. Reference Bennett-Levy, McManus, Westling and Fennell2009). Studies of therapist development have highlighted its vital role (e.g. Bennett-Levy et al. Reference Bennett-Levy, Turner, Beaty, Smith, Patterson and Farmer2001, Reference Bennett-Levy, Lee, Travers, Pohlman and Hamernik2003; Milne & James, Reference Milne and James2002; Freeston et al. Reference Freeston, Armstrong and Twaddle2003). However, notwithstanding the importance placed on reflection, supervisees in the current study were unable to identify specific strategies which they used to aid their reflection, other than reflective writing (e.g. Bolton, Reference Bolton2005), via their supervision notes. At earlier stages, the supervisees' notes helped them to process the more pragmatic aspects of receipt. As their conceptual and technical skills developed, trainees reported a greater focus on procedural knowledge and interpersonal processes in therapy (Safran & Segal, Reference Safran and Segal1990; Bennett-Levy, Reference Bennett-Levy2006). Processing the receipt of interpersonal material had a gradual, experimental, and accidental quality about it.

Bennett-Levy (Reference Bennett-Levy2006) stresses the significance of reflection in his DRP model and proposes that it is at later stages in the development of clinical expertise that tacit procedural skills are converted into declarative knowledge. The current study would certainly lend support for this proposition, but further work is required to explore the intricacies involved. One explanation is that because subtle dynamics are less likely to be made explicit; trainees may not be clear about what they are actually reflecting upon? For example, the supervisees described this as an implicit feeling state, rather than an explicit knowing state. Another explanation may be that interpersonal processes need to be applied before they begin to make sense. It may be that trainees did not explicitly bring interpersonal process material to supervision until later stages, because of a lack of ability to clearly articulate their needs. Arguably, this may be where videotapes of sessions are essential (Bennett-Levy & Thwaites, Reference Bennett-Levy, Thwaites, Gilbert and Leahy2009). While some trainees found it useful, it was not the norm for trainees, in the current study, to audio- or video-record their therapy sessions with clients. At earlier stages supervisors may not have forced this issue, due to higher levels of anxiety in trainees. Interestingly, Shepherd et al. (Reference Shepherd, Salkovskis and Morris2009) have found support for the use of therapy recordings for supervision purposes. Further work examining the intricacies of why these recordings are helpful would be of use. There is some evidence that supervisees, in the current study, developed an ability to move from a position of ‘reflecting on action’ (i.e. after their supervision session) to a position of reflection in action (i.e. within supervision). This supports findings from previous research regarding the development of the reflective practitioner (e.g. Schon, Reference Schon1986). Finally, ‘meta-reflection’ on overall supervision experiences across different placements was viewed, by supervisees in the current study, as a significant gap in their training.

Limitations

This study was restricted to supervisees from one course who were known to the researcher. While there is evidence that interviewees prefer to talk to people with whom they have established rapport (Kvale & Brinkmann, Reference Kvale and Brinkmann2008), it is acknowledged that this may have influenced some of the responses provided. This study did not test a model of supervision, but it may have been useful to specifically select trainees who were known to have been supervised by supervisors who had undertaken specialist training in one supervision model (e.g. CBT-based supervision).

Implications for future research

Cross-sectional or longitudinal research designs employing ‘traditional’ statistical approaches which aim to examine isolated aspects of supervision often fail to account for the inherent complexity involved (e.g. Ellis & Ladany, Reference Ellis, Ladany and Watkins1997). However, it should be noted that there are positivist accounts which match the complexity of the present analysis, such as a systematic review conducted by Milne et al. (Reference Milne, Aylott, Fitzpatrick and Ellis2008). Based on 24 empirical studies, they too recognized the interacting core processes and the developmental context. Indeed, Kolb's (Reference Kolb1984) model anticipated the developmental complexity emphasized by the present study and by Milne et al. (Reference Milne, Aylott, Fitzpatrick and Ellis2008). Therefore, the present study appears to be novel in stressing the role of awareness in guiding experiential learning, and in providing an elegant, relatively parsimonious model, based on an established constructionist methodology. It is also heartening that this approach yielded findings that essentially corroborate the conclusions of quantitative research. An alternative (positivistic) approach may be to utilize statistical approaches which are specifically designed to study complex interactions, e.g. multi-level modelling (University of Bristol, 2009), multi-dimensional scaling techniques (MDS; StatSoft Inc., 2009), or single-subject designs.

The following reflections emerge from the use of GTM within the present study. First, it would be useful to study receipt processes with experienced supervisees (i.e. as continuing professional development), to explore the way in which receipt of supervision is processed at later developmental stages. Second, it would be useful to explore whether supervisees trained in particular theoretical approaches process the receipt of supervision in qualitatively different ways. Given the negative comments made by the present supervisees in relation to their psychodynamic supervision, it may be especially helpful to explore the receipt of supervision from those who elect to work psychodynamically. This may provide some insight into whether it was the imposition of the psychodynamic model at a particular stage of training, or the model itself, which is viewed as pedagogically problematic. Third, it would appear that there is a great deal of potential in the micro-analysis of processes involved in receipt. Video-recorded observations of live supervision sessions would be valuable to see what actually happens in practice. Further, investigating the role of multi-media in the receipt of supervision may also be helpful (i.e. by selecting supervisees who use audio and video recordings to aid their processing of receipt). Given that qualitative data analysis software (e.g. NVIVO) now facilitates the direct analysis of video-recorded material, this may offer a useful tool to progress research in micro-skills. Finally, using qualitative approaches to study other aspects of the fidelity framework may helpfully differentiate aspects of receipt which overlap with other fidelity components (e.g. enactment or generalization), which may help move our understanding towards a more complete model of supervision.

Implications for practice

The current study suggests that the developmental stage of the supervisee needs careful consideration in terms of the selection of supervisors. Given some of the negative experiences highlighted in the current study, it may be sensible to expose trainees to supervisors who have demonstrated an appropriate level of competency in supervision. This could involve attendance at accredited supervisor training (e.g. Milne, Reference Milne2010). For safety reasons it may be prudent to ask new supervisors to: jointly supervise a minimum number of trainees before being permitted to be a lead or sole supervisor; ensure they are working within an established evidence-based pedagogic model of supervision (e.g. Milne, Reference Milne2009); and to agree an explicit contract. Similarly, one could argue that it may be prudent to provide training to help supervisees to understand that they too have a responsibility for helping the relationship to work. Further, for continuing professional development for both parties, it may be useful to encourage digital recordings of supervision sessions.

Conclusions

The fidelity construct ‘receipt’ is highly complex and does not lend itself easily to quantification and measurement using traditional statistical approaches. However, the use of a GTM may have captured this complexity. Specifically, the current study suggests that receipt changes according to a supervisee's developmental stage and that this is influenced by four key constructs which allow learning to take place (Scaffolding, Socratic Information Exchange, Reflection, Supervisory Alliance). The use of GTM in the present study has both corroborated and further developed the broad conclusions about experiential learning from quantitative research (Milne, Reference Milne2009), adding a fresh emphasis on the pivotal role of the supervisee's awareness as a determinant of learning within supervision. Further research using a similar methodology may illuminate the micro-skills involved and provide further explanation as to how the four core processes interact, contributing towards a sound model of CBT supervision to fill the theoretical vacuum.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the supervisees for their willingness to participate in the interviews and for sharing their understanding of supervision.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Learning objectives

After reading this paper the reader should be able to:

  1. (1) List five reasons why a model matters.

  2. (2) Summarize progress to date on developing a CBT supervision model.

  3. (3) Describe the main themes of the CBT supervision model that are outlined in the present paper.

References

Recommended follow-up reading

Colquitt, JA, LePine, JA, Noe, RA (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: a meta-analytic path analysis of twenty years of research Journal of Applied Psychology 85, 678707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, D, James, I (2005). Clinical supervision: 10 tests of the tandem model. Clinical Psychology Forum 151, 69.Google Scholar
Watkins, CE (1995). Psychotherapy supervisor development: on musings, models, and metaphor. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research 4, 150158.Google Scholar

References

Amrhein, PC (2004). How does motivational interviewing work? What client talk reveals. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 18, 323336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amrhein, PC, Miller, WR, Yahne, CE, Palmer, M, Fulcher, L (2003). Client commitment language during motivational interviewing predicts drug use outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 71, 862878.Google Scholar
Bandura, A (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Beinart, H (2004). Models of supervision and the supervisory relationship and their evidence base. In: Supervision and Clinical Psychology: Theory, Practice and Perspectives (ed. Fleming, I. and Steen, L.), pp. 3650. New York: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
Bellg, AJ, Borrelli, B, Resnick, B, Hecht, J, Minicucci, DS, Ory, M, Ogedegbe, G, Orwig, D, Ernst, D, Czajkowski, S (2004) Enhancing treatment fidelity in health behavior change studies: Best practices and recommendations from the NIH behavior change consortium. Health Psychology 23, 443451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett-Levy, J (2006). Therapist skills: a cognitive model of their acquisition and refinement. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy 34, 5778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett-Levy, J, Lee, N, Travers, K, Pohlman, S, Hamernik, E (2003). Cognitive therapy from the inside: Enhancing therapist skills from practising what we preach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 31, 143158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett-Levy, J, McManus, F, Westling, BE, Fennell, M (2009). Acquiring and refining CBT skills and competencies: Which training methods are perceived to be most effective? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 37, 571583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett-Levy, J, Thwaites, R (2009). Self and self reflection in the therapeutic relationship. In: The Therapeutic Relationship in the Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies (ed. Gilbert, P. and Leahy, R. L.), pp. 255281. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bennett-Levy, J, Turner, F, Beaty, T, Smith, M, Patterson, B, Farmer, S (2001). The value of self-practice of cognitive therapy techniques and self-reflection in the training of cognitive therapists. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy 29, 203220.Google Scholar
Bernard, JM, Goodyear, RK (2004). Fundamentals of Clinical Supervision. Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar
Blackburn, IM, James, IA, Milne, DL, Baker, C, Standart, SH, Garland, A (2001). The revised Cognitive Therapy Scale (CTS-R): psychometric properties. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy 29, 431446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolton, G (2005). Reflective Practice, 2nd edn. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Bordin, ES (1983). A working alliance model of supervision. Counseling Psychologist 11, 3542.Google Scholar
Boud, D, Keogh, R, Walker, D (1985). Reflection: Turning Experience into Learning. New York: Kogan Page.Google Scholar
Bringer, JD, Johnston, LH, Brackenridge, CH (2004). Maximizing transparency in a doctoral thesis: the complexities of writing about the use of QSR*NVIVO within a Grounded Theory study. Qualitative Research 4, 247265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bringer, JD, Johnston, LH, Brackenridge, CH (2006 a). Using computer-assisted qualitative data analysis software to develop a grounded theory project, Field Methods 18, 245266.Google Scholar
Bringer, JD, Johnston, LH, Brackenridge, CH (2006 b). Swimming coaches’ perceptions of sexual exploitation in sport: a preliminary model of role conflict and role ambiguity. The Sport Psychologist 20, 469475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bringer, JD, Johnston, LH, Brackenridge, CH (2007). Maximizing transparency in a doctoral thesis: the complexities of writing about the use of QSR*NVIVO within a Grounded Theory study. In: Qualitative Research 2, Volume 4: Qualitative Data Analysis (ed. Bryman, A.), pp. 6384. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Centre for Investigative Psychology (2009). (http://www.i-psy.com/publications/publications_reading.php). Accessed 31 May 2009.Google Scholar
Charmaz, K (2006). Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide through Qualitative Analysis. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Coyne, IT (1997). Sampling in qualitative research. Purposeful and theoretical sampling; merging or clear boundaries? Journal of Advanced Nursing 26, 623630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culloty, T, Milne, DL, Sheikh, AI (2010). Evaluating the training of clinical supervisors: a pilot study using the fidelity framework. The Cognitive Behaviour Therapist 3, 132144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denzin, NK, Lincoln, YS(eds) (2008). Collecting and Interpreting Qualitative Materials, 3rd edn.London: Sage.Google Scholar
Dewald, PA (1997). The process of supervision in psychoanalysis. In: Handbook of Psychotherapy Supervision (ed. Watkins, C. E.), pp. 3143. Chichester: WileyGoogle Scholar
Ellis, MV, Ladany, N (1997). Inferences concerning supervisees and clients in clinical supervision: an integrative review. In: Handbook of Psychotherapy Supervision (ed. Watkins, C. E.), pp. 447507. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ellis, MV, Ladany, N, Krengel, M, Schult, D (1996). Clinical supervision research from 1981 to 1993: A methodological critique. Journal of Counseling Psychology 43, 3550.Google Scholar
Freeston, MH, Armstrong, P, Twaddle, V (2003). Supervision: integrating practical skills with a conceptual framework [unpublished document]. Available from Newcastle Cognitive and Behavioural Therapies Centre, UK.Google Scholar
Glaser, BG, Strauss, AL (1967). The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Green, D (2004). Organizing and evaluating supervisor training. In: Supervision and Clinical Psychology: Theory, Practice and Perspectives (ed. Fleming, I. and Steen, L.), pp. 93107. New York: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
Hart, G (1982). The Process of Clinical Supervision. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, P, Shohet, R (2007). Supervision in the Helping Professions. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Holloway, EL (1984). Outcome evaluation in supervision research. Counseling Psychology 12, 167174.Google Scholar
Holloway, EL (1995). Clinical Supervision: A Systems Approach. Sage: London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holloway, EL, Neufeldt, SA (1995). Supervision: its contribution to treatment efficacy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 63, 207213.Google Scholar
Hutchison, AJ, Johnston, LH, Breckon, J (2010). Using QSR-NVivo to facilitate the development of a grounded theory project: an account of a worked example. International Journal of Social Research Methodology 13, 283302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, IA, Milne, DL, Blackburn, I-M, Armstrong, P (2006). Conducting successful supervision: novel elements towards an integrative approach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 35, 191200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, IA, Milne, DL, Morse, R (2008). Microskills of clinical supervision: Scaffolding skills. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly 22, 2936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, LH, Breckon, JD, Hutchison, A (2009). Influencing health behavior: applying theory to practice. In: Physical Activity and Health Promotion (ed. Dugdill, L., Crone, D. and Graham, R.), pp. 2142. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnston, LH, Corban, RM, Clarke, P (1999). Multi-method approaches to the investigation of adherence issues within sport and exercise: qualitative and quantitative techniques. In: Adherence Issues in Exercise and Sport (ed. Bull, S. J.), pp. 263288. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kirschner, PA, Sweller, J, Clark, RE (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching, Educational Psychologist 41, 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, DA (1984). Experiential Learning. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Kvale, S, Brinkmann, S (2008). Inter Views: Learning the Craft of Qualitative Research Interview. London: SageGoogle Scholar
Ladany, N, Ellis, MV, Friedlander, ML (1999). The supervisory alliance, trainee self-efficacy, and satisfaction with supervision. Journal of Counseling and Development 77, 447455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladany, N, Melincoff, DS (1999). The nature of counselor non-disclosure. Counselor Education and Supervision 38, 161176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, WR, Rollnick, S (1991) Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, WR, Rollnick, S (2002). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change, 2nd edn.New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Milne, DL (2006). Developing clinical supervision research through reasoned analogies with therapy. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 13, 215222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, DL (2007). An empirical definition of clinical supervision. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 46, 437447.Google Scholar
Milne, DL (2008). CBT supervision: from reflexivity to specialization. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy 36, 779786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, DL (2009). Evidence-Based Clinical Supervision. Chichester: BPS/Blackwell.Google Scholar
Milne, DL (2010). Can we enhance the training of clinical supervisors? A national pilot study of an evidence-based approach. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 17, 321328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, DL, Aylott, H, Fitzpatrick, H, Ellis, MV (2008). How does clinical supervision work? Using a Best Evidence Synthesis approach to construct a basic model of supervision. The Clinical Supervisor 27, 170190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, DL, James, I (2002). The observed impact of training on competence in clinical supervision. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 41, 5572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, DL, Pilkington, J, Gracie, J, James, IA (2003). Transferring skills from supervision to therapy: a qualitative and quantitative N = 1 analysis. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 31, 193202.Google Scholar
Milne, DL, Reiser, RP, Cliffe, T, Raine, R (2011). SAGE: preliminary evaluation of an instrument for observing competence in CBT supervision. The Cognitive Behaviour Therapist 4, 123138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padesky, CA (1996). Developing cognitive therapist competency: teaching and supervision models. In: Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy (ed. Salkovskis, P. M.), pp. 266292). London: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Palomo, M, Beinart, H, Cooper, MJ (2010). Development and validation of the Supervisory Relationship Questionnaire (SRQ) in UK trainee clinical psychologists. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 49, 131149.Google Scholar
Popper, KR (1972). Conjectures and Refutations: The Growth of Scientific Knowledge. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pretorius, WM (2006). Cognitive-behavioural supervision: recommended practice. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 34, 413420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
QSR International (2009). (http://www.qsrinternational.com/). Accessed 31 May 2009.Google Scholar
Richards, L, Morse, JM (2007). ReadME First for a User's Guide to Qualitative Methods. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Resnick, B, Bellg, AJ, Borrelli, B, DeFrancesco, C, Breger, R, Hecht, J, Sharp, DL, Levesque, C, Orwig, D, Ernst, D, Ogedegbe, G, Czajkowski, S (2005). Examples of implementation and evaluation of treatment fidelity in the BCC studies: where we are and where we need to go. Annals of Behavioural Medicine 29, 4654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, A, Pilling, S (2008). The competence framework for supervision [unpublished document]. (http://www.ucl.ac.uk/clinical-psychology/CORE/supervision_framework.htm).Google Scholar
Ryan, RM, Deci, EL (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist 55, 6878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Safran, JD, Segal, ZV (1990). Interpersonal Process in Cognitive Therapy. New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Scaife, J (2001). Supervision in the Mental Health Professions: A Practitioner's Guide. Hove: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
Schon, D (1986). Educating the Reflective Practitioner. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Schwandt, TA (2003). Three epistemological stances for qualitative inquiry: interpretivism, hermeneutics, and social constructionism. In: The Landscape of Qualitative Research: Theories and Issues, 2nd edn (ed. Denzin, N. K. and Lincoln, Y S.), pp. 292331. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Shepherd, L, Salkovskis, P, Morris, M (2009). Recording therapy sessions: an evaluation of patient and therapist reported behaviors, attitudes and preferences. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy 37, 141150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StatSoft Inc. (2009). Multi-dimensional scaling (http://www.statsoft.com/textbook/stmulsca.html). Accessed 31 May 200.Google Scholar
Stoltenberg, CD, Delworth, U (1987). Supervising Counselors and Therapists. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Stoltenberg, CD, McNeil, BW (1997). Clinical supervision from a developmental perspective: research and practice. In: Handbook of Psychotherapy Supervision (ed. Watkins, C. E.), pp. 184202. Chichester: WileyGoogle Scholar
Townend, M (2008). Clinical supervision in cognitive-behavioural psychotherapy: Development of a model for mental health nursing through grounded theory. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 15, 328339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townend, M, Iannetta, L, Freeston, MH (2002). Clinical supervision in practice: a survey of UK cognitive behavioural psychotherapists accredited by the BABCP. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 485–50.Google Scholar
University of Bristol (2009). Multilevel models (http://www.cmm.bristol.ac.uk/learning-training/multilevel-models/index.shtml). Accessed 31 May 2009.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, LS (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wampold, BE, Holloway, EL (1997). Research and methods in supervision research. In: Handbook on Psychotherapy Supervision (ed. Watkins, C. E. Jr.), pp. 1127. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Watkins, CE (ed.) (1997). Handbook of Psychotherapy Supervision. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Weaks, D (2002). Unlocking the secrets of ‘good supervision’: a phenomenological exploration of experienced counsellors’ perceptions of good supervision. Counseling and Psychotherapy Research 2, 3339.Google Scholar
Webb, A, Wheeler, S (1998). How honest do counselors dare to be in the supervisory relationship? An exploratory study. British Journal of Guidance and Counseling 26, 509524.Google Scholar
Worthen, V, McNeil, BW (1996). A phenomenological investigation of ‘good’ supervision events. Journal of Counseling Psychology 43, 2534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant's age and supervision experience at time of interview

Figure 1

Fig. 1. The iterative process of data collection and analysis in Grounded Theory Methodology (adapted from Johnston et al. 1999, p. 268).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. NVIVO8 screen print showing the Socratic Method free node, memo and external.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. NVIVO8 screen print showing the results node ‘autonomy and developmental’ with coding stripes.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. The developmental progression of the receipt of clinical supervision.

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.