Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:32:56.875Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Temporal Measure of Attention in Schizophrenia and its Clinical Significance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Stanley R. Kay
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychopharmacology Service, Bronx State Hospital, 1500 Waters Service, Bronx, New York 10461, U.S.A.
Man Mohan Singh
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychopharmacology Service, Bronx State Hospital, and Assistant Clinical Professor of Psychiatry, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, New York 10461, U.S.A.

Extract

Disturbance of attention has been frequently suggested as one of the mechanisms underlying schizophrenic thought disorder (Weckowicz and Blewett, 1959; Chapman, 1961; Payne, 1966). Much effort has, therefore, been directed toward studying this variable in schizophrenia (McGhie, 1971; Neale and Cromwell, 1972). The approach undertaken has been determined by the psychophysiological and theoretical constructs presumed to be involved. To enumerate a few, disordered attention has been studied with indices of arousal (Venables, 1960; Venables and Wing, 1962), vigilance tasks (Mackworth, 1970), reaction time (Shakow, 1962), size constancy (Weckowicz and Blewett, 1959; Silverman, 1964a), tachistoscopic detection (Kornetsky, 1972), the WAIS Digit Span subtest (Blum et al., 1969), and immediate recall (McGhie et al., 1965). The lack of correspondence between these various procedures has become increasingly apparent (Neale and Cromwell, 1972). In one recent study, for example, the intercorrelations of five purported measures of attention ranged from –.23 to +.31 (Kopfstein and Neale, 1972).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1974 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Blum, R. A., Livingston, P. B. & Shader, R. I. (1969) Changes in cognition, attention, and language in acute schizophrenia. Diseases of the Nervous System, 30, 31–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J. (1961) A reinterpretation of some pathological disturbances in conceptual breadth. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 514–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, D. R. & Stuart, A. (1955) Some quick tests for trend in location and dispersion. Biometrika, 42, 8095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrington, R. N. & Claridge, G. S. (1965) Sedation threshold and Archimedes' spiral after-effect in early psychosis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 3, 159–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopfstein, J. H. & Neale, J. M. (1972) A multivariate study of attention dysfunction in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 294–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornetsky, C. (1972) The use of a simple test of attention as a measure of drug effects in schizophrenic patients. Psychopharmacologia, 24, 99106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J. & Buss, A. H. (1965) Psychological deficit in schizophrenia: II. Interference and activation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 77106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackworth, J. A. (1970) Vigilance and Habituation. London.Google Scholar
McGhie, A. (1971) Attention and perception in schizophrenia. In Progress in Experimental Personality Research (ed. Maher, ). New York.Google Scholar
McGhie, A., Chapman, J. & Lawson, J. S. (1965) The effect of distraction on schizophrenic performance: (1) Perception and immediate memory. British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 383–90.Google ScholarPubMed
Neale, J. M. & Cromwell, R. L. (1972) Attention and schizophrenia. In Annual Review of the Schizophrenic Syndrome, Vol. II (ed. Cancro, ). New York.Google Scholar
Payne, R. W. (1966) The measurement and significance of overinclusive thinking and retardation in schizophrenic patients. In Psychopathology of Schizophrenia (eds. Hoch, & Zubin, ). New York.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1928) Judgment and Reasoning in the Child. New York.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Boies, S. J. (1971) Components of attention. Psychological Review, 5, 391408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffé, H. (1953) A method for judging all contrasts in the analysis of variance. Biometrika, 40, 87104.Google Scholar
Shakow, D. (1962) Segmental sets. Archives of General Psychiatry, 6, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silverman, J. (1964a) The problem of attention in research and theory in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 71, 352–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, J. (1964b) The scanning control mechanism and ‘cognitive filtering’ in paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenia. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28, 385–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. M. & Smith, J. M. (1973) Sleeplessness in acute and chronic schizophrenia—Response to haloperidol and anti-parkinsonism agents. Psychopharmacologia, 29, 2132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tizard, J. & Venables, P. H. (1957) The influence of extraneous stimulation of the reaction time of schizophrenics. British Journal of Psychology, 48, 299305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venables, P. H. (1960) The effect of auditory and visual stimulation on the skin potential response of schizophrenics. Brain, 83, 7792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venables, P. H. & Wing, J. K. (1962) Level of arousal and the sub-classification of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 7, 114–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weckowicz, T. E. & Blewett, D. B. (1959) Size constancy and abstract thinking in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Mental Science, 105, 909–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.