Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-03T18:00:30.208Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between the experience of mood symptoms, expectancy judgement and a person's current concern

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 August 2010

Elizabeth S. Reilly*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Glasgow, UK
Warren Mansell
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, UK
*
*Author for correspondence: Miss E. S. Reilly, 3/1, 66 Wilton Street, Glasgow G20 6RU, UK. (email: [email protected])

Abstract

This study examined expectancy judgement and current concerns in high and low depression and anxiety participants. Expectancy judgement was measured using the Personal Future Task. Depression and anxiety symptoms were measured using the Depression and Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS). A novel scale, the Current Concerns Checklist, was developed to measure ten current concerns that were thought to relate to the most salient concerns of common Axis I disorders. Using the DASS, 19 participants were allocated to the distressed group and 17 to the non-distressed group. As hypothesized, there was a main effect for the current concern concept; participants thought of more future events regarding their current concern than their non-concern. However, the hypothesis that the distressed group would generate more negative relative to positive responses than the non-distressed group within the domain of their most prominent current concern was not supported. Future research and implications for CBT are discussed.

Type
Original Research
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Recommended follow-up reading

Byrne, A, MacLeod, AK (1997). Attributions and accessibility of explanations for future events in anxiety and depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 36, 505535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, AG, Watkins, E, Mansell, W, Shafran, R (2004). Cognitive Behavioural Processes Across Psychological Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Approach to Research and Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E (1996). Emotional influences on cognitive processing, with implications for theories of both. In: The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behaviour (ed. Gollwitzer, P. W. and Bargh, J. A.), pp. 168189. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
MacLeod, AK, Byrne, A (1996). Anxiety, depression and the anticipation of future positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 286289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Ackermann, R, DeRubeis, RJ (1991). Is depressive realism real? Clinical Psychology Review 11, 565584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
APA (1994). Diagnostic Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn.Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Beck, AT (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York: Hoeber.Google Scholar
Beck, AT (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Bentall, RP (2003). Madness Explained: Psychosis and Human Nature. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Bock, M, Klinger, E (1996). Interaction of emotion and cognition in word recall. Psychological Research 48, 99106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, A, MacLeod, AK (1997). Attributions and accessibility of explanations for future events in anxiety and depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 36, 505535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constans, JI, Mathews, A (1993). Mood and subjective risk of future events. Cognition and Emotion 7, 545560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, MA, Pleydell-Pearce, CW (2000). The construction of auto-biographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review 107, 261288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drake, RA (1984). Lateral asymmetry of personal optimism. Journal of Research in Personality 18, 497507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dugas, MJ, Hedayati, M, Karavias, A, Buhr, K, Francis, K, Phillips, NA (2005). Intolerance or uncertainty and information processing: evidence of biased recall and interpretations. Cognitive Therapy and Research 29, 5770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, NH (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Haenen, MA, de Jong, PJ, Schmidt, AJ, Stevens, S, Visser, L (2000). Hypochondriac's estimation of negative outcomes: domain specific and responsiveness to reassuring and alarming information. Behaviour Research and Therapy 38, 819833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, AG, Watkins, E, Mansell, W, Shafran, R (2004). Cognitive Behavioural Processes across Psychological Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Approach to Research and Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, SC, Wilson, KG, Strosahl, K, Gifford, EV, Follette, VM (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioural disorders: a functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 64, 11521168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, JD, Crawford, JR (2005). The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44, 227239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hohlstein, LA, Smith, GT, Atlas, JG (1998). An application of expectancy theory to eating disorders: development and validation of measures of eating and dieting expectancies. Psychological Assessment 10, 4958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, RC, Chiu, WT, Demler, O, Walters, EE (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 617627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirsch, I (1985). Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behaviour. American Psychologist 40, 11891202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirsch, I (1990). Changing Expectations: A Key to Effective Psychotherapy. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Kirsch, I (1997). Response expectancy theory and application: a decennial review. Applied and Preventative Psychology 6, 6779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review 82, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E (1977). Meaning and Void: Inner Experience and the Incentives in People's Lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Klinger, E (1978). Modes of normal conscious flow. In: The Stream of Consciousness: Scientific Investigations into the Flow of Human Experience (ed. Pope, K. S. and Singer, J. L.), pp. 225258. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinger, E (1989). Emotional mediation of motivational influences on cognitive processes. In: International Perspectives on Achievement and Task Motivation (ed. Halisch, F. and van den Bercken, J. H. L.), pp. 317326. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Klinger, E (1996). Emotional influences on cognitive processing, with implications for theories of both. In: The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behaviour (ed. Gollwitzer, P. W. and Bargh, J. A.), pp. 168189. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Lara, ME, Klein, DN, Kasch, KL (2001). Psychosocial predictors of short-term course and outcome of major depression: a longitudinal study of a non-clinical sample with recent-onset episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 644650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovibond, SH, Lovibond, PE (1995). Manual for Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation.Google Scholar
MacLeod, AK, Byrne, A (1996). Anxiety, depression and the anticipation of future positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 286289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, AK, Tata, P, Kentish, J, Jacobsen, H (1997). Retrospective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cognition and Emotion 11, 467479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, LL, Tesser, A (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In: Ruminative Thoughts. Advances in Social Cognition, vol. 9 (ed. Wyer, R. S.), pp. 147. USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Murphy, R, Hirsch, CR, Mathews, A, Smith, K, Clark, DM (2007). Facilitating a benign interpretation bias in a high socially anxious population. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 15171529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K (1969). Conjectures and Refutations, 3rd edn.London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Reid, S (2009). Intrusive imagery and goals: a control theory perspective. The Cognitive Behaviour Therapist 2, 134144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanley, B, Gibson, AJ (1985). The prevalence of chronic psychiatric morbidity. British Journal of Psychiatry 140, 372376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, ER, Dodrill, CL, Johnson, N (2001). Depressive cognition: a test of depressive realism versus general negativity using general knowledge questions. Journal of Psychology, Interdisciplinary and Applied 135, 583602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strunk, DR, Lopez, H, DeRubeis, RJ (2005). Expressive symptoms are associated with unrealistic negative predictions of future life events.Behaviour Research and Therapy 44, 861882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, SE, Brown, JD (1989). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin 103, 193210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsao, JCI, Mystkowski, JL, Zucker, BG, Craske, MG (2002). Effects of cognitive-behavioural therapy for panic disorder on comorbid conditions: replication and extension. Behaviour Therapy 33, 493509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A, Kahneman, D (1972). Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology 4, 207232.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.