Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:05:17.724Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’: randomised trial of psychiatrists' personal v. professional perspectives on treatment recommendations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rosmarie Mendel*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München
Johannes Hamann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München
Eva Traut-Mattausch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, München
Markus Bühner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, München
Werner Kissling
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München
Dieter Frey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, München, Germany
*
Rosmarie Mendel, Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München, Möhlstraße 26, 81675 München, Germany. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

If patients are unsure whether a specific treatment is really good for them, they often pose the question, ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ Patients want their psychiatrists to put themselves in their shoes and not to give a ‘standard recommendation’.

Aims

To study whether this question really leads psychiatrists to reveal their personal preferences.

Method

Randomised experimental study with 515 psychiatrists incorporating two decision scenarios (depression scenario: antidepressant v. watchful waiting; schizophrenia scenario: depot v. oral antipsychotic) and three experimental conditions (giving a recommendation to a patient asking, ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’; giving a regular recommendation to a patient without being asked this question; and imagining being ill and deciding for yourself). Main outcome measures were the treatments chosen or recommended by physicians.

Results

Psychiatrists choosing treatment for themselves predominantly selected other treatments (mostly watchful waiting and oral antipsychotics respectively) than what psychiatrists recommended to patients when asked in the ‘regular recommendation role’ (i.e. antidepressant and depot respectively). Psychiatrists in the ‘what-would-you-do role’ gave recommendations similar to the ‘regular recommendation role’ (depression scenario: χ2 = 0.12, P = 0.73; schizophrenia scenario: χ2 = 2.60, P = 0.11) but distinctly different from the ‘self role’.

Conclusions

The question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ does not motivate psychiatrists to leave their professional recommendation role and to take a more personal perspective. Psychiatrists should try to find out why individuals are asking this question and, together with the individual, identify the most appropriate treatment option.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2010 

‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ is a question that is often posed to doctors when individuals have a decisional conflict and when they are unsure which treatment to choose. Reference Baylis and Downie1 By asking this question, patients expect doctors to go beyond giving professional advice and reveal their personal preferences by imagining themselves in the patient's situation. Reference Zaner and Khushf2 Many patients believe that doctors, when deciding for themselves, would choose the best option Reference Hayes3 since they are free from strategic considerations (for example financial limitations, physicians’ self-interests). And in fact, there is evidence that the treatment doctors choose for themselves sometimes differs from the treatment they recommend to their patients. Reference Gardner and Ogden4,Reference Steinert5 Doctors, for example, are less likely to have surgery than the general population, and although they might recommend taking part in clinical trials to their patients, they are less inclined to do so themselves. Reference Belanger, Moore and Tannock6,Reference Domenighetti, Casabianca, Gutzwiller and Martinoli7 In psychiatry, it has also been shown that some health professionals reject the same therapies for themselves that are typically prescribed to patients. Reference Amering, Denk, Griengl, Sibitz and Stastny8 Many individuals with psychiatric conditions are sceptical about psychotropic drugs and the evidence is often ambiguous as to what benefit can really be expected from medications (for example antidepressants). Reference Moncrieff and Kirsch9 In these cases such patients might be especially interested in what their psychiatrists would choose for themselves. However, despite patients often asking ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ little is known about how psychiatrists react to this question and whether they truly recommend what they would choose for themselves if they were ill.

Method

Design

We chose a randomised experimental design with two different decision scenarios: the first treatment for an individual with depression and the second treatment for an individual with schizophrenia; both were described in a case vignette. The study included three experimental conditions: psychiatrists were told to imagine that they were the treating physician and had to give a regular recommendation to the individual (‘regular recommendation role’); psychiatrists were told to imagine that they were ill and had to decide for themselves (‘self role’); psychiatrists were told to imagine that they were the treating physician being asked ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor’ by the individual (‘what-would-you-do role’).

In each scenario, participating psychiatrists could choose between two treatment options (depression scenario: watchful waiting v. antidepressant; schizophrenia scenario: oral v. depot antipsychotic). The decisions between watchful waiting and antidepressant and between oral antipsychotic and depot antipsychotic were chosen because both are frequent in the daily routine and evidence does not suggest that one option is clearly superior to the other. Therefore we expected to see a difference between the self role and the regular recommendation role.

As in studies with lay people, we imagined that physicians in the self role would more often choose the less invasive treatment options (watchful waiting and oral medication) than in the regular recommendation role. Reference Raymark10,Reference Zikmund-Fisher, Sarr, Fagerlin and Ubel11 Two different decision scenarios (depression scenario and schizophrenia scenario) were used to learn whether physicians’ behaviour is also affected by the type of decision scenario, not just by their role. Study participants were asked to participate in a ‘medical decision-making’ questionnaire. In order to avoid influencing the participants' responses, no specific information was given about the study. The study was approved by the institutional review board (Ethikkommission der Technischen Universität München).

Participants

Participants were 515 psychiatrists recruited consecutively during the annual congress of the German Psychiatric Association (Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychiatrie, Psychotherapie und Nervenheilkunde), Germany. All attending psychiatrists were eligible. The response rate was almost 90%.

Procedure

Questionnaires were sorted in a randomised manner and then consecutively handed out to participants. Participating psychiatrists received a questionnaire containing one of the two scenarios (depression or schizophrenia) and one of the three experimental conditions (recommendation role, what-would-you-do role or self role). After reading the case vignette, the participants were requested to make a choice between two treatment options (Fig. 1 and Appendix).

Depression scenario

In this scenario, psychiatrists in the recommendation role and in the what-would-you-do role were asked to imagine treating an out-patient with depressive symptoms. Psychiatrists in the self role were asked to imagine that they had depressive symptoms themselves and were being treated by a psychiatrist as an out-patient. The two treatment options to choose from were immediate treatment with an antidepressant and watchful waiting. In the regular recommendation role and the what-would-you-do role, psychiatrists were asked to indicate what kind of treatment they would recommend to the patient. In the self role, the psychiatrists were requested to decide what treatment option they would prefer if they had depression themselves. Depressive symptoms presented in the case vignettes were identical in all three conditions.

Schizophrenia scenario

The structure of this scenario was identical to the depression scenario. Psychiatrists were asked to imagine treating an out-patient experiencing a second relapse of schizophrenia (in the self role: to imagine having the disease themselves). It was additionally mentioned that the patient (in the self role: the physician) sometimes forgets to take the antipsychotic medication. The two treatment options to choose from were treatment with an oral antipsychotic or a depot antipsychotic.

Fig. 1 Procedure.

After making the treatment decision for their assigned scenario, psychiatrists answered additional questions about their opinion of the effectiveness of the treatment options, about the importance of whether the treatment choice is in accordance with treatment guidelines and about the unpleasantness of the treatment options for the patient/for themselves (6-point Likert scale ranging from ‘low’ (1) to ‘high’ (6)). All participants were requested to supply their age, gender and professional psychiatric experience, along with the percentage of their patients with depression (depression scenario) or schizophrenia (schizophrenia scenario). All responses were anonymous.

Data analysis

To test whether treatment selections differed across the three conditions (regular recommendation role, what-would-you-do role and self role) we used chi-squared tests, including post hoc comparisons. Analyses of variance (also including post hoc Scheffé comparisons) were used to investigate whether psychiatrists in different roles also vary with respect to different aspects of treatment choice (for example perceived effectiveness of treatment options, importance that treatment choice is in accordance with treatment guidelines). Additionally, paired t-tests were applied for within-group comparisons. A hierarchical logistic regression analysis was conducted to test whether the role had, independently of several factors (variables tested in the above mentioned analyses of variance and sociodemographic variables), influenced the psychiatrists' treatment selection. We used a hierarchical approach because we wanted to explore, first of all, the effect of the role and second, the effect of other variables included in the regression analysis. A two-sided P<0.05 was considered significant. Data were analysed using the SPSS Version 16.0 for Windows.

Results

We asked 578 psychiatrists to fill out the questionnaire. In total 7% of them (n = 40) refused to take part in the study, another 4% (n = 23) did not return the questionnaire, giving an overall response rate of 89%. Thus, a total of 515 psychiatrists (228 females, 285 males; data missing for 2) took part in the study. Their mean age was 46.1 years (s.d. = 9.4) and their professional experience averaged 15.0 years (s.d. = 9.7). About 67.2% of the psychiatrists worked in a psychiatric hospital and 30.5% worked in private practice; 2.3% of the participants worked in neither a hospital nor a private practice. Differences regarding sociodemographic variables were not found between the two different decision scenarios or between the three different role conditions.

Influence of psychiatrists' role on treatment decisions

The results of both scenarios showed that psychiatrists preferred different treatment options depending on their role in the decision process (depression scenario: χ2 = 44.36, P<0.001; schizophrenia scenario: χ2 = 37.79, P<0.001). Post hoc comparisons revealed that psychiatrists in both scenarios of the what-would-you-do role differed significantly from psychiatrists in the self role (depression scenario: χ2 = 32.30, P<0.001; schizophrenia scenario: χ2 = 36.66, P<0.001) but not from psychiatrists in the regular recommendation role (depression scenario: χ2 = 0.12, P = 0.73; schizophrenia scenario: χ2 = 2.60, P = 0.11). Moreover, differences between psychiatrists in the regular recommendation role and psychiatrists in the self role were also significant (depression scenario: χ2 = 29.15, P<0.001; schizophrenia scenario: χ2 = 20.93, P<0.001).

With regard to treatment options, psychiatrists in the what-would-you-do role and in the regular recommendation role more often chose an antidepressant in the depression scenario and a depot medication in the schizophrenia scenario than psychiatrists in the self role (psychiatrists who chose an antidepressant: what-would-you-do role 81.4%, regular recommendation role 79.3%, self role 39.1%; psychiatrists who chose a depot medication: what-would-you-do role 58.8%, regular recommendation role 46.4%, self role 14.1%; Fig. 2).

Taken together, these findings indicate that psychiatrists chose distinctly different therapies when deciding in the self role compared with the regular recommendation role and the question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ apparently did not motivate psychiatrists to reveal their true personal preferences.

Fig. 2 Chosen treatment options in relation to psychiatrists' role.

Influence of psychiatrists' role on attitudes towards treatment options

In accordance with the differences seen in treatment choices, psychiatrists' rating of additional treatment aspects was also dependent on the role they were asked to imagine themselves in. For both scenarios, psychiatrists in the what-would-you-do role behaved differently from psychiatrists in the self role, but similarly to the psychiatrists in the regular recommendation role (Table 1). Thus, psychiatrists in the what-would-you-do role and in the regular recommendation role recommended treatment options that were in accordance with treatment guidelines more than psychiatrists in the self role. In addition, they perceived the more invasive option (antidepressants in the depression scenario and depot medication in the schizophrenia scenario) as less unpleasant for the patient than psychiatrists in the self role perceived it for themselves.

Table 1 Influence of physicians' role on attitudes towards treatment optionsa

Mean (s.d.) Post hoc, mean difference (P)
Regular recommendation role What-would-you-do role Self role F Overall P What-would-you-do role v. regular recommendation role What-would-you-do role v. self role Regular recommendation role v. self role
Depression scenario
    Estimated effectiveness of antidepressant 4.63 (1.04) 4.71 (0.87) 4.56 (1.19) 0.43 0.65
    Estimated effectiveness of watchful waiting 3.07 (1.17) 2.87 (1.18) 3.55 (1.24) 7.39 <0.001 –0.20 (0.56) –0.68 (0.001) 0.48 (0.03)
    Importance that treatment choice/recommendation is in accordance with treatment guidelines 3.77 (1.27) 4.16 (1.16) 3.29 (1.55) 9.28 <0.001 0.39 (0.16) 0.88 (<0.001) 0.48 (0.06)
    Estimated unpleasantness of an antidepressant for the patient 3.02 (1.01) 2.80 (1.03) 3.45 (1.22) 7.73 <0.001 –0.22 (0.41) –0.65 (0.001) –0.42 (0.04)
Schizophrenia scenario
    Estimated effectiveness of depot antipsychotic 4.08 (1.09) 4.24 (0.95) 4.27 (1.35) 0.65 0.52
    Estimated effectiveness of oral antipsychotic 3.07 (1.03) 3.08 (1.06) 3.89 (1.25) 15.11 <0.001 0.01 (1.00) –0.81 (<0.001) –0.82 (<0.001)
    Importance that treatment choice/recommendation is in accordance with treatment guidelines 4.16 (1.08) 4.16 (1.14) 3.66 (1.66) 4.17 0.02 0.00 (1.00) 0.51 (0.05) –0.51 (0.05)
    Estimated unpleasantness of depot antipsychotic option for the patient 3.60 (1.14) 3.39 (1.22) 4.32 (1.44) 12.49 <0.001 –0.21 (0.56) –0.93 (<0.001) 0.72 (0.001)
    Estimated unpleasantness of oral antipsychotic for the patient 2.86 (0.99) 2.91 (1.02) 2.75 (1.26) 0.43 0.65

There were no differences between the roles regarding the perceived effectiveness of the more invasive treatments (antidepressants in the depression scenario and depot medication in the schizophrenia scenario). The less invasive options (watchful waiting in the depression scenario and oral medication in the schizophrenia scenario), however, were considered by psychiatrists in the self role to be more effective than by psychiatrists in the two other roles.

Paired t-tests revealed that in all three role conditions the more invasive options of antidepressant and depot medication were estimated to be more effective than the non-invasive options of watchful waiting and oral medication (depression scenario: what-would-you-do role: t = 10.46, P<0.001; regular recommendation role: t = 10.21, P<0.001; self role: t = 5.53, P<0.001; schizophrenia scenario: what-would-you-do role: t = 7.20, P<0.001; regular recommendation role: t = 6.39, P<0.001; self role: t = 2.33, P = 0.02).

Hierarchical logistic regression analysis to control for confounding factors

We performed a hierarchical logistic regression to test whether the role of the psychiatrists had, independently of the above-mentioned factors (for example importance of guidelines), influenced the choice of treatment. Additionally, we controlled for sociodemographic variables. In bloc 1 the independent variable was ‘role’. In bloc 2 the additional factors and sociodemographic variables were entered (Table 2). In bloc 1, we found a strong effect for role (depression scenario: R 2 = 0.19; schizophrenia scenario: R 2 = 0.20): psychiatrists in the self role more often chose the less invasive treatment option than psychiatrists in the what-would-you-do role (depression scenario: odds ratio (OR) = 5.96 (95% CI 2.95–12.06), P<0.001; schizophrenia scenario: OR = 8.43 (95% CI 3.96–17.96), P<0.001). However, psychiatrists in the regular recommendation role did not differ from psychiatrists in the what-would-you-do role (depression scenario: OR = 1.06 (95% CI 0.50–2.28), P = 0.88; schizophrenia scenario: OR = 1.71 (95% CI 0.91–3.22), P = 0.10). Bloc 2 (depression scenario: R 2 = 0.52; schizophrenia scenario: R 2 = 0.66) showed that the incremental effect of the additional variables entered in this bloc was also strong. In addition to role, other predictors, such as ‘unpleasantness of the invasive treatment option for the patient’, influenced the kind of treatment chosen/recommended.

Table 2 Logistic regression models for choosing the less invasive treatment options (watchful waiting in the depression scenario and oral antipsychotic in the schizophrenia scenario)

Depression scenarioa Schizophrenia scenariob
OR (95% CI) P R 2 OR (95% CI) P R 2
Bloc 1 0.19 0.20
Experimental condition
What-would-you-do role Reference Reference
Regular recommendation role 1.06 (0.50-2.28) 0.88 1.71 (0.91–3.22) 0.10
Self role 5.96 (2.95–12.06) <0.001 8.43 (3.96–17.96) < 0.001
Bloc 2 0.52 0.66
Experimental condition
What-would-you-do role Reference Reference
Regular recommendation role 1.03 (0.41–2.56) 0.96 1.66 (0.69–4.00) 0.26
Self role 5.01 (2.03–12.41) <0.001 11.66 (3.52–38.63) <0.001
Estimated effectiveness of invasive treatment optionc 0.65 (0.46–0.91) 0.01 0.58 (0.38–0.89) 0.01
Estimated effectiveness of non-invasive treatment optiond 2.55 (1.85–3.53) <0.001 3.21 (2.01–5.12) <0.001
Importance that treatment choice/recommendation is in accordance with treatment guidelines 1.05 (0.81–1.37) 0.71 1.07 (0.74–1.55) 0.71
Estimated unpleasantness of invasive treatment optiond 1.64 (1.18–2.27) 0.003 2.68 (1.86–3.87) <0.001
Estimated unpleasantness of non-invasive treatment optiond 0.43 (0.28–0.67) <0.001
Psychiatrists' gender
    Female Reference Reference
    Male 1.18 (0.56–2.49) 0.67 2.29 (0.98–5.35) 0.06
Psychiatrists' age 0.96 (0.87–1.05) 0.34 0.96 (0.89–1.04) 0.33
Psychiatrists' place of work
    Hospital Reference Reference
    Private practice 0.41 (0.16–1.08) 0.07 0.77 (0.28–2.16) 0.62
Psychiatrists' professional experience 1.01 (0.93–1.11) 0.76 1.02 (0.94–1.10) 0.63
Percentage of patients with depression (in schizophrenia scenario: schizophrenia) actually treated by participating psychiatrist) 0.99 (0.97–1.01) 0.46 0.98 (0.96–1.00) 0.12

Discussion

Our survey revealed two major findings. First, psychiatrists choosing a treatment for themselves predominantly selected a different treatment than the one recommended to patients by participants asked in the regular recommendation role. Psychiatrists preferred the less effective, less invasive and more risky options for themselves (i.e. watchful waiting and oral antipsychotics) whereas they recommended the more effective and invasive ones for the patient (i.e. antidepressants and depot injections). Second, psychiatrists asked by a patient, ‘what would you do if you were me, doctor?’ remained in their usual professional recommendation role and usually did not recommend the same treatment options as those most often chosen by psychiatrists deciding in the self role.

Why do psychiatrists choose different treatments for themselves than for their patients?

First of all, psychiatrists may see themselves as better educated than most of their patients and thus more competent to take action when the less invasive but also less effective treatment (for example ‘wait and see’) has failed. Doctors might assume that their patients are not reliable enough to return for more intense treatment should a less effective/less invasive treatment fail.

Second, psychiatrists might hesitate to recommend the more risky option (for example ‘wait and see’) to the patient, because they might be held accountable for negative consequences (for example a suicide attempt) of this decision. In fact, evidence shows that accountability to others motivates advice givers to act differently than when making a personal decision. Reference Jonas, Schulz-Hardt and Frey12

Third, personal decision makers have been reported to weigh dimensions of options differently than advice givers. Reference Kray and Gonzalez13,Reference Kray14 Physicians in our study more often preferred the more effective but also more inconvenient treatment options (antidepressant and depot medication) for patients than for themselves. This may be explained by physicians in their usual recommendation role tending to focus predominantly on outcomes and benefits of medication whereas personal decision makers (psychiatrists in the self role) also consider inconvenience, cost and adverse effects when deciding to start or continue medications. Reference Belcher, Fried, Agostini and Tinetti15

Fourth, psychiatrists' decision-making might also have been affected by omission bias. Psychiatrists in the self role might focus predominantly on the unpleasantness of the more invasive treatment option (for example needle pain of depot) whereas they do not consider the risk of omitting an effective treatment for relapse prevention. Previous research has shown that omission bias might be more frequent in personal decision makers than in advice givers. Reference Zikmund-Fisher, Sarr, Fagerlin and Ubel11

Furthermore, the study revealed that doctors in the self role estimated the less invasive options to be more effective than doctors surveyed under the other conditions. This finding might be explained by dissonance theory, Reference Festinger16 which states that after a decision, people tend to defend their decision in order to maintain consistency and to reduce cognitive dissonance. Reference Frey and Berkowitz17 Thus, considering a less invasive treatment option as more effective may help doctors who chose this option to reduce cognitive dissonance. The same might hold true for the finding that psychiatrists in the self role perceived treatment guidelines as less important for their own treatment decision.

Why did the question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ not motivate doctors to reveal their personal preferences?

There might be several reasons why doctors do not share their preferences with their patients, even when being asked for them. First, this quasi ‘private’ question is being asked in the context of a professional patient–physician consultation, with clear roles and legal responsibilities. Reference Heimer18,Reference Sharpe19 If psychiatrists reveal their personal preferences and a patient follows this recommendation (for example not to take drugs in the case of depression), the psychiatrists may – as discussed above – be held accountable for any negative consequences and possibly face legal challenge.

In addition, the question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ might itself be ambiguous. Do patients expect physicians to put themselves in the patient's situation, i.e. ‘If you had my medical status along with my personal history and my values, what would you do?’ Or do they expect physicians to reveal what they would do if they themselves were ill (i.e. with their own personal history and their own values – and with their own professional expertise)?

Finally, the question implies both seeing things as the patient experiences them and remaining oneself. Reference Zaner and Khushf2 However, being both oneself and the patient at the same time is mutually exclusive: ‘How can one person – with his own biological wherewithal, experiences, history, frame of mind etc., assume the place of another person – with his or her own unique personal, bodily, social, and historical situation?’ Reference Zaner and Khushf2 Answering the question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ might therefore be impossible. Reference Zaner and Khushf2,Reference Spiegelberg20

How can psychiatrists respond to the question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’

Should they decline to answer the question and thus leave the patient to make the decision alone? Or, should they give a ‘normal’ recommendation? One option might be to figure out why the person is asking such a question and what his or her concerns are and then discuss them with the individual. The physician may offer a recommendation from his/her professional (not personal) perspective, while accounting for the patient's personal situation and values when possible. Reference Baylis and Downie1,Reference Ubel21 However, psychiatrists should emphasise that their recommendations cannot replace the patient's active involvement in decision-making since they know themselves best. Reference Quill and Brody22 Thus, the physician's professional perspective and the patient's personal perspective are brought together to identify the best treatment option for the individual.

Limitations

Our experimental design potentially threatens external validity. It might be possible that physicians act differently in real-life situations and, for example, choose more effective treatment options when they really have depression or schizophrenia themselves. However, the experimental design of our study, including case vignettes, enabled the investigation of the influence of different roles in decision-making without being confounded by other factors (for example patient or situation characteristics Reference Jones, Gerrity and Earp23 ). Another limitation is that each participant of our study only passed through one of three experimental conditions and therefore role differences within an individual are unknown. However, taking into consideration that the participants might be more inclined to respond consistently Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff24 by choosing the same treatment option for all three roles, we selected a between-participants design that is used in most experiments investigating differences between personal decision makers and advice givers. Reference Kray and Gonzalez13,Reference Kray14,Reference Jonas and Frey25

Implications

The results of our study indicate that the question ‘What would you do if you were me, doctor?’ did not motivate psychiatrists to leave their professional recommendation role and to take a more personal perspective. Considering our results and given that physicians do not always make the optimal decisions for themselves, Reference Chambers and Belcher26,Reference Tyssen27 it is questionable whether motivating physicians to slip into the patient role would really lead to a better recommendation than one given by physicians in their professional role. Psychiatrists should try to find out why patients are asking this question and, together with that individual, identify the most appropriate treatment option.

Appendix

Case vignettes

Depression scenario

For regular recommendation role and what-would-you-do role. ‘Imagine that for the past 3 weeks a patient of yours has been feeling increasingly sad and tired and is now seeking your advice for treatment. The patient finds it a struggle to go to work, which has become more and more stressful for months. Even hobbies no longer provide any pleasure.’

Self role. ‘For the following experiment we would like you to imagine that for the past 3 weeks you have been feeling increasingly sad and tired. For this reason you are now seeking psychiatric treatment. You find it a struggle to go to work, which has become more and more stressful for months. Even your hobbies no longer provide any pleasure.’

Schizophrenia scenario

Regular recommendation role and what-would-you-do role. ‘Imagine that you are treating a patient with schizophrenia who is currently experiencing a relapse. You are aware that the patient sometimes forgets to take the necessary antipsychotics.’

Self role. ‘For the following experiment we would like you to imagine that you suffer from schizophrenia and are seeking psychiatric treatment. You are currently experiencing a relapse. Sometimes you forget to take the necessary antipsychotics.’

Acknowledgements

Grateful thanks to Rosa-Maria Conrad for the idea for this study and to all participating psychiatrists for their help.

Footnotes

See editorial, pp. 429–430, this issue.

Declaration of interest

R.M. has received honoraria from Janssen-Cilag. J.H. has received honoraria and/or research supports from Boehringer, Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Sanofi-Aventis, Astra Zeneca and Bristol-Myers Squibb. W.K. has received honoraria and/or research supports from Janssen-Cilag, Sanofi-Aventis, Johnson & Johnson, Pfizer, Bristol-Myers Squibb, AstraZeneca, Lundbeck, Novartis and Eli Lilly.

References

1 Baylis, F, Downie, J. Professional recommendations: disclosing facts and values. J Med Ethics 2001; 27: 20–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Zaner, R. Physicians and patients in relation – clinical interpretation and dialogues of trust. In Handbook of Bioethics – Taking Stock of the Field from a Philosophical Perspective (ed Khushf, G): 223–50. Kluwer Academic Publishers, 2004.Google Scholar
3 Hayes, DF. What would you do if this were your … wife, sister, mother, self? J Clin Oncol 1991; 9: 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 Gardner, M, Ogden, J. Do GPs practice what they preach? A questionnaire study of GPs' treatments for themselves and their patients. Patient Educ Couns 2005; 56: 112–5.Google ScholarPubMed
5 Steinert, T. Which neuroleptic would psychiatrists take for themselves or their relatives? Eur Psychiatry 2003; 18: 40–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Belanger, D, Moore, M, Tannock, I. How American oncologists treat breast cancer: an assessment of the influence of clinical trials. J Clin Oncol 1991; 9: 716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Domenighetti, G, Casabianca, A, Gutzwiller, F, Martinoli, S. Revisiting the most informed consumer of surgical services. The physician-patient. Int J Technol Assess Health Care 1993; 9: 505–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Amering, M, Denk, E, Griengl, H, Sibitz, I, Stastny, P. Psychiatric wills of mental health professionals: a survey of opinions regarding advance directives in psychiatry. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999; 34: 30–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Moncrieff, J, Kirsch, I. Efficacy of antidepressants in adults. BMJ 2005; 331: 155–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Raymark, PH. Accepting or rejecting medical treatment: a comparison of decisions made for self versus those made for a significant other. J Applied Soc Psychology 2000; 30: 2409–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11 Zikmund-Fisher, BJ, Sarr, B, Fagerlin, A, Ubel, PA. A matter of perspective: choosing for others differs from choosing for yourself in making treatment decisions. J Gen Intern Med 2006; 21: 618–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Jonas, E, Schulz-Hardt, S, Frey, D. Giving advice or making decisions in someone else's place: the influence of impression, defense, and accuracy motivation on the search for new information. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2005; 31: 977–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Kray, L, Gonzalez, R. Differential weighting in choice versus advice: I'll do this, you do that. J Behav Decis Making 1999; 12: 207–17.3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14 Kray, LJ. Contingent weighting in self-other decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 2000; 83: 82106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Belcher, VN, Fried, TR, Agostini, JV, Tinetti, ME. Views of older adults on patient participation in medication-related decision making. J Gen Intern Med 2006; 21: 298303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford University Press, 1957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
17 Frey, D. Recent research on selective exposure to information. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol 19 (ed Berkowitz, L): 4180. Academic Press, 1986.Google Scholar
18 Heimer, CA. Responsibility in health care: spanning the boundary between law and medicine. Wake Forest Law Rev 2006; 41: 465507.Google Scholar
19 Sharpe, VA. Behind closed doors: accountability and responsibility in patient care. J Med Philos 2000; 25: 2847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Spiegelberg, H. Steppingstones toward an Ethics for Fellow Existers. Essays 1944–1983. Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, 1986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21 Ubel, PA. “What should I do, doc?”: some psychologic benefits of physician recommendations. Arch Intern Med 2002; 162: 977–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 Quill, TE, Brody, H. Physician recommendations and patient autonomy: finding a balance between physician power and patient choice. Ann Intern Med 1996; 125: 763–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23 Jones, TV, Gerrity, MS, Earp, J. Written case simulations: do they predict physicians' behavior? J Clin Epidemiol 1990; 43: 805–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 Podsakoff, PM, MacKenzie, SB, Lee, JY, Podsakoff, NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol 2003; 88: 879903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25 Jonas, E, Frey, D. Information search and presentation in advisor-client interaction. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 2003; 91: 154–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26 Chambers, R, Belcher, J. Self-reported health care over the past 10 years: a survey of general practitioners. Br J Gen Pract 1992; 42: 153–6.Google Scholar
27 Tyssen, R. Health problems and the use of health services among physicians: a review article with particular emphasis on Norwegian studies. Ind Health 2007; 45: 599610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Procedure.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Chosen treatment options in relation to psychiatrists' role.

Figure 2

Table 1 Influence of physicians' role on attitudes towards treatment optionsa

Figure 3

Table 2 Logistic regression models for choosing the less invasive treatment options (watchful waiting in the depression scenario and oral antipsychotic in the schizophrenia scenario)

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.