Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:16:57.100Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Socioeconomic status as a risk factor for dementia death: individual participant meta-analysis of 86 508 men and women from the UK

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Tom C. Russ*
Affiliation:
Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research Centre, University of Edinburgh; Scottish Dementia Clinical Research Network, NHS Scotland; and Centre for Cognitive Ageing & Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh
Emmanuel Stamatakis
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London
Mark Hamer
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London
John M. Starr
Affiliation:
Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research Centre, University of Edinburgh; Scottish Dementia Clinical Research Network, NHS Scotland; and Centre for Cognitive Ageing & Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh
Mika Kivimäki
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London
G. David Batty
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, UK
*
Tom C. Russ, Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research Centre, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Life-course socioeconomic factors may have a role in dementia aetiology but there is a current paucity of studies. Meta-analyses of individual participant data would considerably strengthen this evidence base.

Aims

To examine the association between socioeconomic status in early life and adulthood with later dementia death.

Method

Individual participant meta-analysis of 11 prospective cohort studies (1994–2004, n = 86508).

Results

Leaving full-time education at an earlier age was associated with an increased risk of dementia death in women (fully adjusted hazard ratio (HR) for age ⩽14 v. age ⩾16: HR = 1.76, 95% Cl 1.23–2.53) but not men. Occupational social class was not statistically significantly associated with dementia death in men or women.

Conclusions

Lower educational attainment in women was associated with an increased risk of dementia-related death independently of common risk behaviours and comorbidities.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

Socioeconomic inequalities in cardiovascular disease Reference Marmot, Bosma, Hemingway, Brunner and Stansfeld1 and selected cancers Reference Stringhini, Dugravot, Kivimaki, Shipley, Zins and Goldberg2 are well recognised. More recently, research attention has focused on such differentials in mental health, including depression Reference Lorant, Deliege, Eaton, Robert, Philippot and Ansseau3 and other common mental disorders such as dementia. Reference Fryers, Melzer and Jenkins4-Reference Sharp and Gatz6 There is a suggestion that socioeconomic factors may have a role in the aetiology of dementia including lifetime manual occupation Reference Qiu, Karp, von Strauss, Winblad, Fratiglioni and Bellander7 as well as various indicators of socioeconomic status in early life Reference Persson and Skoog8 and lower educational attainment. Reference Persson and Skoog8,Reference Karp, Kåreholt, Qiu, Bellander, Winblad and Fratiglioni9 However, because of the paucity of large-scale, well-characterised studies, the extant evidence is discordant and there has been inconsistent control for confounding variables. Thus, the precise nature of the socioeconomic status-dementia relationship remains unclear. Although individual studies undoubtedly have value in improving this evidence base, the pooling of raw data from multiple studies, which would also represent an important technical advance in this context, has yet to be utilised. We therefore undertook the first individual participant meta-analysis using data from 11 large, community-based cohort studies that held data on socioeconomic status, covariates and dementia death. The purpose of this paper is twofold: first, as a proof of principle that this methodology can be applied to the study of the role of socioeconomic position in the risk of dementia; and second, to add to the evidence base by further investigating the association between socioeconomic factors and dementia-related death.

Method

Study samples

Participants were taken from the Health Survey for England, Reference Mindell, Biddulph, Hirani, Stamatakis, Craig and Nunn10 a representative general population-based health examination study sampling individuals living in households in England. From 1994 to 2004, 11 independent, cross-sectional studies with identical methodologies were conducted on an annual basis. Consenting study members (89.6%) were followed up by linkage to the UK National Health Service mortality registry. Study participants gave full informed consent and ethical approval was obtained from the London Research Ethics Council.

Assessment of socioeconomic status

During a household visit, interviewers collected information using computer-assisted personal interviewing modules. Information on occupational social class was collected during the interview and coded according to the Registrar General classification (professional (I), intermediate (II), skilled non-manual (IIINM), skilled manual (IIIM), part-skilled (IV) and unskilled (V)), a standard approach in the UK. Reference Macintyre, McKay, Der and Hiscock11 Age on leaving full-time education was recorded as <15, 15, 16, 17, 18, >18, never went to school and still in full-time education. For this study occupational social class was coded into four groups: professional/intermediate (the referent), skilled non-manual, skilled manual and part-skilled/unskilled. Educational attainment was coded into three groups: 14 years or younger, 15 years old and 16 years or older (the referent).

Assessment of other risk factors and comorbidities

Smoking status (not a current smoker; <5 per day; 5-10 per day; 10-15 per day; 15-20 per day; >20 per day), weekly alcohol consumption (converted to units of alcohol), and history of cardiovascular disease and diabetes (including hyperglycaemia) were collected by self-report at interview. Individuals drinking above safe limits of alcohol were identified using gender-specific safe limits (⩽14 units per week for women and ⩽21 units per week for men). 12

Psychological distress was measured using the 12-item version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12), a widely used measure in population studies. Reference Goldberg, Gater, Sartorius, Ustun, Piccinelli and Gureje13 A score of four is often used as a threshold to denote psychological distress, Reference Aalto, Elovainio, Kivimaki, Uutela and Pirkola14 but since we have previously shown that even low levels of psychological distress - that is, scores below four - are associated with an increased risk of dementia in these cohort studies, Reference Russ, Hamer, Stamatakis, Starr and Batty15 we adjusted for total GHQ-12 score as a continuous variable.

Ascertainment of dementia

Causes of death recorded on death certificates were coded using ICD-9 16 and ICD-10. 17 Any mention of dementia death was identified using codes 290.0-290.4, 294.9, 331.0-331.2, and 331.9 for ICD-9, and F01, F03, F09, G30 and G31 for ICD-10.

Statistical analyses

We ascertained that the proportional hazards assumption had not been violated by inspecting the log(−log(survival)) plot. We then used Cox proportional hazards models Reference Cox18 to compute study-specific hazard ratios with accompanying 95% confidence intervals for the association between the measures of socioeconomic status and dementia death. Heterogeneity in the effect estimates between studies was examined using the I 2 statistic, which indicates the proportion of the total variation in the estimates that is due to between-studies variation. It varied between 0% and 36.9% depending on the measure of socioeconomic status used in the analysis. To obtain a conservative estimate, we pooled the study-specific effect estimates and their standard errors in random effects meta-analyses. Calendar time (days) was the time scale; for participants with no record of an event, the data were censored at the 15 February 2008.

Models were initially unadjusted, then a series of variables were added to the multivariable model: age, smoking status, alcohol consumption (units per week), baseline cardiovascular disease (yes/no), diabetes (yes/no), psychological distress (GHQ-12 score), occupational social class and educational attainment. Since, as described, the association between socioeconomic status and dementia has been reported to be different in men and women, Reference Launer, Andersen, Dewey, Letenneur, Ott and Amaducci19 gender-specific analyses were conducted. We compared the effects of controlling for different covariates/mediators on the magnitude of the association by examining a change in the size of hazard ratio rather than a change in significance level. Reference Batty, Der, Macintyre and Deary20

Individuals with data missing for one or more variables and those with no missing data were compared using Student's t-test for continuous variables and χ2 tests for categorical variables. The main analysis was based on participants with no missing data. In the sensitivity analysis, missing values for covariates were imputed with PASW statistics version 18.0 for Windows using five imputations based on maximum likelihood estimates. All other analyses were conducted using R version 2.15.0 for Windows and the survival and metafor Reference Viechtbauer21 packages. The reporting of this study conforms to the STROBE statement. Reference von Elm, Altman, Egger, Pocock, Gøtzsche and Vandenbroucke22

Results

The initial sample comprised 96 605 individuals. The derivation of the sample is shown in Fig. 1. After removing individuals who declined linkage to mortality records (n = 10 065) and for whom survival was incalculable or who had no cause of death recorded (n = 32) the maximum analytic sample comprised 86 508 people (mean age 56.1 years, s.d. = 14.4): 39 125 men and 47 383 women. Data were missing for occupational social class for 2325 individuals (analytic n = 84 183) and for educational attainment for 61 individuals (analytic n = 86 447). Table 1 shows the characteristics of study members from the 11 cohorts and pooled summaries.

Table 2 shows the baseline characteristics of the pooled sample according to occupational social class separately in men and women. Individuals from a lower occupational social class were older, were more likely to smoke, and had a somewhat greater likelihood of baseline cardiovascular disease, diabetes and psychological distress. There was little association between occupational social class and alcohol consumption. Individuals from a lower occupational social class were more likely, as anticipated, to have spent less time in full-time education (women r = 0.41, P<0.001; men r = 0.43, P<0.001). Similar patterns of association were seen with education as the exposure of interest (results not shown but available from the authors on request).

Fig. 1 Flow chart of participants from initial pooled sample through to analytic sample showing subsequent mortality: the Health Survey for England 1994-2004.

Of the 12 952 deaths during a mean follow-up of 8.6 years (s.d. = 3.5), 622 were ascribed to dementia. Figures 2 and 3 show the fully adjusted hazard ratios for the association of occupational social class and educational attainment respectively, with dementia death for each cohort study, in addition to gender-specific totals and meta-analysed effects. Overall, relative to study members from professional/intermediate occupational social classes, there was no increase in the risk of dementia death among those belonging to the lower social classes in women (fully adjusted hazard ratio (HR) for skilled non-manual: HR = 0.88 (95% CI 0.59-1.31); skilled manual: HR = 0.61 (95% CI 0.36-1.06); semi-skilled and unskilled manual: HR = 0.92 (95% CI 0.62-1.36)) or men (skilled non-manual: HR = 1.03 (95% CI 0.53-2.00); skilled manual: HR = 1.03 (95% CI 0.63-1.69); semi-skilled/unskilled manual: HR = 1.33 (95% CI 0.80-2.21), Fig. 2). Relative to study members who left school aged 16 or older, there was an increase in the risk of dementia death among those leaving school earlier in women (leaving school aged 15: HR = 1.64 (95% CI 1.02-2.65); leaving school aged 14 or younger: HR = 1.76 (95% CI 1.23-2.53)) but not in men (leaving school aged 15: HR = 0.98 (95% CI 0.51-1.88); leaving school aged 14 or younger: HR = 1.20 (95% CI 0.77-1.87), Fig. 3).

Table 3 shows the impact of controlling for covariates on the association between the two indicators of socioeconomic status and dementia in women and men. The association between occupational social class and dementia death seen in age-adjusted models was completely explained by covariates in women but was more robust to statistical adjustment in men. The association between leaving full-time education at an earlier age and later dementia death observed in the age-adjusted models was fully attenuated on adjustment for covariates in men but remained in women.

Table 1 Characteristics of participantsFootnote a according to cohort study: the Health Survey for England 1994–2004

Characteristic 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 Overall
n 10 599 10 905 11 344 5875 11 058 5522 8684 11 283 5399 10 887 5049 96 605
Household response, % 77 78 79 76 74 76 75 74 74 73 72
Estimated adult interview response, % 71 73 75 71 69 70 68 67 67 66 66
Age, years
    Mean (s.d.) 56.1 (14.4) 56.2 (14.3) 56.0 (14.3) 55.6 (14.0) 56.1 (14.3) 55.7 (14.3) 65.8 (18.5) 56.0 (14.3) 55.4 (14.4) 56.3 (14.3) 57.0 (14.3) 56.9 (15.0)
    Range 35–97 35–100 35–102 35–95 35–97 35–96 35–107 35–97 35–97 35–97 35–96 35–107
Female, % 54.9 54.1 54.3 54.6 55.0 54.3 62.1 55.5 56.3 55.6 57.6 55.7
Occupational social class I–IIINM, % 53.8 55.3 55.2 54.8 54.6 56.5 56.4 56.6 57.8 57.7 59.7 53.2
Current smoker, % 23.5 23.2 24.2 23.3 23.2 22.6 17.7 21.6 22.5 21.2 19.3 22.2
Drinks more than recommended alcohol limit,Footnote b % 18.8 19.6 20.1 20.0 21.3 19.1 13.1 21.2 20.5 19.4
Cardiovascular disease at baseline,Footnote c % 4.8 5.0 4.9 4.5 4.5 4.9 7.6 5.0 4.2 4.4 4.5 5.0
Diabetes (including hyperglycaemia) at baseline, % 2.6 3.0 3.1 3.5 3.0 3.7 4.7 4.3 4.1 4.7 5.5 3.7
GHQ-12 score,Footnote d mean (s.d.) 1.5 (2.6) 1.7 (2.8) 1.5 (2.7) 1.5 (2.7) 1.7 (2.8) 1.6 (2.8) 1.3 (2.5) 1.6 (1.7) 1.3 (2.5) 1.3 (2.6) 1.5 (2.7)
Consented to mortality linkage, % 95.2 93.5 93.5 94.1 94.5 93.8 68.2 88.4 88.4 87.2 85.5 89.6
Follow-up, years: median 13.5 12.5 11.5 10.5 9.5 8.5 7.4 6.6 5.6 4.6 3.5 8.4
Deaths from any cause, n 2320 2193 2023 892 1591 697 1364 867 337 503 165 12 952
Dementia deaths, n 106 104 88 43 83 30 101 34 11 17 5 622

I–IIINM, professional (I), intermediate (II) and skilled non-manual (IIINM).

a. All participants in the surveys are represented in this table, irrespective of consent to mortality linkage. All subsequent tables and figures only represent individuals who consented to linkage and were therefore included in the present study.

b. Calculated using gender-specific safe limits: ⩽14 units per week for women and ⩽21 units for men. Alcohol intake was recorded in a different format to other years in the Health Surveys for England in 2003 and 2004 and data are not included for these years.

c. History of cardiovascular disease including angina, myocardial infarction or stroke (haemorrhagic or thrombotic).

d. The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) was not administered in the Health Survey for England in 1996.

Table 2 Study members' baseline characteristics according to occupational social class: the Health Survey for England 1994–2004

Occupational social class Total, n
I II IIINM IIIM IV V
Women
    n 754 11 663 15 956 4079 8954 4081 45 487
    Age, years: mean (s.d.) 49.1 (12.5) 53.4 (13.7) 56.0 (14.4) 59.0 (15.3) 56.6 (14.6) 60.8 (14.6) 45 487
    Left school <16 years, % 4.4 23.7 43.2 65.8 63.7 39.6 45 455
    Current smoker, % 12.1 18.0 19.9 26.8 28.2 29.7 45 469
    Drinks more than recommended alcohol limit,Footnote a % 19.1 15.7 12.9 9.7 8.6 7.8 37 752
    Cardiovascular disease at baseline,Footnote b % 1.6 2.7 3.5 5.1 4.9 6.4 45 487
    Diabetes (including hyperglycaemia) at baseline, % 0.9 2.1 2.6 4.2 3.5 4.2 45 487
    GHQ-12 score,Footnote c mean (s.d.) 1.5 (2.7) 1.6 (2.7) 1.5 (2.7) 1.8 (2.9) 1.8 (2.9) 1.8 (3.0) 38 162
Men
    n 2987 12 272 3759 12 835 5163 1680 38 696
    Age, years: mean (s.d.) 54.7 (13.5) 54.3 (13.5) 56.2 (14.6) 56.4 (13.9) 56.7 (14.1) 57.2 (13.7) 38 696
    Left school <16 years, % 11.5 27.8 40.0 64.1 64.2 71.4 38 667
    Current smoker, % 9.2 16.1 19.8 28.6 31.2 37.6 38 672
    Drinks more than recommended alcohol limit,1Footnote a % 22.8 25.7 21.1 23.0 20.6 22.8 32 554
    Cardiovascular disease at baseline,Footnote b % 2.8 4.5 6.4 6.8 6.7 6.7 38 696
    Diabetes (including hyperglycaemia) at baseline, % 2.7 4.0 4.8 4.6 5.4 4.5 38 696
    GHQ-12 score,Footnote c mean (s.d.) 1.1 (2.2) 1.2 (2.3) 1.2 (2.4) 1.3 (2.5) 1.5 (2.7) 1.6 (2.9) 32 560

I, professional; II, intermediate, IIINM, skilled non-manual; IIIM, skilled manual; IV, part skilled; and V, unskilled.

a. Calculated using gender-specific safe limits: ⩽14 units per week for women and ⩽21 units for men. Alcohol intake was recorded in a different format to other years in the Health Surveys for England in 2003 and 2004 and data are not included for these years.

b. History of cardiovascular disease including angina, myocardial infarction or stroke (haemorrhagic or thrombotic).

c. The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) was not administered in the Health Survey for England 1996.

Fig. 2 Fully adjusted hazard ratios (HRs) with 95% confidence intervals of dementia death by survey year for individuals, from left to right, from (a) skilled non-manual, (b) skilled manual and (c) semi-skilled/unskilled manual occupational social classes compared with professional/intermediate: the Health Survey for England 1994-2004.

For women: I 2 values for these models are 4.1%, 0.0% and 0.0%. For men: I 2 values for these models are 0.0%, 12.9% and 6.4%.

Fig. 3 Fully adjusted hazard ratios (HRs) with 95% confidence intervals of dementia death by survey year for individuals who left full-time education aged, from left to right, (a) 15 years and (b) 14 years or younger compared with those who left full-time education aged 16 years or older: the Health Survey for England 1994-2004.

For women: I 2 values for these models are 0.0% and 0.0%. For men: I 2 values for these models are 0.0% and 0.0%.

Sensitivity and subgroup analyses

Data were missing for one or more variables in 32.8% (n = 28 375) of the sample. Online Table DS1 shows a comparison of the characteristics of individuals with complete data for all variables v. individuals who were missing data for one or more variables. Individuals with complete data were more likely to be male, younger, drink more alcohol per week and were more likely to drink over the recommended limits and a larger proportion were current smokers. They were less likely to have diabetes and cardiovascular disease at baseline. A slightly larger proportion of individuals with complete data belonged to a non-manual occupational social class and women were less likely to have left school early (the reverse was true of men). Therefore, individuals with missing data did not always have unfavourable levels of risk factors. Accounting for missing data by multiple imputation did not alter our conclusions (online Table DS2). Similarly, excluding dementia-related deaths occurring within 5 years of follow-up, to explore reverse causality in the case of current occupational social class, did not affect our results (online Table DS3).

Table 3 Hazard ratio (95% confidence interval) for the association between occupational social class and educational attainment with dementia death: Health Survey for England 1994–2004

Occupational social class Age on leaving full-time education
Dementia
deaths
I–II IIINM IIIM IV–V Dementia
deaths
⩾16 15 ⩽14
n HR HR (95% CI)y I 2 HR (95% CI) I 2 HR (95% CI) I 2 n HR HR (95% CI) I 2 HR (95% CI) I 2
Women
    Unadjusted model 358 45 487 1 1.34 (0.97–1.86) 0.0 2.27 (1.49–3.44) 0.0 2.18 (1.59–2.98) 0.0 411 47 351 1 1.32 0.90–1.95) 5.4 9.87 (7.67–12.7) 0.0
(ref) (ref)
    Adjusted for age (basic model) 358 45 487 1 1.05 (0.75–1.48) 5.9 0.96 (0.63–1.47) 0.0 1.33 (0.97–1.83) 0.0 411 47 351 1 1.54 (0.96–2.47) 28.4 1.61 (1.25–2.08) 0.0
    + risk factorsFootnote a 343 37 998 1 1.07 (0.75–1.53) 8.5 0.94 (0.61–1.46) 0.0 1.30 (0.94–1.79) 0.0 393 39 612 1 1.46 (0.88–2.43) 36.9 1.56 (1.18–2.07) 6.6
    + somatic comorbiditiesFootnote b 358 45 487 1 1.05 (0.74–1.49) 8.5 0.96 (0.63–1.46) 0.0 1.34 (0.98–1.84) 0.0 411 47 351 1 1.57 (0.98–2.53) 29.3 1.65 (1.28–2.13) 0.0
    + psychological distress 275 38 553 1 0.99 (0.69–1.43) 0.0 0.81 (0.49–1.35) 0.0 1.23 (0.86–1.75) 0.0 317 40 035 1 1.60 (1.04–2.48) 3.3 1.61 (1.20–2.16) 0.0
    + age on leaving full-time education 358 45 487 1 0.93 (0.67–1.31) 2.7 0.74 (0.48–1.15) 0.0 1.01 (0.72–1.42) 0.0
    + occupational social class 358 45 465 1 1.57 (1.02–2.43) 6.2 1.68 (1.24–2.28) 0.0
    Fully adjusted 264 31 343 1 0.88 (0.59–1.31) 4.1 0.61 (0.36–1.06) 0.0 0.92 (0.62–1.36) 0.0 264 31 343 1 1.64 (1.02–2.65) 0.0 1.76 (1.23–2.53) 0.0
Men
    Unadjusted model 210 36 880 1 1.51 (0.88–2.59) 0.0 1.43 (0.95–2.15) 21.8 1.81 (1.23–2.67) 4.3 211 37 263 1 0.77 (0.44–1.33) 0.0 8.89 (6.38–12.4) 0.0
(ref) (ref)
    Adjusted for age (basic model) 210 36 880 1 1.13 (0.66–1.94) 0.0 1.29 (0.88–1.89) 11.5 1.52 (1.01–2.29) 10.3 211 37 263 1 1.03 (0.59–1.80) 0.0 1.44 (1.03–2.02) 0.0
    + risk factorsFootnote a 204 32 596 1 1.07 (0.62–1.85) 0.0 1.29 (0.85–1.95) 19.8 1.38 (0.86–2.22) 24.8 205 32 939 1 0.96 (0.54–1.72) 0.0 1.30 (0.92–1.84) 0.0
    + somatic comorbiditiesFootnote b 210 36 880 1 1.14 (0.66–1.96) 0.0 1.28 (0.86–1.90) 16.8 1.49 (0.97–2.30) 16.4 211 37 263 1 1.05 (0.60–1.84) 0.0 1.38 (0.99–1.94) 0.0
    + psychological distress 158 30 899 1 1.15 (0.60–2.19) 0.0 1.24 (0.83–1.85) 1.3 1.72 (1.12–2.64) 0.0 158 31 215 1 1.05 (0.56–1.97) 0.0 1.45 (0.98–2.14) 0.0
    + age on leaving full-time education 210 36 854 1 1.06 (0.61–1.84) 0.0 1.12 (0.72–1.74) 20.8 1.38 (0.90–2.12) 6.1
    + occupational social class 210 36 854 1 0.94 (0.53–1.67) 0.0 1.29 (0.88–1.88) 0.0
    Fully adjusted 155 26 790 1 1.03 (0.53–2.00) 0.0 1.03 (0.63–1.69) 13.0 1.33 (0.80–2.21) 6.4 155 26 790 1 0.98 (0.51–1.89) 0.0 1.20 (0.77–1.87) 0.0

I–II, professional and intermediate; IIINM, skilled non-manual; IIIM, skilled manual; IV–V, part skilled and unskilled.

a. Risk factors comprise smoking and alcohol consumption.

b. Somatic comorbidities comprise cardiovascular disease and diabetes.

Discussion

The purposes of the current study were to show that an individual participant meta-analysis technique can be applied to a socioeconomic risk factor and also to investigate the association between socioeconomic status and dementia-related death. In order to achieve the former we used a convenience sample of 11 large UK population-based cohort studies. With regard to the latter, the main finding of this study was that educational attainment was associated with dementia death in women but not in men. In men, we also found that, relative to the highest two classes, belonging to the lowest two occupational social classes was associated with an elevated risk of dementia death; a relationship that did not reach statistical significance at conventional levels. There was no association between occupational social class and dementia death in women.

Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this study is the first to use an individual participant meta-analysis methodology to examine the association between socioeconomic variables and dementia. In contrast to previous literature-based analyses, an individual participant meta-analysis has the advantage of providing precise estimates of risk marker-disease relationships, reliable information on the shape of a given risk factor-disease relationship (for example dose-response v. threshold) and a consistent approach to statistical control for plausible covariates and subgroup analyses. We used a large, well-characterised sample that is representative of the general population in England. This provided sufficient power to allow gender-specific analyses and allowed us to explore the role of a series of explanatory factors. By including two measures of socioeconomic status at different time points we offer some insights into the influence of life-course socioeconomic status on dementia risk.

This notwithstanding, the data have their limitations. Despite the large sample and over 12 000 deaths during follow-up, there were only 622 dementia-related deaths. This is due to the wide age range of individuals included in the survey at baseline - adults aged 35 and over. Although this wide age range increases the generalisability of the findings, it has resulted in a relatively small proportion of individuals dying with dementia and consequently wide confidence intervals, limiting the conclusions that can be drawn. The identified gender differentials should be viewed in the light of this question of power. The number of dementia-related deaths in this study is likely to have been additionally affected by the unavoidable problems of underdiagnosis of dementia in the community, Reference Sampson, Blanchard, Jones, Tookman and King23 underrecording of dementia on death certificates Reference Martyn and Pippard24 and diagnoses being inaccurately coded. Non-recording of dementia on death certificates has been highlighted as an important issue, but this seems to be improving. A recent study identified that 71.5% of a clinic sample diagnosed with probable Alzheimer's disease had dementia correctly recorded on their death certificate. Reference Russ, Batty and Starr25 There was no association in that sample between correct dementia certification and area deprivation or premorbid IQ estimated by the National Adult Reading Test (unpublished results available from the author on request), suggesting that individuals reported as having dementia on their death certificate are representative of the population of people with diagnosed dementia in the community, at least in terms of intelligence and level of deprivation.

Comparison with previous studies

Dementia was estimated to affect over 24 million people globally in 2001 and this is expected to rise to over 80 million by 2040. Reference Ferri, Prince, Brayne, Brodaty, Fratiglioni and Ganguli26 In England and Wales the prevalence of dementia is estimated to vary from approximately 1.5% at age 65-69 to approximately 25.3% over the age of 84. Reference MRC27 Dementia incidence ranges from 7.4 (95% CI 3.6-16.1) per 1000 person-years at age 65-69 to 84.9 (95% CI 63.0-107.8) per 1000 person-years over the age of 84. Reference Matthews and Brayne28 However, existing prospective studies of socioeconomic factors have generally been small in scale - only one analysis reported a combined sample larger than 10 000 Reference Launer, Andersen, Dewey, Letenneur, Ott and Amaducci19 - and few studies measure socioeconomic status at more than one time point. Reference Persson and Skoog8 On a related note, the association between education and dementia risk has been reported to be present in women but not in men; Reference Launer, Andersen, Dewey, Letenneur, Ott and Amaducci19 it is therefore important that studies are sufficiently large to allow gender-specific analyses. In addition, for older participants - who are most likely to have died with dementia during follow-up - the social class may have been allocated to females on the basis of their husband's occupation, further muddying the waters.

Factors across the lifespan have been implicated in the aetiology of dementia. A series of studies confirm a link between early life socioeconomic status and dementia risk with early parental death - and, potentially, consequent socioeconomic hardship - being highlighted as an important risk factor. Reference Norton, Ostbye, Smith, Munger and Tschanz29

A total of 51 of 88 studies included in a systematic review Reference Sharp and Gatz6 reported a significant association between a basic education and dementia risk, the remainder reporting no effect. However, only two previous studies report gender-specific effects, showing an effect of education on dementia risk in women but not in men, as found in the current study.

In England, the Education Act 1918 raised the school leaving age from 12 to 14. It was further increased to 15 in 1947 and to 16 in 1973. Participants in the Health Survey for England came from all these educational eras and overall approximately half of men and women stayed on after completion of the compulsory period of education. However, individuals were much more likely to remain in education longer if they were born later: 86.7% of women and 87.2% of men born after 1956 (who therefore had to remain in school until 16) compared with 17.8% of women and 18.5% of men born before 1906 and who could therefore leave school at 12. However, the reference category used, of individuals who remained in full-time education up to 16 years or later, exactly matches those who remained in education after the compulsory school leaving age.

Since children scoring higher on IQ tests, as well as children from a higher occupational social class, are more likely to be given the opportunity to remain in education for longer, a number of studies have investigated the association between childhood mental ability and dementia. A Scottish study identified 50 participants in the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey who had developed dementia and ascertained that their scores on the Moray House Test in 1932 had been significantly lower than their peers in the same area who did not develop dementia. Reference Whalley, Starr, Athawes, Hunter, Pattie and Deary30 However, a larger subsequent study confirmed that this was the case for vascular dementia but not for Alzheimer's disease. Reference McGurn, Deary and Starr31 On the other hand, the Nun study identified that low linguistic ability at a mean age of 22 was associated with Alzheimer's disease in the 14 study participants aged 79-96 whose brains were neuropathologically examined post-mortem. Reference Snowdon, Kemper, Mortimer, Greiner, Wekstein and Markesbery32

Occupation in adulthood has also been shown to be associated with dementia risk - high ‘occupational attainment’ is associated with lower dementia risk. Reference Stern, Gurland, Tatemichi, Tang, Wilder and Mayeux33 However, since the pathology of Alzheimer's disease and probably vascular dementia develops over a very long period of time Reference Braak and Braak34 risk factors must be measured sufficiently early in life for them to have an effect, and exposures immediately before retirement may have little or no effect on dementia risk.

Few studies measure socioeconomic status across the whole lifespan. One study to do so identified early parental death, manual work and physical illness in the spouse or serious illness in a child, both after the age of 65, to be independent risk factors for dementia. Reference Persson and Skoog8 However, many of the socioeconomic factors measured in studies are closely linked and disentangling their individual effects can be extremely difficult.

Mechanisms of effect

The role of education and mental ability as potential risk factors for dementia has been linked to the hypothesis of cognitive reserve - that certain individuals' brains are structurally or functionally more resilient to disease or injury. This sprang from the observation that there was no clear relationship between the extent of brain pathology and the clinical manifestations of dementia in an individual. Cognitive reserve might relate to a person's intrinsic make up or could result from external experience, i.e. education and occupation. Indeed, there is evidence that occupation is associated with differences in parietal blood flow in Alzheimer's disease that could be a marker of reserve. Reference Stern, Alexander, Prohovnik, Stricks, Link and Lennon35

Criticisms of the cognitive reserve hypothesis include the suggestion that the effect merely reflects performance on cognitive tests Reference Gilleard36 or that the effect is mediated by lifestyle factors and cerebrovascular risk. Reference Del Ser, Hachinski, Merskey and Munoz37 The present study used clinical diagnoses of dementia recorded on death certificates and two measures of socioeconomic status at different stages of life. This allows us to demonstrate that the observed association between education and dementia in women was not mediated by adult occupation.

Since an individual with more cognitive reserve would have more advanced pathological changes at the time of diagnosis, it has been suggested that this could be linked with a swifter decline and poorer survival. Some studies have found that individuals with Alzheimer's disease and higher educational and occupational attainment declined faster in their performance on cognitive tests. Reference Stern, Albert, Tang and Tsai38 This aspect of the cognitive reserve hypothesis was not examined by the current study.

Implications

An association between lower educational attainment and dementia in women but not men has a number of implications. First, the mechanism of this association, currently hypothesised to relate to cognitive reserve, must be clarified. Second, the reason that the association is observed only in women should be investigated. It may be that fewer women entered further education and that those who did had to be very intelligent to do so. Thus, length of education in these cohorts could be confounded by intelligence in women to a greater extent than men. However, the identified gender difference may also potentially hold the clue to an intervention to decrease the risk of dementia in women, if a modifiable risk (or protective) factor could be identified. Lastly, if this association were to reflect a causative link between education and dementia risk, promoting higher and further education, especially for women, could have important public health consequences for a common and serious condition.

In conclusion, this large prospective study shows an association between leaving full-time education at a younger age and dementia death in women, but not in men. This relationship remained after adjustment for alcohol, smoking, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, psychological distress and occupational social class. An association between lower occupational social class and dementia death that did not reach statistical significance at conventional levels was also observed in men, but not in women.

Funding

This work was supported by Alzheimer Scotland. T.C.R. is supported by Alzheimer Scotland and is employed in the National Health Service by the Scottish Dementia Clinical Research Network, which is funded by the Chief Scientist Office (part of the Scottish Government Health Directorates). T.C.R. and J.M.S. are members of the Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research Centre funded by Alzheimer Scotland. T.C.R., J.M.S. and G.D.B. are members of the University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross-council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (G0700704/84698). Funding from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council, Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council, Economic and Social Research Council and Medical Research Council is gratefully acknowledged. E.S. is supported by a National Institute for Health Research Career Development Fellowship. M.K. is supported by the Medical Research Council, UK, the US National Institutes of Health (R01HL036310; R01AG034454), the Academy of Finland, Finland, and by a professorial fellowship from the Economic and Social Research Council, UK. G.D.B. was a Wellcome Trust Fellow during the preparation of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Preliminary results from this study were presented as a poster at the 2011 Alzheimer's Association International Conference in Paris.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Marmot, MG, Bosma, H, Hemingway, H, Brunner, E, Stansfeld, S. Contribution of job control and other risk factors to social variations in coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet 1997; 350: 235–9.Google Scholar
2 Stringhini, S, Dugravot, A, Kivimaki, M, Shipley, M, Zins, M, Goldberg, M, et al Do different measures of early life socioeconomic circumstances predict adult mortality? Evidence from the British Whitehall II and French GAZEL studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 2011; 65: 1097–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Lorant, V, Deliege, D, Eaton, W, Robert, A, Philippot, P, Ansseau, M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol 2003; 157: 98112.Google Scholar
4 Fryers, T, Melzer, D, Jenkins, R. Social inequalities and the common mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003; 38: 229–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Jokela, M, Batty, GD, Vahtera, J, Elovainio, M, Kivimäki, M. Socioeconomic inequalities in common mental disorders and psychotherapy treatment in the UK between 1991 and 2009. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202: 115–20.Google Scholar
6 Sharp, ES, Gatz, M. Relationship between education and dementia: an updated systematic review. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2011; 25: 289304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Qiu, CX, Karp, A, von Strauss, E, Winblad, B, Fratiglioni, L, Bellander, T. Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer's disease in the Kungsholmen Project. Am J Ind Med 2003; 43: 204–11.Google Scholar
8 Persson, G, Skoog, I. A prospective population study of psychosocial risk factors for late onset dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1996; 11: 1522.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 Karp, A, Kåreholt, I, Qiu, C, Bellander, T, Winblad, B, Fratiglioni, L. Relation of education and occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident Alzheimer's disease. Am J Epidemiol 2004; 159: 175–83.Google Scholar
10 Mindell, J, Biddulph, JP, Hirani, V, Stamatakis, E, Craig, R, Nunn, S, et al Cohort profile: the Health Survey for England. Int J Epidemiol 2012; 41: 1585–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Macintyre, S, McKay, L, Der, G, Hiscock, R. Socio-economic position and health: what you observe depends on how you measure it. J Public Health 2003; 25: 288.Google Scholar
12 Royal College of Physicians of London. A Great and Growing Evil: The Medical Consequences of Alcohol Abuse. Royal College of Physicians of London, 1987.Google Scholar
13 Goldberg, DP, Gater, R, Sartorius, N, Ustun, T, Piccinelli, M, Gureje, O, et al The validity of two versions of the GHQ in the WHO study of mental illness in general health care. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 191–7.Google Scholar
14 Aalto, A-M, Elovainio, M, Kivimaki, M, Uutela, A, Pirkola, S. The Beck Depression Inventory and General Health Questionnaire as measures of depression in the general population: a validation study with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview as the gold standard. Psychiatry Res 2012; 197: 163–71.Google Scholar
15 Russ, TC, Hamer, M, Stamatakis, E, Starr, JM, Batty, GD. Psychological distress as a risk factor for dementia death. Arch Intern Med 2011; 171: 1858–9.Google Scholar
16 World Health Organization. Manual of the International Statistical Classification of Diseases, Injuries, and Causes of Death (9th revision). WHO, 1977.Google Scholar
17 World Health Organization. ICD-10: International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (10th revision). WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
18 Cox, DR. Regression models and life-tables. J Roy Stat Soc B 1972; 34: 187220.Google Scholar
19 Launer, L, Andersen, K, Dewey, ME, Letenneur, L, Ott, A, Amaducci, L, et al Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1999; 52: 7884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Batty, GD, Der, G, Macintyre, S, Deary, IJ. Does IQ explain socioeconomic inequalities in health? Evidence from a population based cohort study in the west of Scotland. BMJ 2006; 332: 580–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21 Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw 2010; 36: 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22 von Elm, E, Altman, D, Egger, M, Pocock, SJ, Gøtzsche, PC, Vandenbroucke, JP. Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. BMJ 2007; 335: 806–8.Google Scholar
23 Sampson, EL, Blanchard, MR, Jones, L, Tookman, A, King, M. Dementia in the acute hospital: prospective cohort study of prevalence and mortality. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 61–6.Google Scholar
24 Martyn, CN, Pippard, EC. Usefulness of mortality data in determining the geography and time trends of dementia. J Epidemiol Community Health 1988; 42: 134.Google Scholar
25 Russ, TC, Batty, GD, Starr, JM. Cognitive and behavioural predictors of survival in Alzheimer disease: results from a sample of treated patients in a tertiary-referral memory clinic. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 844–53.Google Scholar
26 Ferri, CP, Prince, M, Brayne, C, Brodaty, H, Fratiglioni, L, Ganguli, M, et al Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet 2006; 366: 2112–7.Google Scholar
27 MRC, CFAS. Cognitive function and dementia in six areas of England and Wales: the distribution of MMSE and prevalence of GMS organicity level in the MRC CFA Study. Psychol Med 1998; 28: 319–35.Google Scholar
28 Matthews, F, Brayne, C. The incidence of dementia in England and Wales: findings from the five identical sites of the MRC CFA Study. PLoS Med 2005; 2: e193.Google Scholar
29 Norton, MC, Ostbye, T, Smith, KR, Munger, RG, Tschanz, JT. Early parental death and late-life dementia risk: findings from the Cache County Study. Age Ageing 2009; 38: 340–3.Google Scholar
30 Whalley, LJ, Starr, JM, Athawes, R, Hunter, D, Pattie, A, Deary, IJ. Childhood mental ability and dementia. Neurology 2000; 55: 1455–9.Google Scholar
31 McGurn, B, Deary, IJ, Starr, JM. Childhood cognitive ability and risk of late-onset Alzheimer and vascular dementia. Neurology 2008; 71: 1051–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32 Snowdon, DA, Kemper, SJ, Mortimer, JA, Greiner, LH, Wekstein, DR, Markesbery, WR. Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life – findings from the Nun Study. JAMA 1996; 275: 528–32.Google ScholarPubMed
33 Stern, Y, Gurland, B, Tatemichi, TK, Tang, MX, Wilder, D, Mayeux, R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's Disease. JAMA 1994; 271: 1004–10.Google Scholar
34 Braak, H, Braak, E. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging 1997; 18: 351–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35 Stern, Y, Alexander, G, Prohovnik, I, Stricks, L, Link, B, Lennon, M, et al Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow. Neurology 1995; 45: 5560.Google Scholar
36 Gilleard, C. Education and Alzheimer's disease: a review of recent international epidemiological studies. Aging Ment Health 1997; 1: 3346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37 Del Ser, T, Hachinski, V, Merskey, H, Munoz, DG. An autopsy-verified study of the effect of education on degenerative dementia. Brain 1999; 122: 2309–19.Google Scholar
38 Stern, Y, Albert, S, Tang, MX, Tsai, WY. Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation: cognitive reserve? Neurology 1999; 53: 1942–7.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow chart of participants from initial pooled sample through to analytic sample showing subsequent mortality: the Health Survey for England 1994-2004.

Figure 1

Table 1 Characteristics of participantsa according to cohort study: the Health Survey for England 1994–2004

Figure 2

Table 2 Study members' baseline characteristics according to occupational social class: the Health Survey for England 1994–2004

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Fully adjusted hazard ratios (HRs) with 95% confidence intervals of dementia death by survey year for individuals, from left to right, from (a) skilled non-manual, (b) skilled manual and (c) semi-skilled/unskilled manual occupational social classes compared with professional/intermediate: the Health Survey for England 1994-2004.For women: I2 values for these models are 4.1%, 0.0% and 0.0%. For men: I2 values for these models are 0.0%, 12.9% and 6.4%.

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Fully adjusted hazard ratios (HRs) with 95% confidence intervals of dementia death by survey year for individuals who left full-time education aged, from left to right, (a) 15 years and (b) 14 years or younger compared with those who left full-time education aged 16 years or older: the Health Survey for England 1994-2004.For women: I2 values for these models are 0.0% and 0.0%. For men: I2 values for these models are 0.0% and 0.0%.

Figure 5

Table 3 Hazard ratio (95% confidence interval) for the association between occupational social class and educational attainment with dementia death: Health Survey for England 1994–2004

Supplementary material: PDF

Russ et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1-S3

Download Russ et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 48.4 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.