Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:48:09.631Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Validity issues in transcultural epidemiology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Mark Van Ommeren*
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health and Substance Dependence, World Health Organization, Avenue Appia, 1211 Geneva 27, Switzerland. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © 2003 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

In a world with numerous refugees and increased concern for their well-being, governmental and non-governmental organisations are asking researchers for accurate estimates describing the extent of psychopathology in displaced populations. Although exact numbers are sought, the researcher soon learns that answers are filled with uncertainty. Turner and colleagues in this issue show that results from different assessment methods among Kosovan Albanian refugees in the UK do not agree with each other (Reference Turner, Bowie and DunnTurner et al, 2003, this issue). An Albanian-speaking clinician administering diagnostic measures identified relatively low prevalence rates of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression compared with rates obtained from self-report measures in the same sub-sample. Studies of help-seeking Cambodian refugees in specialised clinics in the USA have indicated PTSD prevalence rates ranging between 22% and 92% (Reference Abueg, Chun, Marsella, Friedman and GerrityAbueg & Chun, 1996). Also, my colleagues and I have been confronted with quite different prevalence rates in two studies of a sample of Bhutanese refugees in Nepal (Reference Shrestha, Sharma and Van OmmerenShrestha et al, 1998; Reference Van Ommeren, de Jong and SharmaVan Ommeren et al, 2001).

Inconsistent findings in any research effort may result from random processes and non-equivalent measures, procedures, or samples, but may also be explained by problems of low validity. Problems of validity are not new to epidemiology (Reference DohrenwendDohrenwend, 1990), but are more likely to occur in transcultural epidemiology — defined here as research in which the views, concepts or measures of the investigator extend beyond the scope of one cultural unit to another (Reference PrincePrince, 1997).

Although crossing cultural units may be experienced as exotic or romantic, it is best to stay with good old conventional terminology to examine the effects of culture on the validity of transcultural studies. Dimensions of validity of field research have been conceptualised by Cook & Campbell (Reference Cook and Campbell1979) and clarified by Gliner & Morgan (Reference Gliner and Morgan2000). Table 1 presents definitions of classic types and subtypes of evidence of validity. Surprisingly, systematic and correct analysis of validity is uncommon in transcultural epidemiology. Rather, in the debate about the validity of transcultural studies, expressed opinions tend to be at polar ends — ranging from dismissing findings as socially constructed medicalisation of social distress to presuming that epidemiological constructs, methods and findings are not affected by context.

Table 1 Types of evidence for the validity of a research study

Type and subtype Definition
Research validity Extent of validity of the whole study
Measurement validity Extent to which measure assesses what it purports to measure for a particular setting, population and purpose
Measurement reliability (e.g. test—retest, interrater and internal consistency) Consistency of scores from the measure for a particular setting, population and purpose
Construct validity (e.g. discriminant, convergent and factorial evidence) Extent to which a measure assesses the theoretical construct it is intended to measure
Diagnostic validity Extent to which a category meets a consensus definition of psychiatric disorder and is distinguishable from other disorders
Content validity Extent to which the measure's content represents the concept(s) to be measured
Criterion-related validity (i.e. predictive or concurrent evidence) Strength of relationship with a measurable external criterion
Statistical validity Proper use and interpretation of statistical methods and power
Internal validity (i.e. group equivalence, control of independent variables) Extent to which a significant relationship is a causal relationship and not explicable by a third variable
External validity Extent of generalisability to the target populations, to other populations, and across time and place
Population validity Extent to which a sample represents the target population
Ecological validity Extent of generalisability of findings across time and place to real life

The aim of this editorial is to generate awareness about the various ways in which context affects research validity. Such awareness may facilitate the identification and implementation of realistic and effective methods to reduce uncertainty in findings of transcultural studies.

MEASUREMENT VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY

Measurement validity and reliability (Table 1) are established in relation to the measure's intended purpose. Evidence of measurement validity and reliability cannot be assumed to generalise across populations. This lack of generalisability may be especially problematic when the original measure is translated into another language, as is common in transcultural studies. Creating a culturally acceptable, comprehensible, relevant and semantically equivalent translation is difficult (Reference Van Ommeren, Sharma and ThapaVan Ommeren et al, 1999), making it essential to study the internal consistency and test—retest reliability of translated measures that might have changed during imperfect translations.

Construct and diagnostic validity

Construct validity is the degree to which a measure assesses the theoretical construct it has been designed for. If one assumes that diagnoses are atheoretical — as the later versions of the DSM strive to do — then trying to establish construct validity for measures of diagnoses is somewhat illogical. Avoiding this language issue, we discuss ‘diagnostic validity’, which is the extent to which a cluster of symptoms is markedly distressing or sufficiently impairing to warrant the label ‘psychiatric disorder’, and also is distinguishable from other disorders in terms of symptoms, course, clinical features, laboratory findings and findings from family studies (cf. Reference Robins and GuzeRobins & Guze, 1970). Systems of diagnosis such as the DSM and ICD cannot be presumed to have high diagnostic validity across cultures, because there is evidence that sociocultural factors in varying degrees influence the clustering of symptoms and the extent to which symptoms are experienced as distressing (Reference Mezzich, Kleinman and FabregaMezzich et al, 1996).

Should the transcultural epidemiologist provide evidence of diagnostic validity in each research context? Researching evidence of diagnostic validity is a lengthy process. The current Western systems of disorders, DSM—IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and ICD—10 (World Health Organization, 1992), have been created by numerous leading mental health researchers, who have had available more than a century of Western psychiatric and psychological literature, extensive data-sets for reanalysis, and, in the case of DSM—IV, funding for in-depth field trials. Even then, evidence of diagnostic validity is still sparse for many disorders. Accordingly, it may not always be realistic for transcultural epidemiologists to research diagnostic validity for the disorders they assess in various contexts. Nevertheless, this area of study benefits from continuous efforts to validate diagnostic categories (including the socalled ‘culture-bound’ disorders) in different contexts. The aforementioned definition of diagnostic validity suggests that diagnostic validation is achieved through laboratory and family studies as well as through epidemiological and ethnographic studies of distress, disability, symptoms, course and clinical features.

Content and criterion-related validity

Literal translation can reduce a measure's content validity, which is the extent to which a measure's content represents the concept to be assessed. For example, the widely used Short Form—12 (Reference Ware, Kosinki and KellerWare et al, 1996) contains the terms ‘bowling’ and ‘playing golf’ to assess physical functioning — terms that are unknown to many respondents in low-income countries. To use the Short Form—12 in such countries, locally meaningful equivalent terms must be substituted to maintain content validity.

Epidemiologists tend to focus their efforts on establishing criterion-related validity, which is the strength of relation between the measure and a measurable external criterion. The ideal external criterion is considered to be diagnosis by independent clinicians who are trained in using a semi-structured diagnostic instrument that has evidence of measurement validity and reliability (especially interrater reliability) for the local context. This poses a problem for transcultural epidemiology, because research is frequently conducted in contexts with very few mental health professionals, who may not have been trained in the use of standard semi-structured diagnostic instruments, which themselves seldom have any psychometric evidence for the local context.

Even though the aforementioned assessment standard of criterion-related validity is unlikely to occur in transcultural epidemiology, the researcher should try to gather data to test this validity. This effort is one of the strengths of the study by Turner et al in this issue.

INTERNAL AND EXTERNAL VALIDITY

Attempts to identify causes for differences in epidemiological findings between two sociocultural settings often have low internal validity. Internal validity refers to the degree to which a significant relationship is a causal relationship and is not explicable by a third variable. Societies can differ in so many ways that it is difficult to prove that one variable is one of the causes of differences in epidemiological findings. Rather than finding causes for different prevalence rates across settings, it might be more realistic to compare patterns of findings across settings — see, for example, Patel et al (Reference Patel, Araya and Lima1999) and de Jong et al (Reference de Jong, Komproe and Van Ommeren2001).

Users of epidemiological data (such as policy-makers) need to know to what extent findings have external validity, i.e. generalisability to the target population, to other populations, and across time and place. Generalisability to the target population depends on the ability to randomly draw a representative sample from the entire population of relevant persons. The ability to do so requires the availability of reliable registers with contact information for the entire target population. However, the availability and quality of population registers vary and are likely to be poor in countries with fewer resources. Generalisability to the target population also depends on the study's participation rate, i.e. the percentage of sampled people who are willing to participate in the study. Fortunately, participation rates appear to be much higher in research outside the industrialised world.

The extent to which findings from one cultural unit can be generalised to other populations is still open to debate. Can we generalise findings from one continent to another, or from one ethnic group to another within the same country? We still know little of the generalisability of epidemiological findings across populations. Multi-site studies are the answer. Moreover, in rapidly changing societies longitudinal studies may assess the extent to which findings generalise over time.

CONCLUSIONS

Systematically considering and addressing validity issues will reduce uncertainty in findings from transcultural epidemiological studies. The challenges inherent in addressing these issues are no reason for discouragement. Validity is a continuous construct. Perfectly valid studies tend to be unlikely in any science. A study certaintly does not have to be highly valid in every regard to be valuable or useful. Yet, a sustained focus on validity issues — as has been demonstrated in the USA (Reference Narrow, Rae and RobinsNarrow et al, 2002) — will guide researchers to more-exact and useful epidemiological estimates.

Acknowledgements

This paper has benefited from comments by Rob Baltussen, Etzel Cardeña, Daniel Chisholm, Laurence Kirmayer, Joop de Jong, George Morgan, Michael Spittel and Jos Van Ommeren.

Footnotes

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

None.

References

Abueg, F. R. & Chun, K. M. (1996) Traumatization stress among Asians and Asian Americans. In Ethnocultural Aspects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Issues, Research, and Clinical Applications (eds Marsella, A.J., Friedman, M.J., Gerrity, E.T., et al). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM—IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Cook, T. D. & Campbell, D. T. (1979) Quasi-experimentation: Design and Analysis Issues for Field Settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
de Jong, J. T. V. M., Komproe, I. H., Van Ommeren, M., et al (2001) Lifetime events and posttraumatic stress disorder in 4 postconflict settings. JAMA, 286, 555-562.Google Scholar
Dohrenwend, B. P. (1990) ‘The problem of validity in field studies of psychological disorders’ revisited. Psychological Medicine, 20, 195-208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gliner, J. A. & Morgan, G. A. (2000) Research Methods in Applied Settings: An Integrated Approach to Design and Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mezzich, J. E., Kleinman, A., Fabrega, H., et al (eds) (1996) Culture and Psychiatric Diagnosis. Washington: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Narrow, W. E., Rae, D. S., Robins, L. N., et al (2002) Revised prevalence estimates of mental disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 115-123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patel, V., Araya, R., Lima, M. S., et al (1999) Women, poverty and common mental disorders in four restructuring societies. Social Science and Medicine, 49, 1461-1471.Google Scholar
Prince, R. H. (1997) What's in a name? Transcultural Psychiatry, 34, 151-154.Google Scholar
Robins, E. & Guze, S. B. (1970) Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: its application to schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 107-111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shrestha, N. M., Sharma, B., Van Ommeren, M., et al (1998) Impact of torture on refugees displaced within the developing world: symptomatology among Bhutanese refugees in Nepal. JAMA 280, 443-448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, S. W., Bowie, C., Dunn, G., et al (2003) Mental health of Kosovan Albanian refugees in the UK. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 444-448.Google Scholar
Van Ommeren, M., Sharma, B., Thapa, S., et al (1999) Preparing instruments for transcultural research: use of the Translation Monitoring Form with Nepali-speaking Bhutanese refugees. Transcultural Psychiatry, 36, 285-301.Google Scholar
Van Ommeren, , de Jong, J. T. V. M., Sharma, B., et al (2001) Psychiatric disorders among tortured Bhutanese refugees in Nepal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 475-482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, J. E., Kosinki, M., & Keller, S. D. (1996) A 12 item short form health survey: construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical Care, 34, 220-233.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992) Tenth Revision of the International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD—10). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Types of evidence for the validity of a research study

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.