Exposure to trauma – namely, an event that involves danger of death, serious injury or sexual violation1 – is an important risk factor for mental illness. Descriptions of trauma-related mental illness largely originated from investigations of people exposed to traumas that occurred in adulthood (e.g. military combat) and/or in single instances (e.g. disasters). These descriptions led to the definition of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),Reference Andreasen2 which informs assessment and treatment after all traumas.Reference Ehlers and Clark3 However, clinical observations have suggested that more ‘complex’ types of trauma – namely, a traumatic experience involving multiple events with interpersonal threats during childhood or adolescence (e.g. repeated child abuse) – might result in more severe outcomes than other ‘non-complex’ traumas.Reference Terr4–Reference Cook, Blaustein, Spinazzola and van der Kolk7 These clinical observations have become very influential in clinical practice; for instance, leading to proposed new diagnoses linked with complex trauma exposure, such as complex PTSD.Reference Herman5,Reference Maercker, Brewin, Bryant, Cloitre, van Ommeren and Jones8,Reference van der Kolk9 Yet, they have not been comprehensively tested in population studies, leaving important knowledge gaps in the literature.
Psychopathology and cognitive deficits
It is unclear if people exposed to complex trauma have more severe psychopathology and cognitive deficits than those exposed to non-complex trauma, or trauma-unexposed peers. Although a small number of studies have found that complex trauma is associated with broader psychopathology and poorer cognitive function than non-complex trauma,Reference van der Kolk, Roth, Pelcovitz, Sunday and Spinazzola10–Reference op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer13 these studies focused on a restricted range of outcomes and used clinical/convenience samples. Other studies have investigated exposures that, in some cases, overlap with the construct of complex trauma (e.g. child victimisation, maltreatment or adverse childhood experiences), and have found that these exposures are also associated with psychopathology and cognitive deficits.Reference Schaefer, Moffitt, Arseneault, Danese, Fisher and Houts14,Reference Danese, Moffitt, Arseneault, Bleiberg, Dinardo and Gandelman15 However, because in other cases such exposures do not overlap with the construct of complex trauma (as shown in Supplementary Fig. 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.57), the relevance of those findings to complex trauma is unclear. Furthermore, there has been no direct comparison of such exposures with other potentially traumatic events that would be classified as non-complex traumas. Previous studies are therefore unlikely to have fully captured the psychopathology and cognitive deficits that are uniquely linked with complex trauma in the population.
Possible underlying mechanisms
It is also unclear why complex trauma exposure may be associated with more severe psychopathology and cognitive deficits. The dominant hypothesis is that complex trauma exerts more detrimental effects on the brain than non-complex trauma, because of repeated activation of the stress response during sensitive developmental periods, giving rise to more severe psychopathology and cognitive deficits.Reference Danese and Lewis16 An alternative hypothesis is that individuals exposed to complex trauma have greater pre-existing vulnerability, which may confound the associations of complex trauma with psychopathology and cognitive deficits.Reference Danese, Moffitt, Arseneault, Bleiberg, Dinardo and Gandelman15 Although experimental research using animal models has shown that early-life stress can have causal effects on later behaviours, most human research undertaken to test the translational validity of these findings has been cross-sectional, lacking pre-trauma assessments or genetically sensitive designs needed to investigate the role of pre-existing vulnerabilities.Reference Danese17 A small number of studies of victimisation have tested for confounding by pre-existing vulnerabilities, using longitudinal methods that accounted for pre-victimisation measures or twin designs that accounted for genetic and family-wide environmental factors, and have found mixed results.Reference Schaefer, Moffitt, Arseneault, Danese, Fisher and Houts14,Reference Danese, Moffitt, Arseneault, Bleiberg, Dinardo and Gandelman15,Reference Silberg, Copeland, Linker, Moore, Roberson-Nay and York18,Reference Dinkler, Lundström, Gajwani, Lichtenstein, Gillberg and Minnis19 However, no studies of complex trauma have assessed the role of pre-existing vulnerabilities to date.
In this study, we aimed to address these knowledge gaps. To better understand psychopathology and cognitive deficits linked with complex trauma, we investigated differences across a broad range of measures of psychopathology and cognitive function in young people exposed to complex, non-complex and no trauma, in a large population-representative cohort of twins. To explore the origins of these presentations, we investigated the role of pre-existing vulnerabilities measured prospectively in early childhood, and undertook twin-difference analyses.
Method
Sample
Participants were members of the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study, which tracks the development of a birth cohort of 2232 British children. The sample was drawn from a larger birth register of twins born in England and Wales in 1994–1995.Reference Trouton, Spinath and Plomin20 Full details about the sample are reported elsewhere.Reference Moffitt21 Briefly, the E-Risk sample was constructed in 1999–2000, when 1116 families (93% of those eligible) with same-sex 5-year-old twins participated in home visit assessments. This sample comprised 56% monozygotic (MZ) and 44% dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs; sex was evenly distributed within zygosity (49% male). Families were recruited to represent the UK population of families with new-borns in the 1990s, on the basis of residential location throughout England and Wales and mother's age. Teenaged mothers with twins were overselected to replace high-risk families who were selectively lost to the register through non-response. Older mothers having twins via assisted reproduction were underselected to avoid an excess of well-educated older mothers. The study sample represents the full range of socioeconomic conditions in the UK, as reflected in the families’ distribution on a neighbourhood-level socioeconomic index (called ACORN (A Classification of Residential Neighbourhoods), developed by CACI Inc. for commercial use in Great Britain):Reference Odgers, Caspi, Russell, Sampson, Arseneault and Moffitt22 25.6% of E-Risk families live in ‘wealthy achiever’ neighbourhoods compared with 25.3% nationwide; 5.3% live in ‘urban prosperity’ neighbourhoods compared with 11.6% nationwide; 29.6% live in ‘comfortably off’ neighbourhoods compared with 26.9% nationwide; 13.4% live in ‘moderate means’ neighbourhoods compared with 13.9% nationwide; and 26.1% live in ‘hard-pressed’ neighbourhoods compared with 20.7% nationwide. E-Risk underrepresents ‘urban prosperity’ neighbourhoods because such households are likely to be childless.
Follow-up home visits were conducted when the children were aged 7 (98% participation), 10 (96% participation), 12 (96% participation) and 18 (93% participation) years. Home visits at ages 5, 7, 10 and 12 years included assessments with participants and their mother (or primary care-taker); the home visit at age 18 years included interviews only with participants. Each twin participant was assessed by a different interviewer. There were 2066 children who participated in the E-Risk assessments at age 18 years, and the proportions of MZ (55%) and male same-sex (47%) twins were almost identical to those found in the original sample at age 5 years. The average age of the twins at the time of assessment was 18.4 years (s.d. 0.36); all interviews were conducted after their 18th birthday. The study sample at age 18 years was equally distributed across all deciles of the Index of Multiple Deprivation 2015, which measures relative levels of deprivation in small areas in England (Supplementary Fig. 2). There were no differences between those who did and did not take part at age 18 years, in terms of socioeconomic status assessed when the cohort was initially defined (χ 2 = 0.86, P = 0.65), internalising or externalising symptoms at age 5 years (t = 0.40, P = 0.69 and t = 0.41, P = 0.68, respectively) or IQ scores at age 5 years (t = 0.98, P = 0.33).
We assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures were approved by the Joint South London and Maudsley and the Institute of Psychiatry Research Ethics Committee (reference 1997/122). Parents gave written informed consent, and twins gave assent between 5–12 years and then written informed consent at age 18 years.
Trauma exposure
Trauma exposure was assessed at age 18 years, during private interviews. Participants were asked whether they had been exposed to trauma during their lifetime, according to DSM-5 PTSD criterion A.1 Participants who reported trauma exposure were then asked to describe the traumas they had experienced. We reviewed these descriptions, alongside information gathered in the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire, Second Revision, adapted as a clinical interview,Reference Fisher, Caspi, Moffitt, Wertz, Gray and Newbury23 to identify participants who had been exposed to complex trauma and non-complex trauma. We defined complex trauma exposure as (a) a traumatic experience that involved (b) multiple events that were (c) interpersonal assaults or threats and (d) occurred in childhood or adolescence. These criteria were selected because they have been consistently highlighted as key elements for the definition of complex trauma in previous literature, supported by evidence suggesting that each criterion considered alone is associated with psychopathology or cognitive deficits.Reference Terr4–Reference Cook, Blaustein, Spinazzola and van der Kolk7 Examples of complex traumas included repeated child abuse, severe bullying and witnessing neighbourhood violence. Participants were classified as having experienced non-complex trauma if they were exposed to trauma that involved a single event or non-interpersonal events only (i.e. their experience met criterion (a), but not all other criteria needed to indicate complex trauma exposure in their lifetime). Examples of non-complex traumas included a one-off assault, an accident and learning about the sudden death of a parent. Two mutually exclusive groups of trauma-exposed participants were therefore formed. Further details are provided in Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. 1. Trauma dossiers for each trauma-exposed participant were coded to indicate complex or non-complex trauma exposure by two psychiatrists (interrater reliability of coding for trauma-exposed participants: κ = 0.86). Of all participants with available data, 9.1% (188/2064) reported complex trauma, 21.7% (448/2064) reported non-complex trauma and 0.3% (6/2064) reported trauma but declined to provide a description (excluded from these analyses).
Psychopathology
We focused on a measure of general psychopathology, ‘p’, which was derived by fitting a bi-factor model to 11 symptom scales (PTSD, major depressive disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, disordered eating, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), conduct disorder, alcohol dependence, cannabis dependence, nicotine dependence, psychotic symptoms and prodromal symptoms) obtained from data collected during interviews conducted at age 18 years, about psychopathology in the previous year.Reference Schaefer, Moffitt, Arseneault, Danese, Fisher and Houts14 Scores were scaled to a mean of 100 and s.d. of 15. To investigate more specific and clinically relevant measures of psychopathology, we also considered the count and occurrence of nine psychiatric disorders (PTSD, major depressive disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, ADHD, conduct disorder, alcohol dependence, cannabis dependence, nicotine dependence and psychotic symptoms) in the previous year, assessed during interviews conducted at age 18 years. Details are provided in Supplementary Table 1.
Cognitive function
We focused on a measure of general intelligence, IQ, which was tested at age 18 years, using a short version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition.Reference Wechsler24 To investigate more specific aspects of cognitive function, we also considered executive function and processing speed, assessed with subtests of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated BatteryReference Cambridge Cognition25 (including rapid visual information processing (RVP), spatial working memory (SWM) and spatial span subtests) at age 18 years. Scores were scaled to a mean of 100 and s.d. of 15, and reverse-coded for tests where lower scores indicate better cognitive functioning, so that for all measures of cognitive function lower scores indicate poorer functioning. Details are provided in Supplementary Table 1.
Early childhood vulnerabilities
Participants were prospectively assessed for early childhood characteristics: internalising and externalising symptoms at age 5 years, IQ score at age 5 years, proportion of family members with a history of mental illness, family socioeconomic status and sex (measures described in Supplementary Table 1). These characteristics were selected for this analysis a priori, to comprehensively capture vulnerabilities known to be associated with trauma exposure, psychopathology and cognitive deficits,Reference Koenen, Moffitt, Poulton, Martin and Caspi26–Reference Deary, Pattie and Starr28 which might therefore confound observed associations. Because most trauma-exposed participants reported that their most upsetting trauma occurred during adolescence (age ≥12 years: 529/635 total, 141/187 complex, 388/448 non-complex trauma-exposed), and because very-early-life events are unlikely to have been recalled during the trauma interview, these characteristics likely capture pre-trauma vulnerabilities.
Statistical analysis
To investigate psychopathology and cognitive deficits linked with complex and non-complex trauma in young people, we compared participants exposed to complex trauma or non-complex trauma with trauma-unexposed participants. Additionally, to investigate psychopathology and cognitive deficits uniquely linked with complex trauma within trauma-exposed individuals, we compared participants exposed to complex trauma with those exposed to non-complex trauma.
First, we tested group differences in psychopathology and cognitive function. Next, to map possible confounding factors for the observed associations, we tested whether participants with greater early childhood vulnerability were at higher risk of complex or non-complex trauma exposure. Then, to examine possible confounding effects in the associations of traumas with psychopathology and cognitive deficits, we tested group differences in psychopathology and cognitive function and covaried early childhood vulnerabilities. All of these analyses used generalised estimating equation regression models, accounting for clustering within families. Additionally, we tested whether unobserved genetic or family-wide environmental factors accounted for psychopathology or cognitive deficits linked to complex or non-complex traumas, by examining whether differences in trauma exposure were correlated with differences in ‘p’ or IQ score within twin pairs. Further details are provided in the Supplementary Methods.
Results
Is complex trauma exposure associated with more severe psychopathology?
We first examined group differences in general psychopathology, ‘p’, at age 18 years (Fig. 1(a)). Compared with trauma-unexposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had higher scores of ‘p’ (regression coefficient β = 16.49, 95% CI 13.97–18.96), and so did those exposed to non-complex trauma (β = 8.16, 95% CI 6.72–9.60). Within trauma-exposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had higher scores of ‘p’ than participants exposed to non-complex trauma (β = 9.30, 95% CI 6.47–12.12).
We then considered specific psychiatric disorders (Fig. 1(b) and 1(c)). Compared with trauma-unexposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had more psychiatric disorders and were more likely to experience each psychiatric disorder assessed. In addition, compared with trauma-unexposed participants, those exposed to non-complex trauma had more psychiatric disorders and were more likely to experience major depressive disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, ADHD, conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. Within trauma-exposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had more psychiatric disorders and were more likely to experience PTSD, major depressive disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, conduct disorder, cannabis dependence, nicotine dependence and psychotic symptoms than participants exposed to non-complex trauma.
Is complex trauma exposure associated with poorer cognitive function?
We first examined group differences in general intelligence, determined by IQ score, at age 18 years (Fig. 2(a)). Compared with trauma-unexposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had lower IQ scores (β = −2.46, 95% CI −4.48 to −0.44). However, participants exposed to non-complex trauma did not have significantly lower IQ scores than those not exposed to trauma (β = 0.94, 95% CI −0.39 to 2.27). Within trauma-exposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had lower IQ scores than participants exposed to non-complex trauma (β = −4.55, 95% CI −6.89 to −2.20).
We then considered executive function and processing speed (Fig. 2(b) and 2(c)). Compared with trauma-unexposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had poorer functioning on several measures of executive function (RVP A’, SWM errors, spatial span and spatial span reversed) and on both measures of processing speed. However, participants exposed to non-complex trauma did not have significantly different scores on measures of these cognitive functions compared with participants not exposed to trauma. Within trauma-exposed participants, those exposed to complex trauma had poorer functioning on several measures of executive function (RVP A’, SWM errors and spatial span) and on both measures of processing speed than participants exposed to non-complex trauma.
Is greater early childhood vulnerability associated with higher risk of complex trauma exposure?
Most of the a priori selected early childhood vulnerabilities were associated with complex trauma exposure over no trauma exposure. In contrast, the only early vulnerability significantly associated with non-complex trauma exposure over no trauma exposure was family history of mental illness. Likewise, most early vulnerabilities were associated with complex trauma exposure over non-complex trauma exposure (Table 1).
Unadjusted relative risk ratios (RRR) with 95% confidence intervals are presented for associations between early childhood vulnerabilities and trauma exposure in all participants (n = 2004–2058). We provide results of multinomial models with no trauma as the base of the dependent variable, and with non-complex trauma as the base of the dependent variable (results for no trauma over non-complex trauma have been omitted, as the more meaningful results for non-complex trauma over no trauma have been provided). Early childhood vulnerabilities measured on a continuous scale had been standardised to a mean of 0 and s.d. of 1 (internalising and externalising symptoms at age 5 years, IQ at age 5 years and proportion of family members with a history of mental illness). Bold text signifies P < 0.05.
Do early childhood vulnerabilities account for psychopathology linked to complex trauma?
We found that early childhood vulnerabilities did not explain the associations of complex or non-complex trauma exposure with psychopathology. The association between complex trauma (versus no trauma) and ‘p’ did not substantially change after accounting for all early childhood vulnerabilities (β reduced in magnitude by 7.0% and remained statistically significant) (Fig. 3(a) column 1, and Supplementary Table 2(a)). Similarly, the association between non-complex trauma (versus no trauma) and ‘p’ did not substantially change after early childhood vulnerabilities were taken into account (β reduced in magnitude by 2.9% and remained statistically significant) (Fig. 3(a) column 2, and Supplementary Table 2(a)). Within trauma-exposed participants, the association between complex trauma (versus non-complex trauma) and ‘p’ did not substantially change after accounting for early childhood vulnerabilities (β reduced in magnitude by 15.5% and remained statistically significant) (Fig. 3(a) column 3, and Supplementary Table 2(a)). Twin analyses found consistent results. Within pairs of twins who grew up in the same family environment and share some (DZ twins) or all (MZ twins) of their genetic material, participants exposed to complex trauma had higher scores of ‘p’ than their co-twin not exposed to complex trauma. This indicates that complex trauma is associated with ‘p’ independent of family environment and genetic risk (Supplementary Table 3). When considering the count and occurrence of psychiatric disorders, similar results were also found (Fig. 3(b) and 3(c) columns 1–3, and Supplementary Table 2(b) and 2(c)).
Do early childhood vulnerabilities account for cognitive deficits linked to complex trauma?
We found that early childhood vulnerabilities largely accounted for the associations of complex trauma with lower IQ score and poorer executive function, but not the associations with slower processing speed. The association between complex trauma (versus no trauma) and IQ score was substantially attenuated after early childhood vulnerabilities were taken into account (β reduced in magnitude by 63.6% and became statistically non-significant) (Fig. 4(a) column 1, and Supplementary Table 4(a)). As in unadjusted analyses, non-complex trauma (versus no trauma) was not associated with IQ score after accounting for early childhood vulnerabilities (Fig. 4(a) column 2, and Supplementary Table 4(a)). Within trauma-exposed participants, the association between complex trauma (versus non-complex trauma) and IQ score was substantially attenuated after accounting for early childhood vulnerabilities (β reduced in magnitude by 54.1% and became statistically non-significant) (Fig. 4(a) column 3, and Supplementary Table 4(a)). Twin analyses found consistent results. Within twin pairs who grew up in the same family environment and share genetic material, complex trauma was not correlated with IQ score. This suggests that family environment and genetic factors likely explain the associations observed at the individual level (Supplementary Table 5). When considering executive function and processing speed, early childhood vulnerabilities accounted for the associations of complex trauma (versus no trauma) with some measures of executive function (RVP A’, SWM errors and spatial span reversed), but not processing speed (Fig. 4(b) and 4(c) column 1, and Supplementary Table 4(b) and 4(c)). Finally, within trauma-exposed participants, early childhood vulnerabilities accounted for the associations of complex trauma (versus non-complex trauma) with some measures of executive function (RVP A’ and SWM errors), but not processing speed (Fig. 4(b) and 4(c) column 3, and Supplementary Table 4(b) and 4(c)).
Discussion
We found that young people exposed to complex traumas had more severe psychopathology and poorer cognitive function compared with trauma-unexposed peers and those exposed to non-complex traumas. Although their psychopathology was considerably more severe, their cognitive deficits were relatively modest on average. Early childhood vulnerabilities had an important role in these presentations, as they predicted later exposure to complex trauma and largely explained the associations between complex trauma and cognitive deficits. Nevertheless, psychopathology remained associated with complex trauma even after accounting for early childhood vulnerabilities. These findings from a large, population-based cohort strengthen and extend the limited empirical evidence in this area,Reference van der Kolk, Roth, Pelcovitz, Sunday and Spinazzola10–Reference op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer13 and caution that conflating complex and non-complex traumas in research and clinical practice has likely led to an underestimation of psychopathology, cognitive deficits and pre-existing vulnerabilities linked with complex trauma.
Limitations
Our findings should be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, lifetime trauma exposure was measured by retrospective self-report at age 18 years, when psychopathology and cognitive function were also assessed. As such, it is possible that our results partly reflect common method bias. However, these findings based on self-reported complex trauma are consistent with those from studies of victimisation measured prospectively or through informants,Reference Schaefer, Moffitt, Arseneault, Danese, Fisher and Houts14,Reference Danese, Moffitt, Arseneault, Bleiberg, Dinardo and Gandelman15 suggesting that common method bias is unlikely to fully explain our results. Regardless of the underlying mechanisms, our findings suggest that self-reports of complex trauma can pragmatically be used to identify a group of individuals with greater mental health needs than peers reporting non-complex trauma or no trauma. Second, our findings may only be valid within the boundaries of our measures. For example, complex traumas in pre-school years (unlikely to be retrospectively recalled and possibly masked by our adjustment for vulnerabilities at age 5 years) or complex trauma types not observed in our study (e.g. living in conflict areas) may have different sequelae. Furthermore, it is unclear if patterns similar to the ones described here would apply to other aspects of psychopathology (e.g. complex PTSD, personality disorders or autism spectrum disorders) or cognitive functions (e.g. reward or threat processing) not included in our study. Third, findings from our twin sample might not generalise to singletons. However, the prevalence of trauma and psychiatric disorders and the strength of the associations observed in our sample are similar to those found in samples of singletons,Reference Lewis, Arseneault, Caspi, Fisher, Matthews and Moffitt29 supporting the generalisability of our results. Despite these limitations, our findings have implications for research and clinical practice.
Implications
Our findings demonstrate that individuals exposed to complex trauma have greater mental health needs than those exposed to non-complex trauma, since they experience more severe psychopathology and poorer cognitive function. These findings may reflect qualitative differences between complex and non-complex trauma as originally proposed,Reference Terr4–Reference Cook, Blaustein, Spinazzola and van der Kolk7 or may reflect quantitative differences where complex traumas are at the upper end of a trauma continuum, for instance because of high severity or frequency. Nevertheless, underappreciation of these differences in trauma research and clinical practice has likely led to underrecognition of the greater mental health needs of people exposed to complex traumas. Research that further explores these differences will therefore be valuable to better understand and address trauma-related mental health needs.
Individuals exposed to non-complex trauma have less severe psychopathology than those exposed to complex trauma, but they nevertheless have more severe psychopathology than trauma-unexposed peers. The higher risk of psychopathology in people exposed to non-complex trauma is not limited to PTSD, as originally implied,Reference Terr4–Reference Cook, Blaustein, Spinazzola and van der Kolk7 but extends across several internalising and externalising disorders. Furthermore, because non-complex trauma has higher prevalence than complex trauma, non-complex trauma contributes to a large number of cases of trauma-related psychopathology in the population.
To map the broad mental health needs of people exposed to trauma, and particularly complex trauma, it is therefore necessary to undertake comprehensive assessments that evaluate wide-ranging psychopathology and cognitive deficits, both in research and clinical practice. Furthermore, research is needed to understand how to address these needs most effectively, including investigating whether broader treatment approaches might be more helpful than standard treatments, and whether these might be more useful for individuals exposed to complex trauma compared with those exposed to non-complex trauma. Such treatments might involve phase-based approaches that target symptoms sequentially,Reference Cloitre, Koenen, Cohen and Han30,Reference Cohen, Mannarino, Kliethermes and Murray31 or trans-diagnostic approaches that target common mechanisms underlying the cooccurrence of disorders.Reference Dalgleish, Black, Johnston and Bevan32
Our findings also indicate that pre-existing vulnerabilities predict complex trauma, but not non-complex trauma exposure, and largely explain associations between complex trauma and cognitive deficits. These findings challenge the hypothesis that complex trauma leads to effects on the brain detectable through cognitive deficits,Reference Danese and Lewis16 and therefore warn against simplistic causal interpretations of associations in research and clinical case formulation.Reference Danese17 Additionally, our findings indicate that pre-existing vulnerabilities do not explain psychopathology linked with complex trauma. These findings suggest that features of the trauma (e.g. nature, severity, recurrence or timing) or responses to the trauma (e.g. maladaptive cognitions, behavioural coping strategies or emotion processing) could lead to psychopathology. Further research that provides a better understanding of these underlying mechanisms could inform the development of new, more effective interventions for mental illness in people exposed to complex trauma.Reference Danese17
Finally, our findings are consistent with our team's research on victimisation,Reference Schaefer, Moffitt, Arseneault, Danese, Fisher and Houts14,Reference Danese, Moffitt, Arseneault, Bleiberg, Dinardo and Gandelman15 suggesting that despite differences in these constructs (Supplementary Fig. 1), both capture exposures related to psychopathology and cognitive deficits, which have similar origins. These findings highlight the importance of reconciling nomenclature and measurement in these related fields to most effectively inform research and clinical progress.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.57.
Data availability
E-Risk study data is available to researchers upon successful application for access, according to the E-Risk study governance processes (eriskstudy.com).
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the study mothers and fathers, the twins and their teachers for their participation, and to members of the E-Risk team for their dedication, hard work and insights. We thank the Young Person's Mental Health Advisory Group from the National Institute for Health Research Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London for their feedback, which helped shape these research plans. We are also grateful to Dr Jia Ying Teng, who assisted with coding trauma variables.
Author contributions
S.J.L. and A.D. formulated the research questions and designed the analysis plan. K.C.K., L.A., A.C., H.L.F. and T.E.M. contributed to the analysis plan. S.J.L. coded the trauma variables and analysed the data. A.A. checked the analysis for reproducibility. S.J.L. and A.D. drafted the manuscript. All authors assisted with interpretation, commented on drafts of the manuscript and approved the final version.
Funding
The E-Risk study is funded by the UK Medical Research Council (MRC; grant G1002190). Additional support was provided by the US National Institute of Child Health and Development (grant HD077482), the Jacobs Foundation, the UK National Society for Prevention of Cruelty to Children and the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC). S.J.L. is supported by an MRC Clinical Research Training Fellowship and received a UK Royal College of Psychiatrists Margaret Slack Fellowship. L.A. is the Mental Health Leadership Fellow for the ESRC. H.L.F. receives salary support from the ESRC Centre for Society and Mental Health (ES/S012567/1). A.D. was funded by the MRC (grant P005918) and the UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, the Department of Health and Social Care or the ESRC.
Declaration of interest
None.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.