A trait-related dysfunction in the ventral prefrontal cortex in bipolar I disorder Reference Kronhaus, Lawrence, Williams, Frangou, Brammer and Williams1,Reference Blumberg, Leung, Skudlarski, Lacadie, Fredericks and Harris2 has been proposed to underlie deficits in the effortful inhibition of pre-potent cognitive responses in favour of less automatic ones. Reference Frangou, Haldane, Roddy and Kumari3 Response inhibition deficits can be detected using neuropsychological screening tests such as the Stroop Reference Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe and Leber4 paradigm. Impairment in the latter showed the second largest effect size in relatives of bipolar probands in a recent meta-analysis, Reference Arts, Jabben, Krabbendam and van5 showing considerable promise as a candidate endophenotype. However, due to the paucity of relevant studies (n=4), and the substantial heterogeneity of effect sizes (0.00–1.03), Reference Arts, Jabben, Krabbendam and van5 this possibility needs further investigation.
We report Stroop findings from a study of twin pairs discordant for bipolar I disorder, and a study of singletons with bipolar disorder and their first-degree relatives without bipolar disorder or psychosis from families multiply affected with psychosis. Of the restricted number of neuropsychological measures that overlapped in the two studies, Stroop was selected for analysis due to its appeal as a candidate endophenotype. Reference Arts, Jabben, Krabbendam and van5 Relatives in both studies were presumed to have enhanced risk for carrying bipolar susceptibility genes owing to increased genetic overlap with an affected proband (56% of the twin pairs were identical) or to increased familial loading for functional psychosis. We predicted that patients and relatives in each study would show deficits compared with controls and the performance of relatives would fall in-between those of affected probands and controls.
Method
The two studies were conducted at the Institute of Psychiatry between 1999 and 2007 using non-overlapping samples. Main sources of recruitment were referrals from consultant psychiatrists across the UK, user-led organisations for people affected by bipolar disorder and local/national press advertisements. All participants were characterised diagnostically according to DSM–IV criteria. 6 Structured diagnostic interviews using the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry Reference Wing, Babor, Brugha, Burke, Cooper and Giel7 (twin study) and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Version Reference Spitzer and Endicott8 (family study) were performed with all participants. Medical notes were also consulted for most patients. Information regarding psychiatric diagnoses of family members not directly assessed in the family study was obtained from reliable informants using the Family Interview for Genetic Studies Reference Maxwell9 and, when available, from psychiatric notes. All participants were native speakers of English and had no history of neurological disorder, organic brain disease or head trauma resulting in loss of consciousness for more than 10 min. Controls with personal/family histories of psychotic, bipolar or schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (up to second-degree relatives) were excluded from both studies. The twin study further excluded controls with any lifetime psychiatric diagnoses. No participant was acutely ill at testing (47% of probands obtained normal-range scores in the Beck Depression Inventory Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh10 (1–9) and Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale Reference Altman, Hedejer, Peterson and Davis11 (1–5)) or had been alcohol or substance dependent over the 12 months preceding assessment (except one co-twin who was alcohol dependent). The Institute of Psychiatry's ethical committee approved both studies.
Eighteen twin pairs discordant for bipolar I disorder and 17 healthy control pairs participated in the twin study. The family study Reference Schulze, Hall, McDonald, Marshall, Walshe and Murray12 included 40 patients with bipolar I disorder who had both a history of delusions or hallucinations during illness exacerbation and one or more first- or second-degree relative(s) with bipolar disorder or other functional psychosis. The study further included 46 of the patients' first-degree relatives who had no personal history of psychotic, bipolar or schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, and 48 unrelated control participants. The descriptive characteristics of the studies and the demographic, clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of their respective samples are summarised in online Table DS1.
The Stroop Reference Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe and Leber4 was administered to all participants, yielding a colour-word score (number correct in the colour-word condition) and an interference score (number correct in the colour condition minus number correct in the colour-word condition). An alternative interference score (colour-word completion time minus colour completion time) was further estimated in the twin study.
In each study, group differences in Stroop scores were investigated in a series of two-step linear regressions for clustered observations, Reference Williams13 using robust standard errors to safeguard against potential violations of the standard ANOVA assumptions. In the first step, socio-demographic/IQ variables that had shown significant group differences in separate clustered regressions (twin study: age and IQ; family study: no differences emerged) (online Table DS1) were included in the models. In the second step, affective scores that had shown significant group differences in separate clustered regressions (twin study: depression score; family study: depression and mania scores) (online Table DS1) were further added to the models.
To examine whether controls with lifetime psychiatric diagnoses (family study only) or dizygotic discordant pairs ‘diluted’ the pattern of findings, all analyses were repeated after excluding both subgroups.
Linear regressions with robust standard errors were used to examine associations between Stroop performance and illness duration/affective symptoms in the total patient sample of each study. Age was included as covariate in the twin-study model, because it emerged as a significant predictor of interference score (P<0.01) in the first-step analysis (described above).
Results
In both studies, significant or trend deficits were detected in the bipolar probands compared with co-twins/first-degree relatives, who were indistinguishable from controls, and in the family-study/monozygotic probands compared with controls (independent of control sample composition in the family study) (online Table DS1). After covarying for affective symptoms, twin probands (full sample) and monozygotic probands no longer showed trend deficits compared with co-twins/controls, but the remaining differences remained significant or were reduced to trends (online Table DS1). Depression significantly predicted ‘colour-word completion time minus colour completion time’ in the twin patients (coefficient=2.82, 95% CI 1.18–4.46, P=0.002).
Discussion
We report findings from a twin study, and the largest family study to date, investigating Stroop deficits in bipolar I disorder. The majority of co-twins without bipolar disorder were genetically identical with an affected proband, while the relatives of the family study came from families with multiple occurrences of functional psychosis. Both studies were ideally suited to detect inhibitory control deficits in individuals at presumed enhanced risk for carrying bipolar susceptibility genes. The large sample size, strict statistical control for socio-demographic and intellectual confounders, and combination of two powerful study designs are unique strengths of the present investigation.
Both studies found increased susceptibility to interference in patients with bipolar disorder and intact inhibitory control in their first-degree relatives. The twin study further showed an association between depression and an interference score. Corroborating two earlier studies, Reference Ferrier, Chowdhury, Thompson, Watson and Young14,Reference McIntosh, Harrison, Forrester, Lawrie and Johnstone15 these findings do not support our hypothesis that increased susceptibility to interference is an endophenotype for bipolar I disorder.
Most studies of response inhibition in patients with euthymic bipolar disorder Reference Dixon, Kravariti, Frith, Murray and McGuire16–Reference Olley, Malhi, Bachelor, Cahill, Mitchell and Berk18 and relatives of probands Reference Kronhaus, Lawrence, Williams, Frangou, Brammer and Williams1,Reference McIntosh, Harrison, Forrester, Lawrie and Johnstone15,Reference Zalla, Joyce, Szoke, Schurhoff, Pillon and Komano19 have ascertained index cases through specialist clinical settings. A notable difference of our study is its broader source of ascertainment. In addition to the usual routes, affected probands were identified through user-led organisations and press advertisements. It is, therefore, possible that a familial risk for enhanced susceptibility to interference is confined to a subgroup of patients, who are more likely to be found among in-patient and out-patient cohorts. Reference Loughland, Lewin, Carr, Sheedy and Harris20
Our findings do not rule out the possibility that trait-related dysfunction in neural substrates of inhibitory control coexists with intact Stroop performance in individuals at risk for bipolar I disorder. Reference Kronhaus, Lawrence, Williams, Frangou, Brammer and Williams1,Reference Blumberg, Leung, Skudlarski, Lacadie, Fredericks and Harris2
We conclude that being a first-degree relative of an individual with bipolar I disorder, even a severe form with psychotic features and increased familial loading, does not necessarily confer risk for enhanced susceptibility to interference.
Acknowledgements
E.K. received funding support from the Psychiatry Research Trust. The work was funded by a Guy's & St Thomas' Charitable Foundation Research Studentship to K.S. M-H.H. received funding support from the Kaplan Fellowship, Harvard Medical School, and the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) Sidney R. Baer Jr. Foundation Young Investigator Award. Both C.McD. and E.B. were Wellcome Trust Fellows. The research project also received support from the Medical Research Council (UK), the Stanley Medical Research Institute and NARSAD. We thank the Manic Depression Fellowship for help with recruitment of participants. We are grateful to all the families who participated in the study.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.