Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T09:44:50.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sensible Hypothesis Testing in Deluded, Depressed and Normal Subjects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Richard P. Bentall*
Affiliation:
University of Liverpool
Heather F. Young
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Reaside Clinic, Bristol Road South, Rubery, Birmingham B45 9BE
*
Professor P. Bentall, Department of Clinical Psychology, Whelan Building, University of Liverpool, PO Box 147, Liverpool L69 3BX

Abstract

Background

Previous research has indicated that deluded patients may experience difficulties when testing hypotheses. In this study, hypothesis-testing strategies were assessed in patients with persecutory delusions, depressed patients and normal controls.

Method

Subjects were presented problem items describing typical everyday situations with either positive or negative outcomes and were required to choose strategies to prove that one of three variables was responsible for the outcomes.

Results

Consistent with previous research into sensible reasoning, subjects chose to manipulate the variable hypothesised to be responsible for the outcome (disconfirmation strategy) more when the outcome was negative than when it was positive, and chose to manipulate the remaining variables (confirmation strategy) more when the outcome was positive. No group differences were observed.

Conclusions

No evidence was found of abnormal hypothesis-testing strategies in deluded patients.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 1996 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM–III–R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., et al (1961) An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 5363.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P. (1994) Cognitive biases and abnormal beliefs: Towards a model of persecutory delusions. In The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, (eds David, A. S. & Cutting, J.), pp. 337360. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chadwick, P. & Lowe, C. F. (1990) The measurement and modification of delusional beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 225232.Google Scholar
Farris, H. H. & Revlin, R. (1989) Sensible reasoning in two tasks: Rule discovery and hypothesis evaluation. Memory and Cognition, 17, 221232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P. A., Hemsley, D. R. & Wessely, S. (1991) Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 194201.Google Scholar
Garety, P. A., Kuipers, L., Fowler, D., et al (1994) Cognitive behavioural therapy for drug-resistant psychosis. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 67, 259271.Google Scholar
Huq, S. F., Garety, P. A. & Hemsley, D. R. (1988) Probabilistic judgements in deluded and nondeluded subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 801812.Google Scholar
John, C. H. & Dodgson, G. (1994) Inductive reasoning in delusional thought. Journal of Mental Health, 3, 3149.Google Scholar
Lea, M. (1981) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation Ltd.Google Scholar
Tschirgi, J. E. (1980) Sensible reasoning: A hypothesis about hypothesis. Child Development, 51, 110.Google Scholar
Wason, P. C. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972) Psychology of Reasoning. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Young, H. F. & Bentall, R. P. (1995) Hypothesis testing in patients with persecutory delusions: Comparison with depressed and normal subjects. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 353369.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.