Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T03:58:49.556Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Religion, spirituality and mental health: results from a national study of English households

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Michael King*
Affiliation:
Unit of Mental Health Sciences, Faculty of Brain Sciences, university College London Medical School, London
Louise Marston
Affiliation:
Department of Primary Care and Population Health, university College London Medical School, Royal Free Campus, London
Sally McManus
Affiliation:
National Centre for Social Research, London
Terry Brugha
Affiliation:
Academic Unit of Social and Epidemiological Psychiatry, Department of Health Sciences, university of Leicester, Leicester
Howard Meltzer
Affiliation:
Academic Unit of Social and Epidemiological Psychiatry, Department of Health Sciences, university of Leicester, Leicester
Paul Bebbington
Affiliation:
Unit of Mental Health Sciences, Faculty of Brain Sciences, university College London Medical School, London, UK
*
Michael King, Unit of Mental Health Sciences, Faculty of Brain Sciences, University College London Medical School, Charles Bell House, 67-73 Riding House Street, London W1W 7EH, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Religious participation or belief may predict better mental health but most research is American and measures of spirituality are often conflated with well-being.

Aims

To examine associations between a spiritual or religious understanding of life and psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses.

Method

We analysed data collected from interviews with 7403 people who participated in the third National Psychiatric Morbidity Study in England.

Results

Of the participants 35% had a religious understanding of life, 19% were spiritual but not religious and 46% were neither religious nor spiritual. Religious people were similar to those who were neither religious nor spiritual with regard to the prevalence of mental disorders, except that the former wereless likely to have ever used drugs (odds ratio (OR)=0.73, 95% CI 0.60-0.88) or be a hazardous drinker (OR=0.81, 95% CI 0.69-0.96). Spiritual people were more likely than those who were neither religious nor spiritual to have ever used (OR = 1.24, 95% CI 1.02-1.49) or be dependent on drugs (OR = 1.77, 95% CI 1.20-2.61), and to have abnormal eating attitudes (OR = 1.46, 95% Cl 1.10-1.94), generalised anxiety disorder (OR =1.50, 95% Cl 1.09-2.06), any phobia (OR = 1.72, 95% CI 1.07-2.77) or any neurotic disorder (OR = 1.37, 95% CI 1.12-1.68). They were also more likely to be taking psychotropic medication (OR = 1.40, 95% CI 1.05-1.86).

Conclusions

People who have a spiritual understanding of life in the absence of a religious framework are vulnerable to mental disorder.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

It is often reported that, even after taking account of other influences, such as age, gender and social support, people who report religious belief and practice experience better mental and physical health than those who do not. Reference Wong, Rew and Slaikeu1Reference Johnstone, Franklin, Yoon, Burris and Shigaki3 However, in a meta-analysis of 147 studies on religious belief and depression, Smith et al found only a weak negative correlation (–0.096) between religiousness and depressive symptoms. Reference Smith, McCullough and Poll4 On average, positive religiousness accounted for only 1% of the variance in the severity of depressive symptoms. Furthermore, extrinsic religious orientation and so-called negative religious coping (for example, avoiding difficulties through religious activities, blaming God for difficulties) were associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms.

The way religiosity is measured in research can be problematic, particularly when well-being is included as a component of the measure itself. Reference Koenig5 Furthermore, most research into this association has entailed Judeo-Christian concepts of religion in White, North American populations, and has seldom taken account of spiritual beliefs unconnected with personal or public religious practice. Reference King and Dein6 In a number of studies in the UK, we have reported that religious and spiritual beliefs are not linked to better physical health outcomes Reference King, Speck and Thomas7,Reference King, Speck and Thomas8 and, in particular that spiritual beliefs in the absence of a religious framework may be associated with poorer mental health. Reference King, Weich, Nazroo and Blizard9 The third National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey in England Reference McManus, Meltzer, Brugha, Bebbington and Jennings10 was the first general population survey in this country to include questions on religious and spiritual beliefs. In this study we aimed to examine the association between such beliefs and receipt of psychiatric treatment, and a range of psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses in a random sample of the English population.

Method

Survey methods

The third National Psychiatric Morbidity Study was conducted between October 2006 and December 2007 across England, using individual or groups of postcode sectors as sampling units. Reference McManus, Meltzer, Brugha, Bebbington and Jennings10 In the first stage of sampling, postcode sectors were divided into regions based on strategic health authority. All the sampling units within each strategic health authority were further stratified on the basis of the proportion of people in non-manual classes, and sorted by the proportion of households without a car based on UK 2001 Census data. A total of 519 postal sectors were selected by sampling from each stratum with a probability proportional to size in terms of the number of delivery points. In the second stage of sampling, 28 delivery points were randomly selected within each of the selected postal sectors, providing a sample of 14 532 delivery points. Interviewers visited the addresses to identify private households containing at least one person aged 16 or over. When visited, 1318 of the selected addresses were found not to contain a private household and were excluded from the survey sample. After contact was made with each eligible household, one person was randomly selected to take part, using the Kish method. Reference Kish11 Ethical constraints meant that data on people refusing to participate could not be collected. Trained interviewers conducted a structured, laptop computerised interview in respondents' homes, or elsewhere if requested, each interview lasting on average 90 min. Interviewers were fully briefed on the administration of the survey, and followed full sets of written instructions. Topics covered on 1-day survey-specific training included introducing the survey, questionnaire content, confidentiality and respondent distress.

The survey data were weighted to take account of likelihood of selection and non-response, so that the results were representative of the English household population aged 16 years and over. Sample weights were first applied to take account of the different probabilities of selecting respondents in different sized households. Second, to reduce household non-response bias, a household-level weight was calculated from a logistic regression model using interviewer observation and area-level variables (collected from Census 2001 data) available for responding and non-responding households. Finally, calibration weighting based on age, gender and region weighted the data to represent the structure of the national population, and take account of differential non-response between regions and age×gender groups.

We focus on the weighted results in the text, but provide both weighted and unweighted bases in the table showing sociodemographics and life views (online Table DS1). The unweighted bases are presented where appropriate to show the number of respondents included. The weighted base shows the relative size of the various sample elements after weighting, reflecting their proportions in the English population. The absolute size of the weighted base has no particular significance, since it has been scaled to the achieved sample size. Reference McManus, Meltzer, Brugha, Bebbington and Jennings10

Measures

Following confirmation of demographic data, the following standardised instruments were used.

  1. (a) Six questions adapted from those in the questionnaire version of the Royal Free interview for religious and spiritual beliefs, which assesses the nature and strength of beliefs and practice. Reference King, Speck and Thomas12,Reference King, Speck and Thomas13 Because of the length of the overall interview, questions on each topic had to be kept as brief as possible. Participants were provided with the statement: ‘By religion, we mean the actual practice of a faith, e.g. going to a temple, mosque, church or synagogue. Some people do not follow a religion but do have spiritual beliefs or experiences. Some people make sense of their lives without any religious or spiritual belief’. Although slightly shorter than in the Royal Free Interview, the words used are exactly the same, in order not to lose the statement's essential meaning or threatening the validity of the original instrument. The interview then went on: ‘Would you say that you have a religious or spiritual understanding of your life?’ Participants could indicate whether their understanding was predominately (i) religious; (ii) spiritual or (iii) neither. If they had a specific religion, they were asked to name it. If religious or spiritual, they were asked to indicate on two sliding scales of zero to ten how strongly they held to their understanding of life and how important practice of their faith (for example private meditation, religious services) was to them. These respondents were also asked to report how often they attended services, prayer meetings or places of worship.

  2. (b) The revised Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS-R) collects data on symptoms of common mental disorder and derives psychiatric diagnoses according ICD-10. Reference Goldberg, Cooper, Eastwood, Kedward and Shepherd1416 The CIS-R enquires about the presence and severity of 14 non-psychotic psychiatric symptoms during the week prior to interview. The CIS-R score may be analysed as: (i) a continuous score, along a single continuum of severity; (ii) a dichotomous variable (case threshold ⩾12); and (iii) ICD-10 diagnostic categories. Reference Lewis, Pelosi, Araya and Dunn15,Reference Meltzer, Gill and Petticrew17 Diagnostic algorithms for use with the CIS-R cover (i) depressive episodes (classified as mild, moderate or severe); (ii) four types of anxiety disorder, namely generalised anxiety disorder, panic disorder, phobias (classified as agoraphobia, social phobia and simple phobia), obsessive– compulsive disorder (OCD); and (iii) mixed anxiety/depressive disorder. The latter include those who scored above the case threshold on the CIS-R, but do not meet diagnostic criteria for any other ICD-10 disorder.

  3. (c) The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ) is a brief schedule that screens for symptoms commonly found in psychotic disorders. Reference Bebbington and Nayani18 Rather than making a definitive diagnosis, it indicates whether a psychotic disorder may be present.

  4. (d) A shortened version of the Close Persons Questionnaire that evaluates social support. Reference Stansfeld and Marmot19 This had been used in previous national surveys. Reference King, Weich, Nazroo and Blizard9,Reference Meltzer, Gill and Petticrew17 Seven questions enquire about the level of intimacy and social support from close friends and family. The answers are scored from one to three and are summed to a total score; higher scores indicate higher social support.

  5. (e) The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) questionnaire is a widely used and well validated instrument that contains 10 questions about use of, and attitudes to, alcohol consumption, in this instance over the preceding 12 months. Reference Barbor, de la Fuente, Saunders and Grant20,Reference Coulton, Drummond, James, Godfrey and Bland21 Men who scored more than seven and women who scored more than four were classified as hazardous drinkers.

  6. (f) The Trauma Screening Questionnaire (TSQ) Reference Walters, Bisson and Shepherd22 was used to collect information on the presence of trauma-related symptoms in the past week. A traumatic event is one in which a person experiences, witnesses or is confronted with a death or a serious injury or threat to self or close others. It is more severe than a stressful life event. Scoring positively on this questionnaire indicates the presence of trauma-related symptoms in the preceding week and that a clinical assessment for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is warranted; it does not specify a diagnosis of PTSD, in particular because it evaluates only two of the four criteria taken to identify a traumatising event. As a screening tool it has high sensitivity and specificity but lower positive predictive value (0.48) as a result of the usually low prevalence of PTSD (1%) in most populations. Reference Walters, Bisson and Shepherd22

  7. (g) The SCOFF is a five-item screening tool for anorexia and bulimia nervosa that enquires about eating attitudes and behaviour over the preceding year. Reference Morgan, Reid and Lacey23 Endorsement of two or more items represented a positive screening for an eating disorder. A further question (‘in the last year… did your feelings about food interfere with your ability to work, meet personal responsibilities, and/or enjoy a social life?’) was added to judge the impact of such eating attitudes and behaviour.

  8. (h) Problem gambling is gambling to a degree that compromises, disrupts or damages family, personal or recreational pursuits. This categorisation was based on the DSM-IV 24 criteria for problem and pathological gambling. Participants who met three or more of these diagnostic criteria were classified as problem gamblers.

  9. (i) Questions on use of recreational drugs came from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Reference Malgady, Rogler and Tryon25 Use of a drug in the past year and the presence of one of five symptoms were used to indicate drug dependence. Dependence was categorised as dependence on cannabis only and dependence on other drugs (with or without cannabis dependence).

  10. (j) One question explored how happy the participants felt ranging from very, to fairly and not too happy.

  11. (k) Finally, one binary variable summarised whether or not the respondent was taking any form of psychotropic medication and one summarised whether or not they were receiving any form of psychotherapy or counselling.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted in Stata release 12.0, for Windows, using the survey command to account for the unequal probability of selection and to adjust the standard errors to account for clustering in areas sampled in data collection. Our main analyses concerned possible associations between holding a religious, spiritual or secular life view and: receiving counselling/psychotherapy or psychotropic medication for a mental disorder; mental disorders classified by the CIS-R; possible PTSD, psychosis or an eating disorder; hazardous drinking; recreational drug use and dependence; and level of happiness.

We explored these associations using logistic regression, except for happiness, which utilised ordered logistic regression and for drug dependence where multinomial logistic regression was used. We adjusted for gender, age group, ethnicity, educational attainment, marital status or perceived social support if they were significantly associated with the given outcome. Social support is known to be associated with religious belief and practice. Reference Ferraro and Koch26,Reference George, Larson, Koenig and McCollough27 The reference group chosen for these analyses were participants reporting a secular (neither religious nor spiritual) life view, given it was the largest and most distinct of the three groups in terms of absence of any spiritual belief.

Results

Response rates and description of the population

A total of 9% of sampled addresses contained no private households and 4% were addresses of unknown eligibility. This left a known eligible sample of 12 694 addresses. Applying the eligibility rate among those where it was established, to those where it was not, it was estimated that 91% of those of unknown eligibility would have been eligible to take part. This increased the set sample of households to 13 171. In total, 7403 respondents (56.2%) provided a productive interview. The mean age of the participants was 46.3 years (s.e. = 0.28, unweighted range 16–97), 4206 (51.4%) were women, 85% were White British, 26% had no educational qualifications and 52% were married (online Table DS1).

Table 1 Univariable associations with life view: sociodemographics (n = 7403)

Characteristics Neither religious or spiritual Spiritual Religious P
Male, % 55 44 43 <0.001
Age, years: % <0.001
    16–24 19 11 10
    25–34 19 16 13
    35–44 20 21 17
    45–54 16 18 16
    55–64 13 17 17
    65–74 7 10 14
    75+ 5 8 14
Ethnicity, % <0.001
    White British 93 86 74
    White non-British 4 6 6
    Black 1 3 6
    South Asian 1 2 9
    Mixed or Other 2 3 4
Education, % <0.001
    No qualifications 25 21 30
    School or foreign qualifications 51 46 39
    Post-school qualifications 24 32 30
Civil status, % <0.001
    Married 46 53 60
    Cohabiting 13 12 6
    Single 28 21 17
    Widowed 5 6 10
    Divorced 6 7 5
    Separated 2 2 2
Social support, mean 20.0 20.2 20.3 <0.001
Strength of religious understanding, meanFootnote a 6.2 7.0 <0.001
Importance of religious practice, meanFootnote a 4.4 6.2 <0.001

a. Only applicable for those who are religious or spiritual.

Understanding of life

Of the participants 35% had a religious understanding of life, 19% were spiritual (without religious participation) and 46% were neither religious nor spiritual in outlook (online Table DS1). In total 53% gave a nominal religious affiliation, with the majority citing Christianity (86%). Demographic characteristics of the sample stratified by understanding of life are shown in Table 1. People with no religious or spiritual understanding were significantly younger and more often White British, but were less likely to have qualifications beyond secondary school or to be married. Religious people had stronger beliefs and placed more importance on practice of those beliefs than people with a purely spiritual understanding (Table 1).

Mental disorders and substance use

People with a religious understanding of life were less likely to use or be dependent on drugs, or be hazardous drinkers (Table 2). People with a spiritual understanding of life were more likely to have abnormal eating attitudes and any neurotic disorder. There were no differences in overall happiness or level of social support. People with no religious or spiritual understanding were least likely to be taking psychotropic medication.

Comparison of groups after adjustment for other influences

In a multivariate regression, we selected those people with neither a religious nor spiritual understanding of life (the largest group) to serve as a reference against which to compare the other two groups, namely those with a religious understanding and those with a spiritual understanding of life. After adjustment for important associated influences, there were few substantial differences in prevalence of mental disorder or receipt of treatment between people with no religious or spiritual understanding and those who were religious, except for less drug use and hazardous drinking in the latter (Table 3). However, people with a spiritual understanding were more likely than those who were neither religious nor spiritual to take psychotropic medication, to use recreational drugs or be dependent on them, to have a generalised anxiety disorder, phobia or any neurotic disorder or to have abnormal eating attitudes.

Discussion

Main findings

Our main finding is that people who had a spiritual understanding of life had worse mental health than those with an understanding that was neither religious nor spiritual. Those who were religious were broadly similar, in terms of prevalence of mental disorder and use of mental health treatments, to those who were neither religious nor spiritual after adjustment for potential confounders, except they were significantly less likely to use, or be dependent on, drugs or alcohol. This supports evidence from a national UK sample of people from a range of Black and minority ethnic groups Reference King, Weich, Nazroo and Blizard9 on the vulnerability of people who describe themselves as spiritual. It adds to the evidence that people with a spiritual understanding in the absence of a religious framework appear to have the worst mental health. It also confirms that religious people are less likely to use alcohol Reference Michalak, Trocki and Bond28 and recreational drugs Reference Hope and Cook29 but fails to confirm North American evidence that holding a religious understanding of life provides protection against mental disorders. Reference Koenig30 It also concurs with other evidence from England that there is no clear relationship between religiosity and happiness. Reference Lewis, Maltby and Day31

Table 2 Univariable associations with life view: outcomes (n = 7403)

%
Neither religious or spiritual Spiritual Religious P
Drug use
    Ever used drugs 32 30 16 <0.001
    Used any drug in the past year 12 11 5 <0.001
    Drug dependent 4 5 2 <0.001
        Dependent on cannabis 3 4 1 <0.001
        Dependent on another drug (+/– cannabis) 1 1 1
Hazardous drinker 30 23 17 <0.001
Problem gambling 0.8 0.6 0.7 0.880
Post-traumatic stress disorder 3 3 3 0.527
Eating attitudes
    SCOFF ≥2 7 9 5 0.001
    SCOFF score ≥2 and food interferes with lifeFootnote a 2 2 1 0.646
Psychosis Screening Questionnaire
    Definitely psychotic 0.1 0.4 0.4 0.106
    Probably psychotic 0.3 0.6 0.5 0.118
Clinical Interview Schedule
    Panic disorder 1 1 1 0.618
    Generalised anxiety disorder 4 5 4 0.079
    Mixed anxiety/depressive disorder 8 10 8 0.154
    Obsessive–compulsive disorder 1 1 0.8 0.127
    Any phobia 2 3 2 0.055
    Depression 3 3 2 0.057
    Any neurotic disorder 16 19 15 0.011
Happiness 0.764
    Very happy 39 39 40
    Fairly happy 53 52 52
    Not too happy 8 9 8
Receiving pharmacological treatment 5 7 6 0.026
Receiving counselling/therapy 3 3 2 0.360

a. ≥2 on the SCOFF questionnaire and an affirmative reply to whether or not the eating difficulties has a serious impact on their lives.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study are the random selection of a nationally representative sample of participants, the sample size, an in-depth assessment of mental health and the use of standardised questions on religion and spirituality that were developed and standardised in the UK and do not overlap with questions on well-being. However, there are also a number of limitations. First, our results depend on the definitions of religion and spirituality that were posed to participants. Although spirituality is notoriously difficult to define, Reference King and Koeing32 this way of describing it to research participants has at least been shown to be reliable. Reference King, Speck and Thomas12,Reference King, Speck and Thomas13 However, we did not examine religious belief in detail, nor to what extent it was intrinsic or extrinsic. Reference Pargament33 Second, the cross-sectional nature of the data means that we cannot attribute cause and effect to any relationship between spiritual beliefs and mental health.

Table 3 Regression modelling showing the relationship between psychiatric outcomes and life viewFootnote a

OR (95% CI)
Outcome n SpiritualFootnote b Religious (+/– spiritual)Footnote b
Drug use
    Ever used drugs 7166 1.24 (1.02–1.49) 0.73 (0.60–0.88)
    Used any drug in the past year 7298 1.38 (1.06–1.81) 0.64 (0.47–0.87)
    Drug dependent 7173 1.77 (1.20–2.61) 0.68 (0.43–1.09)
        Dependent on cannabisFootnote c 7173 1.86 (1.18–2.91) 0.46 (0.25–0.84)
        Dependent on other drugs (+/– cannabis) 1.55 (0.78–3.11) 1.34 (0.66–2.74)
Gambler 6890 0.95 (0.33–2.69) 1.00 (0.44–2.27)
Post-traumatic stress disorder 7036 1.24 (0.81–1.88) 1.22 (0.85–1.76)
Eating attitudes
    SCOFF ≥2 and food interferes with life 7298 1.01 (0.58–1.76) 1.10 (0.66–1.85)
    SCOFF ≥2 7302 1.46 (1.10–1.94) 1.00 (0.78–1.28)
Clinical Interview Schedule
    Panic 7325 1.33 (0.71–2.47) 1.41 (0.81–2.45)
    Generalised anxiety disorder 7203 1.50 (1.09–2.06) 1.29 (0.97–1.70)
    Mixed anxiety/depressive disorder 7203 1.26 (0.98–1.62) 1.04 (0.84–1.30)
    Obsessive–compulsive disorder 7203 1.11 (0.60–2.04) 0.87 (0.48–1.58)
    Any phobia 7203 1.72 (1.07–2.77) 1.26 (0.84–1.90)
    Depression 7203 1.05 (0.72–1.51) 0.80 (0.57–1.13)
    Any neurotic disorder 7203 1.37 (1.12–1.68) 1.14 (0.94–1.37)
Hazardous drinker 7315 0.93 (0.78–1.11) 0.81 (0.69–0.96)
Happiness 7192 1.01 (0.87–1.18) 0.95 (0.84–1.08)
Receiving psychotropic treatment 7175 1.40 (1.05–1.86) 1.18 (0.89–1.56)
Receiving counselling or therapy 7325 1.15 (0.77–1.73) 1.00 (0.68–1.47)

a. Numbers were insufficient to investigate psychosis. Results in bold are significant.

b. Reference category is not religious or spiritual, models adjust for those that are significant from age, gender, marital status, ethnicity, educational attainment and social support.

c. Reference category for the outcome is no drug use.

Comparison with findings from other studies

A recent large USA internet survey of people who regarded themselves as non-religious Reference Galen34 also reported that non-religious people who held spiritual beliefs (only 2% of the survey population) had less emotional stability as measured by a personality rating scale. However, it is difficult to know how far we can compare such people with our spiritual group, which made up 19% of the study population. Although the authors concluded that belief is a measure of negative personality traits, their findings (and ours) are in opposition to earlier evidence from the UK that spirituality is associated with greater extraversion and optimism (rather than neuroticism and psychoticism), particularly among men. Reference Maltby and Day35 The limitation of the latter study, however was that it was based on a volunteer student population and is therefore unlikely to be generalisable. Another possible explanation for our finding concerning people with a spiritual life view is that they are caught up in an existential search that is driven by their emotional distress. Reference King, Weich, Nazroo and Blizard9,Reference Bhui, King, Dein and O'Connor36

Why might findings in Europe concerning the emotional stability of religious people vary from those in the USA? There is considerable evidence that the UK is a less religious country than the USA in terms of the number of people professing a belief in God or attending places of worship. Reference Taylor37 We found that a relatively low number of people expressed some sort of spiritual or religious belief in this survey (54%) in comparison with that found in recent surveys both in the UK and around the world where levels have been closer to 70%. Reference Ashworth and Farthing38,39 Given that religious participation in English society is a minority activity, it may be that the social support intrinsic to a more religious society is missing. A further possibility is the way in which we measured spiritual and religious understanding. The Royal Free Interview for Religious and Spiritual Beliefs was developed and standardised in the UK and has not been used extensively in the USA. However, this is not strictly a limitation as one could argue that instruments should be most appropriate to the setting in which they were developed. Certainly, the difference in the findings concerning religion and mental health between the USA and the UK would bear greater investigation.

We conclude that there is increasing evidence that people who profess spiritual beliefs in the absence of a religious framework are more vulnerable to mental disorder. The nature of this association needs greater examination in qualitative and in prospective quantitative research.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Wong, YJ, Rew, L, Slaikeu, KD. A systematic review of recent research on adolescent religiosity/spirituality and mental health. Issues Ment Health Nurs 2006; 27: 161–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Koenig, HK, McCullough, ME, Larson, DB. Handbook of Religion and Health. Oxford University Press, 2001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3 Johnstone, B, Franklin, KL, Yoon, DP, Burris, J, Shigaki, C. Relationships among religiousness, spirituality, and health for individuals with stroke. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 2008; 15: 308–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Smith, TB, McCullough, ME, Poll, J. Religiousness and depression: evidence for a main effect and the moderating influence of stressful life events. Psychol Bull 2003; 129: 614–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Koenig, HG. Concerns about measuring “spirituality” in research. J Nerv Ment Dis 2008; 196: 349–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 King, MB, Dein, S. The spiritual variable in psychiatric research. Psychol Med 1998; 28: 1259–62.Google Scholar
7 King, M, Speck, P, Thomas, A. Spiritual and religious beliefs in acute illness is this a feasible area for study? Soc Sci Med 1994; 38: 631–6.Google Scholar
8 King, M, Speck, P, Thomas, A. The effect of spiritual beliefs on outcome from illness. Soc Sci Med 1999; 48: 1291–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 King, M, Weich, S, Nazroo, J, Blizard, R. Religion, mental health and ethnicity. EMPIRIC - a national survey of England. J Ment Health 2006; 15: 153–62.Google Scholar
10 McManus, S, Meltzer, H, Brugha, T, Bebbington, P, Jennings, R. Adult Psychiatric Morbidity in England 2007. Results of a Household Survey. The NHS Information Centre for Health and Social Care, 2009.Google Scholar
11 Kish, L. Survey Sampling. Wiley, 1965.Google Scholar
12 King, M, Speck, P, Thomas, A. The Royal Free Interview for Religious and Spiritual Beliefs: development and standardization. Psychol Med 1995; 25: 1125–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13 King, M, Speck, P, Thomas, A. The Royal Free Interview for Spiritual and Religious Beliefs: development and validation of a self-report version. Psychol Med 2001; 31: 1015–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14 Goldberg, DP, Cooper, B, Eastwood, MR, Kedward, HB, Shepherd, M. A standardized psychiatric interview for use in community surveys. Br J Prev Soc Med 1970; 24: 1823.Google Scholar
15 Lewis, G, Pelosi, AJ, Araya, R, Dunn, G. Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychol Med 1992; 22: 465–86.Google Scholar
16 World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
17 Meltzer, H, Gill, B, Petticrew, M. OPCS Surveys of Psychiatric Morbidity in Great Britain. Report No 1. The Prevalence of Psychiatric Morbidity among Adults aged 16-64 Living in Private Households in Great Britain. HMSO, 1995.Google Scholar
18 Bebbington, P, Nayani, T. The psychosis screening questionnaire. Int J Meth in Psychiatr Res 1995; 5: 11–9.Google Scholar
19 Stansfeld, S, Marmot, M. Deriving a survey measure of social support: the reliability and validity of the Close Persons Questionnaire. Soc Sci Med 1992; 35: 1027–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Barbor, TF, de la Fuente, JR, Saunders, J, Grant, M. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for the Use in Primary Health Care. World Health Organization, 1989.Google Scholar
21 Coulton, S, Drummond, C, James, D, Godfrey, C, Bland, JM, Parrott S, et al Opportunistic screening for alcohol use disorders in primary care: comparative study. BMJ 2006; 332: 511–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 Walters, JTR, Bisson, JI, Shepherd, JP. Predicting post-traumatic stress disorder: validation of the Trauma Screening Questionnaire in victims of assault. Psychol Med 2007; 37: 143–50.Google Scholar
23 Morgan, JF, Reid, F, Lacey, JH. The SCOFF questionnaire: assessment of a new screening tool for eating disorders. BMJ 1999; 319: 1467–8.Google Scholar
24 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (4th edn) (DSM-IV). APA, 1994.Google Scholar
25 Malgady, RG, Rogler, LH, Tryon, WW. Issues of validity in the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. J Psychiatr Res 1992; 26: 5967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26 Ferraro, KF, Koch, JR. Religion and health among black and white adults: examining social support and consolation. J Sci Study Relig 1994; 33: 362–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27 George, LK, Larson, DB, Koenig, HG, McCollough, ME. Spirituality and health: what we know, what we need to know. J Soc Clin Psychology 2000; 19: 102–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28 Michalak, L, Trocki, K, Bond, J. Religion and alcohol in the U.S. National Alcohol Survey: how important is religion for abstention and drinking? Drug Alcohol Depend 2007; 87: 268–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29 Hope, LC, Cook, CCH. The role of Christian commitment in predicting drug use amongst church affiliated young people. Ment Health Relig Cult 2001; 4: 109–17.Google Scholar
30 Koenig, HG. Handbook of Religion and Mental Health. Academic Press, 1998.Google Scholar
31 Lewis, CA, Maltby, J, Day, L. Religious orientation, religious coping and happiness among UK adults. Pers Individ Dif 2005; 38: 1193–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32 King, M, Koeing, H. Conceptualising spirituality for medical research and health service provision. BMC Health Serv Res 2009; 9: 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Pargament, KI. Psychology of Religion and Coping. Theory, Research and Practice. Guilford Press, 1997.Google Scholar
34 Galen, LW. Profile of the godless. Results of a survey of the nonreligious. Free Inq 2009; 29: 41–5.Google Scholar
35 Maltby, J, Day, L. Spiritual involvement and belief: the relationship between spirituality and Eysenck's personality dimensions. Pers Individ Dif 2001; 30: 187–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36 Bhui, K, King, M, Dein, S, O'Connor, W. Ethnicity and religious coping with mental distress. Ment Health 2008; 17: 141–51.Google Scholar
37 Taylor, H. Harris Poll: The Religious and Other Beliefs of Americans 2003. Harris Interactive, 2003 (http://www.harrisinteractive.com/vault/Harris-Interactive-Poll-Research-The-Religious-and-Other-Beliefs-of-Americans-2003-2003-02.pdf)).Google Scholar
38 Ashworth, J, Farthing, I. Churchgoing in the UK. 2007. Tearfund, 2007.Google Scholar
39 ICM Research. What the world thinks of God. BBC News Channel 2004; 26 February (http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/programmes/wtwtgod/3518375.stm).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Univariable associations with life view: sociodemographics (n = 7403)

Figure 1

Table 2 Univariable associations with life view: outcomes (n = 7403)

Figure 2

Table 3 Regression modelling showing the relationship between psychiatric outcomes and life viewa

Supplementary material: PDF

King et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1

Download King et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 34.4 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.