Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:26:15.912Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Quantitative analysis of sponsorship bias in economic studies of antidepressants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

C. Bruce Baker*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, and Connecticut Mental Health Center, New Haven, Connecticut
Michael T. Johnsrud
Affiliation:
Center for Pharmacoeconomic Studies, University of Texas, Austin, Texas
M. Lynn Crismon
Affiliation:
Center for Pharmacoeconomic Studies, University of Texas, and Office of the Medical Director, Texas Department of Mental Health and Mental Retardation, Austin, Texas
Robert A. Rosenheck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, and VA Northeast Program Evaluation Center, West Haven, Connecticut, and Yale School of Medicine Departments of Epidemiology and Public Health, New Haven, Connecticut
Scott W. Woods
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine; Connecticut Mental Health Center, New Haven, Connecticut, USA
*
Dr C. Bruce Baker, Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, CMHC Rm 38B, 34 Park St, New Haven, CT 06519, USA. Tel: (203) 974 7051; fax: (203) 974 7057; e-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Concern is widespread about potential sponsorship influence on research, especially in pharmacoeconomic studies. Quantitative analysis of possible bias in such studies is limited.

Aims

To determine whether there is an association between sponsorship and quantitative outcomes in pharmacoeconomic studies of antidepressants.

Method

Using all identifiable articles with original comparative quantitative cost or cost-effectiveness outcomes for antidepressants, we performed contingency table analyses of study sponsorship and design v. study outcome.

Results

Studies sponsored by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSR1) manufacturers favoured SSRIs over tricyclic antidepressants more than non-industry-sponsored studies. Studies sponsored by manufacturers of newer antidepressants favoured these drugs more than did non-industry-sponsored studies. Among industry-sponsored studies, modelling studies favoured the sponsor's drug more than did administrative studies. Industry-sponsored modelling studies were more favourable to industry than were non-industry-sponsored ones.

Conclusions

Pharmacoeconomic studies of antidepressants reveal clear associations of study sponsorship with quantitative outcome.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2003 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Long-standing concern exists about the potential influence of financial interests on medical decision-making (e.g. Reference Hillman, Joseph and MagelHillman et al, 1990; Reference Rennie and FlanaginRennie & Flanagin, 1992). Especially vigorous discussion has centred on the conduct and reporting of pharmacoeconomic research (e.g. Reference Hillman, Eisenberg and PaulyHillman et al, 1991; Reference Udrarhelyi, Graham and RaiUdrarhelyi et al, 1992; Reference Gulati and BitranGulati & Bitran, 1995; Reference Siegel, Weinstein and RussellSiegel et al, 1996; Reference NeumannNeumann, 1998; Reference Hill, Mitchell and HenryHill et al, 2000; Reference Jones and CockrumJones & Cockrum, 2000; Reference Neumann, Stone and ChapmanNeumann et al, 2000b). However, there has been little quantitative study of potential bias in pharmacoeconomic research throughout medicine. Reported studies have reached mixed conclusions (e.g. Reference Sacristan, Bolanos and SotoSacristan et al, 1997; Reference Azimi and WelchAzimi & Welch, 1998; Reference Friedberg, Saffran and StinsonFriedberg et al, 1999; Reference Neumann, Sandberg and BellNeumann et al, 2000a), perhaps in part because with one exception (Reference Friedberg, Saffran and StinsonFriedberg et al, 1999) they investigated several drugs and in some cases included medical devices. We are unaware of any study focused on psychiatric medication.

We studied associations between sponsorship and study design with quantitative outcome in pharmacoeconomic studies by examining the test case of antidepressants. We asked the following primary questions. First, is there an association between industry v. non-industry sponsorship of studies and quantitative conclusions? Second, among industry-sponsored studies and between industry-sponsored v. non-industry-sponsored studies, is there an association between study design and quantitative conclusions?

METHOD

We chose antidepressants licensed in the UK or the USA as our test case because of their large market share and the number of pharmacoeconomic studies. Antidepressants rank in the top three drug classes world-wide in terms of sales dollars. Their growth in sales ranks them among the top five drug classes worldwide (IMS Health, 2001). Additionally, these antidepressants are the subject of multiple cost-outcome studies reporting quantitative results.

Study sample

To locate reports of pharmacoeconomic studies of antidepressant drugs we used the Cochrane Library, Medline and Health-STAR databases supplemented by manual searches based on the references cited in the studies located through the databases. We searched for all articles between 1987 - the year the first 'newer' antidepressant, fluoxetine, received US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approval - and April 2001. The search terms we used in Medline and HealthSTAR were COST-BENEFIT ANALYSIS or COST SAVINGS or DRUG COSTS or COST-EFFECTIVENESS (text word) and ANTIDEPRESSIVE AGENTS or ANTIDEPRESSANT (text word). The search term we used in the Cochrane Library was ANTIDEPRESSIVE AGENTS. We identified 46 articles (Jonsson & Bebbington, Reference Jonsson, Bebbington, Jonsson and Rosenbaum1993, Reference Jonsson and Bebbington1994; Reference Hatziandreu, Brown and RevickiHatziandreu et al, 1994; Reference Le Pen, Levy and RavilyLe Pen et al, 1994; Reference McFarlandMcFarland, 1994; Sclar et al, Reference Sclar, Robison and Skaer1994, Reference Sclar, Robison and Skaer1995, Reference Sclar, Skaer and Robison1998, Reference Sclar, Skaer and Robison1999; Reference StewartStewart, 1994; Reference Anton and RevickiAnton & Revicki, 1995; Einarson et al, Reference Einarson, Arikian and Sweeney1995, Reference Einarson, Addis and Iskedjian1997; Reference Lapierre, Bentkover and SchainbaumLapierre et al, 1995; Reference Nuijten, Hardens and SoutreNuijten et al, 1995; Revicki et al, Reference Revicki, Brown and Palmer1995, Reference Revicki, Brown and Keller1997; Reference Skaer, Sclar and RobisonSkaer et al, 1995; Reference Bentkover and FeighnerBentkover & Feighner, 1996; Reference Forder, Kavanagh and FenyoForder et al, 1996; Hylan et al, Reference Hylan, Kotsanos and Andersen1996, Reference Hylan, Crown and Meneades1998; Reference Montgomery, Brown and ClarkMontgomery et al, 1996; Reference Smith and SherrillSmith & Sherrill, 1996; Croghan et al, Reference Croghan, Lair and Engelhart1997, Reference Croghan, Kniesner and Melfi2000; Reference Melton, Kirkwood and FarrarMelton et al, 1997; Reference Obenchain, Melfi and CroghanObenchain et al, 1997; Reference Woods and RizzoWoods & Rizzo, 1997; Reference Boyer, Danion and BisserbeBoyer et al, 1998; Canadian Coordinating Office for Health Technology Assessment, 1998; Reference Crown, Hylan and MeneadesCrown et al, 1998; Reference Simon and FishmanSimon & Fishman, 1998; Reference Thompson, Hylan and McMullenThompson et al, 1998; Brown et al, Reference Brown, Nimmerrichter and Guest1999a,Reference Brown, van Loon and Guestb; Reference Griffiths, Sullivan and FrankGriffiths et al, 1999; Reference Nurnberg, Hensley and ThompsonNurnberg et al, 1999; Reference Russell, Berndt and MiceliRussell et al, 1999; Reference Simon, Heilegenstein and RevickiSimon et al, 1999; Reference Borghi and GuestBorghi & Guest, 2000; Reference Sullivan, Griffiths and FrankSullivan et al, 2000; Reference Casciano, Doyle and ArikianCasciano et al, 2001; Reference Doyle, Casciano and ArikianDoyle et al, 2001; Reference Poret, Neslusan and RicciPoret et al, 2001; Reference Wan, Crown and BerndtWan et al, 2002). We excluded two studies (Reference Boyer, Danion and BisserbeBoyer et al, 1998; Reference Simon, Heilegenstein and RevickiSimon et al, 1999) because they were randomised trials, unlike all the other studies, which were modelling studies or analyses of administrative databases. The remaining articles represent 45 separate studies. Two articles report the results of one study (Jonsson & Bebbington, Reference Jonsson, Bebbington, Jonsson and Rosenbaum1993, Reference Jonsson and Bebbington1994). Two articles report two studies each, one in-patient, one out-patient (Einarson et al, Reference Einarson, Arikian and Sweeney1995, Reference Einarson, Addis and Iskedjian1997). Two articles reported slight variations on two studies, one in-patient and one out-patient (Reference Casciano, Doyle and ArikianCasciano et al, 2001; Reference Doyle, Casciano and ArikianDoyle et al, 2001).

Classification of studies

For the primary analysis we categorised each study according to whether it was industry-sponsored. The study was categorised as industry-sponsored if at least one author was listed as a pharmaceutical company employee, or an acknowledgement listed pharmaceutical company support; otherwise, it was categorised as non-industry-sponsored. For secondary analyses we categorised studies authored by industry employees separately from studies only listing financial support.

Study sponsors were categorised by product into those manufacturing selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs: fluoxetine, sertraline, paroxetine and citalopram) or ‘atypical’ antidepressant drugs (venlafaxine, bupropion and mirtazapine).

Operationalisation of outcomes

For either of the questions posed in our study no single means of operationalising the issue of which antidepressant was favoured could be applied to all studies. Therefore, we performed separate analyses using alternative operationalisations. Specifically, for question one (the industry v. non-industry comparison), no single standard was applicable that allowed analysis of all 46 studies. Seemingly simple standards such as ‘sponsor's antidepressant favoured' could not apply: in non-industry-sponsored studies, there is no ‘sponsor's antidepressant'. In our primary depressant'. analysis of industry-sponsored v. non-industry-sponsored studies we examined whether the outcome favoured SSRIs or tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs), excluding studies sponsored by ‘atypical’ antidepressant manufacturers. To allow analysis of the latter studies, we performed an alternative analysis based on whether the outcome favoured the ‘newest antidepressant’ (‘newness’ was based upon date of FDA approval). In this analysis, studies in which the sponsor's drug was not the newest were excluded.

In addressing our second question, regarding the association of study design with bias on outcome, we examined the issue both within industry-sponsored trials and between industry-sponsored and non-industry-sponsored trials. Within the first group we looked at the association of modelling v. administrative study designs with outcome. We operationalised the outcomes and groups in two alternative ways: favouring the newest drug among all industry-sponsored studies, or favouring the sponsored drug among all industry-sponsored studies.

In examining the association of study design with outcome between industry v. non-industry sponsors, we compared the outcome patterns within modelling studies. We could not compare outcome patterns in administrative data studies given there was only one such non-industry-sponsored study. We operationalised outcomes in two alternative ways: favouring the newest drug, or favouring SSRIs v. TCAs.

Rating study outcomes

Initially two of the authors (C.B.B. and M.N.J.) independently categorised sponsorship and outcomes of each study. If their ratings were inconsistent, a third author (S.W.W.) rated the study. Initial ratings agreed in all cases but one.

Most studies contained several outcomes. However, we wished to rate a single outcome from each study and employed the following decision rules to select that outcome. First, we selected only quantitative outcomes. Second, among base case and variants, we selected the base case. Third, among outcomes adjusted for bias and unadjusted outcomes, we selected the adjusted outcome. Fourth, among outcomes for various time periods, we selected the longest period. Fifth, among multiple pharmacoeconomic indicators, we selected a single outcome on the basis of the following rules: if only cost outcomes were reported, we chose total costs over more limited costs; if cost and cost-effectiveness outcomes were reported, we chose cost-effectiveness outcomes; and if more than one type of cost-effectiveness ratio was reported, we chose incremental over average ratios. Sixth, if results were reported separately for individual countries, we selected the results for the UK and the USA.

After selecting a single outcome for each study, the researchers rated each study as favourable, neutral or unfavourable for the drug of interest, depending on the particular analysis (e.g. SSRI in the SSRI v. TCA analysis, or newest antidepressant in the newest v. older antidepressant analysis): ‘favourable’ meant that a drug's quantitative cost-effectiveness results were unequalled by any of the other drugs in the study; ‘neutral’ meant that although other drugs' results might be equal to it, none surpassed the drug of interest; and ‘unfavourable’ meant that other drugs' results did surpass the drug of interest. Raters used all available information to judge differences in outcomes among drugs. If the study reported statistical significance, raters based their judgements on statistically significant differences. If the study did not report statistical significance, raters based their judgements on the reported numerical differences. With quality-adjusted life-years (QALYs), raters judged a treatment superior if marginal cost-effectiveness was less than US$20 000 per QALY, a common applied limit (Reference Laupacis, Feeny and DetskyLaupacis et al, 1992). Subsequently, we performed a sensitivity analysis by varying the marginal threshold between $20 000 and $100 000 per QALY.

The following is an example of how raters applied the rules noted above to designate a specific study as favourable, neutral or unfavourable. In the SSRI v. tricyclic or heterocyclic antidepressant analysis of the Hatziandreu study (Reference Hatziandreu, Brown and RevickiHatziandreu et al, 1994) the preceding rules led raters to judge that the study favoured the SSRI. The study reported the base case incremental cost-effectiveness ratio to be £2172 ($3692) for each QALY gained by using the SSRI rather than the TCA. This cost per QALY gained is less than the $20 000 per QALY cut-off noted in the raters' decision rules; therefore, the study was rated as favourable for the SSRI.

In addition to the planned analyses described above, we performed two exploratory analyses: one was based on the number of industry authors and the second was based on the ordinal position of any industry authors. Neither of these analyses yielded a significant association.

We analysed the association between sponsorship and outcome using Fisher's exact test as generalised for 2×3 tables. We chose contingency table analysis rather than a meta-analytic technique because of the qualitative heterogeneity of the pharmacoeconomic outcome types across studies, which ranged from direct costs per patient, to direct costs per treatment success, to direct costs per symptom-free day, to lifetime direct costs per discounted QALY. We judged it inappropriate to transform these qualitatively disparate types of outcomes into a common effect size. We selected the 0.05 α level, two-tailed.

RESULTS

Details of the studies are listed in Tables 1, 2, 3.

Table 1 Economic outcome studies of antidepressant therapy sponsored by manufacturers of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors

Study Study design Sponsor's antidepressant (or class) Newest antidepressant is sponsor's (newest) Control antidepressant (or class) Study favours SSRI or SSRI class v. TCA/HCA1 Study favours newest antidepressant or class1 Study favours sponsor's antidepressant1
Jonsson & Bebbington (Reference Jonsson, Bebbington, Jonsson and Rosenbaum1993, Reference Jonsson and Bebbington1994) Simulation Paroxetine Yes Imipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Hatziandreu et al (Reference Hatziandreu, Brown and Revicki1994) A Simulation Sertraline Yes Dothiepin Favourable Favourable Favourable
Lapierre et al (Reference Lapierre, Bentkover and Schainbaum1995) A Simulation Paroxetine Yes Imipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Nuijten et al (Reference Nuijten, Hardens and Soutre1995) Simulation Citalopram Yes TCAs Favourable Favourable Favourable
Bentkover & Feighner (Reference Bentkover and Feighner1996) Simulation Paroxetine Yes Imipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Hylan et al (Reference Hylan, Kotsanos and Andersen1996) A Simulation Fluoxetine Yes Imipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Sclar et al (Reference Sclar, Robison and Skaer1994) A Administrative databases Fluoxetine Yes Amitriptyline, nortriptyline, desipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Sclar et al (Reference Sclar, Robison and Skaer1995) A Administrative databases Fluoxetine No (paroxetine) Sertraline, paroxetine Not applicable Not applicable Favourable
Skaer et al (Reference Skaer, Sclar and Robison1995) Administrative databases Sertraline Yes Amitriptyline, nortriptyline, desipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Forder et al (Reference Forder, Kavanagh and Fenyo1996) Administrative databases Sertraline Yes TCAs Favourable Favourable Favourable
Smith & Sherrill (Reference Smith and Sherrill1996) A Administrative databases SSRI class Yes TCAs Neutral Neutral Neutral
Melton et al (Reference Melton, Kirkwood and Farrar1997) Administrative databases Paroxetine Yes TCAs Neutral Neutral Neutral
Obenchain et al (Reference Obenchain, Melfi and Croghan1997) A Administrative databases Fluoxetine Yes TCAs, trazodone Favourable Favourable Favourable
Croghan et al (Reference Croghan, Lair and Engelhart1997) A Administrative databases Fluoxetine Yes TCAs, trazodone Neutral Neutral Neutral
Crown et al (Reference Crown, Hylan and Meneades1998) A Administrative databases SSRI class Yes TCAs Favourable Favourable Favourable
Hylan et al (Reference Hylan, Crown and Meneades1998) A Administrative databases Fluoxetine No (paroxetine) TCAs, paroxetine, sertraline Not applicable Not applicable Favourable
Sclar et al (Reference Sclar, Skaer and Robison1998) Administrative databases Fluoxetine Yes Amitriptyline, nortriptyline Favourable Favourable Favourable
Thompson et al (Reference Thompson, Hylan and McMullen1998) A Administrative databases SSRI class Yes TCAs Neutral Neutral Neutral
Russell et al (Reference Russell, Berndt and Miceli1999) Administrative databases Sertraline No (paroxetine) Fluoxetine, paroxetine Not applicable Not applicable Neutral
Sclar et al (Reference Sclar, Skaer and Robison1999) Administrative databases Citalopram Yes Amitriptyline, fluoxetine, sertraline, paroxetine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Croghan et al (Reference Croghan, Kniesner and Melfi2000) A Administrative databases Fluoxetine No (sertraline) TCAs, sertraline Not applicable Not applicable Favourable

Table 2 Economic outcome studies of antidepressant therapy sponsored by manufacturers of atypical antidepressants

Study Study design Sponsor's antidepressant Newest antidepressant is sponsor's (newest) Control antidepressant (or class) Study favours SSRI or SSRI class v. TCA1 Study favours newest antidepressant1 Study favours sponsor's antidepressant1
Anton & Revicki (Reference Anton and Revicki1995) A Simulation Nefazodone Yes Imipramine, fluoxetine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Einarson et al (Reference Einarson, Arikian and Sweeney1995) Simulation Venlafaxine (in-patient start) Yes TCAs, SSRIs, HCAs Unfavourable Favourable Favourable
Einarson et al (Reference Einarson, Arikian and Sweeney1995) Simulation Venlafaxine (out-patient start) Yes TCAs, SSRIs, HCAs Favourable Unfavourable Unfavourable
Revicki et al (Reference Revicki, Brown and Palmer1995) A Simulation Nefazodone Yes Imipramine, fluoxetine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Montgomery et al (Reference Montgomery, Brown and Clark1996) Simulation Nefazodone Yes Imipromine Not applicable Favourable Favourable
Revicki et al (Reference Revicki, Brown and Keller1997) Simulation Nefazodone Yes Fluoxetine, imipramine Favourable Favourable Favourable
Einarson et al (Reference Einarson, Addis and Iskedjian1997) Simulation Venlafaxine (in-patient start) Yes TCAs, SSRIs Unfavourable Favourable Favourable
Einarson et al (Reference Einarson, Addis and Iskedjian1997) Simulation Venlafaxine (out-patient start) Yes TCAs, SSRIs Favourable Favourable Favourable
Brown et al (Reference Brown, Nimmerrichter and Guest1999a ) A Simulation Mirtazapine Yes Amitriptyline, fluoxetine Not applicable Favourable Favourable
Brown et al (Reference Brown, van Loon and Guest1999b ) A Simulation Mirtazapine Yes Amitriptyline Not applicable Favourable Favourable
Borghi & Guest (Reference Borghi and Guest2000) Simulation Mirtazapine Yes TCAs, fluoxetine Not applicable Favourable Favourable
Doyle et al (Reference Doyle, Casciano and Arikian2001) Casciano et al (Reference Casciano, Doyle and Arikian2001) Simulation Venlafaxine (in-patient start) Yes SSRIs, TCAs Favourable Favourable Favourable
Doyle et al (Reference Doyle, Casciano and Arikian2001) Casciano et al (Reference Casciano, Doyle and Arikian2001) Simulation Venlafaxine (out-patient start) Yes SSTIs, TCAs Favourable Favourable Favourable
Griffiths et al (Reference Griffiths, Sullivan and Frank1999) Administrative databases Venlafaxine Yes TCA Not applicable Neutral Neutral
Sullivan et al (Reference Sullivan, Griffiths and Frank2000) Administrative databases Venlafaxine No (nefazodone) TCAs, SSRIs, nefazodone, bupropion, trazodone Favourable Not applicable Neutral
Poret et al (Reference Poret, Neslusan and Ricci2001) A Administrative databases Bupropion SR Yes Fluoxetine, paroxetine, sertraline, TCAs, venlafaxine Not applicable Neutral Neutral
Wan et al (Reference Wan, Crown and Berndt2002) A Administrative databases Venlafaxine Yes SSRIs Not applicable Neutral Neutral

Table 3 Non-industry-sponsored economic outcome studies of antidepressant therapy

Study Study design Newest antidepressant or class Control antidepressant Study favours SSRI or SSRI class v. TCA1 Study favours newest antidepressant1
McFarland (Reference McFarland1994) Simulation Paroxetine Imipramine Neutral Neutral
Le Pen et al (Reference Le Pen, Levy and Ravily1994) Simulation Fluoxetine TCAs Favourable Favourable
Stewart (Reference Stewart1994) Simulation Paroxetine Amitriptyline, imipramine, sertraline Unfavourable Unfavourable
Woods & Rizzo (Reference Woods and Rizzo1997) Simulation Paroxetine Imipramine Unfavourable Unfavourable
Canadian Coordinating Office for Health Technology Assessment (1998) Simulation SSRIs TCAs alone TCAs/switch Unfavourable Unfavourable
Simon & Fishman (Reference Simon and Fishman1998) Administrative databases Fluoxetine Imipramine, desipramine Neutral Neutral
Nurnberg et al (Reference Nurnberg, Hensley and Thompson1999) Administrative Paroxetine Fluoxetine, sertraline Not applicable Unfavourable

Association of sponsorship

SSRI v. TCA analysis

For the primary analysis of industry v. non-industry sponsorship of SSRI v. TCA studies, six of seven non-industry-sponsored studies were eligible for analysis (see Table 3). Seventeen industry studies were eligible (see Tables 1 and 2).

Distribution and results for Fisher's exact test are noted in Table 4. The association between industry sponsorship and outcome favouring SSRIs v. TCAs was statistically significant. Each of the two secondary analyses contrasting studies with industry-employed authors v. non-industry-sponsored studies and contrasting studies with industry funding alone v. non-industry-sponsored studies demonstrated a statistically significant association between industry sponsorship and outcome favouring SSRIs v. TCAs, with probability values of 0.0420 and 0.0163 respectively.

Table 4 Fisher's exact test: distributions and probabilities

Outcome
Favourable Neutral Unfavourable
Sponsorship v. outcome favouring SSRIs over TCAs: industry v. non-industry studies 1
Industry sponsor 13 4 0
Non-industry sponsor 1 2 3
Sponsorship v. outcome favouring newest antidepressant: industry v. non-industry studies 2
Industry sponsor 25 7 1
Non-industry sponsor 1 2 4
Study design v. outcome favouring newest antidepressant: within-industry studies 3
Simulation design 18 0 1
Administrative data design 7 7 0
Sponsorship v. outcome favouring newest antidepressant: industry v. non-industry modelling studies 4
Industry sponsor 18 0 1
Non-industry sponsor 1 1 3

New v. old antidepressant analysis

All non-industry-sponsored studies were eligible (see Table 3). Thirty-three industry-sponsored studies (see Tables 1 and 2) were eligible. Distribution and results of Fisher's exact test are noted in Table 4. The association between industry sponsorship and outcome favouring the newest antidepressant was statistically significant. Each of the two secondary analyses contrasting studies with industry-employed authors v. non-industry-sponsored studies and contrasting studies with industry funding alone v. non-industry-sponsored studies demonstrated a statistically significant association between industry sponsorship and outcome favouring the newest antidepressant, with probability values of 0.0047 and 0.0018 respectively.

Association between study design and sponsorship bias

Question 1

Within industry-sponsored studies, is there a difference in tendency to favour the sponsor's drug over a competitor's drug or drug class, based on type of study design? For the principal analysis, ‘favouring the sponsor's drug or drug class’ was defined based on favouring the newest drug among all manufacturer-sponsored studies. Thirty-three industry-sponsored studies were eligible (see Tables 1 and 2). Distribution and results of the Fisher's exact test are noted in Table 4. The association between modelling v. administrative study design and outcome favouring the newest drug was statistically significant.

For our alternative analysis based on whether the sponsor's drug or drug class won, regardless of whether it was newest, all 38 industry-sponsored studies were eligible (see Tables 1 and 2). This analysis yielded a probability value of 0.0011, consistent with the results in the primary analysis.

Question 2

Between industry-sponsored and non-industry-sponsored modelling design studies, is there a difference in outcome patterns? For the principal analysis of this question we examined the patterns of favouring the newest drug. Nineteen industry studies (see Tables 1 and 2) and five non-industry studies (see Table 3) were eligible. The distribution and results of the Fisher's exact test are noted in Table 4. The association between industry v. non-industry sponsorship of modelling studies and outcome favouring the newest drug was statistically significant. Each of the two secondary analyses contrasting studies with industry-employed authors v. non-industry-sponsored studies and contrasting studies with industry funding alone v. non-industry-sponsored studies demonstrated a statistically significant association between industry sponsorship and outcome favouring the newest antidepressant in modelling studies, with probability values of 0.0010 and 0.0100 respectively.

In an alternative analysis we examined the patterns of favouring SSRIs v. favouring TCAs in modelling studies. We performed this analysis with the five eligible non-industry-sponsored studies (see Table 3) contrasted first with all twelve eligible industry-sponsored modelling studies (see Tables 1 and 2) that included SSRI v. TCA comparisons, and then with the six eligible modelling studies sponsored by SSRI manufacturers (see Table 1) that included SSRI v. TCA comparisons. The results of the Fisher's exact test in the two cases were 0.0139 and 0.0151 respectively, indicating that the tendency for industry-sponsored simulations to favour SSRIs more often than non-industry-sponsored studies is unlikely to be due to chance.

The sensitivity analysis varying the marginal cost-effectiveness threshold from $20 000 to $100 000 per QALY did not change any of the results reported above.

DISCUSSION

Our analyses show that, regardless of how the question was operationalised, for each of our study questions there was greater than chance association between study sponsorship and outcome. Among industry-sponsored v. non-industry-sponsored studies, industry-sponsored studies more frequently reported results favourable to the industry sponsor than did non-industry-sponsored studies. This was true whether industry sponsorship was defined as industry authorship, industry financial support alone, or both. Among industry studies, modelling studies were more likely to report results favourable to the sponsor than administrative data studies. Between industry-sponsored and non-industry-sponsored modelling design studies, industry studies were more likely to report results favourable to industry.

Consistency with prior studies

Our overall finding of sponsorship bias is consistent with prior studies in that all three prior fully reported studies found some association between study sponsorship and outcomes (Reference Azimi and WelchAzimi & Welch, 1998; Reference Friedberg, Saffran and StinsonFriedberg et al, 1999; Reference Neumann, Sandberg and BellNeumann et al, 2000a ). However, any detailed comparison between our study and previous studies is necessarily limited given that the previous studies mixed drugs, devices and other health interventions, and mixed various classes of medicines (Reference Azimi and WelchAzimi & Welch, 1998; Reference Neumann, Sandberg and BellNeumann et al, 2000a ); focused on qualitative conclusions (Reference Friedberg, Saffran and StinsonFriedberg et al, 1999); and used various definitions to select the specific study outcomes to be analysed (Reference Azimi and WelchAzimi & Welch, 1998; Reference Friedberg, Saffran and StinsonFriedberg et al, 1999; Reference Neumann, Sandberg and BellNeumann et al, 2000a ). The study by Friedberg et al (Reference Friedberg, Saffran and Stinson1999) of oncology drugs is perhaps most comparable with our current study, given their focus on a single pharmaceutical class and their categorisation of study conclusions as favourable, neutral or unfavourable, although they focused on qualitative rather than quantitative conclusions. Like our study, that of Friedberg et al did find an association between study conclusion and funding source.

Support for concern about modelling studies

In addition to supporting the general concern about sponsorship bias in pharmacoeconomic studies, our findings support the more specific concerns that have been raised about the potential for bias in modelling studies (Reference LuceLuce, 1995; Reference O'BrienO'Brien, 1996; Reference SheldonSheldon, 1996; Reference Maynard and CooksonMaynard & Cookson, 1998; Reference McCabe and DixonMcCabe & Dixon, 2000). Such support stems from the combination of our two findings regarding study design: among industry studies, modelling studies are more favourable to the sponsor than administrative studies, and in a comparison of industry-sponsored and non-industry-sponsored modelling studies, studies sponsored by industry are significantly more favourable to industry.

Limitations of our study

Our study has clear limitations. Randomised pharmacoeconomic trials could not be compared on the basis of sponsorship because there were only two such trials in this area. Relatively few non-industry-sponsored studies were available. We examined only one class of medications; analyses of other classes of medications should be conducted.

Bias v. accuracy

Although we have demonstrated several associations between study sponsorship and outcome, these associations do not suggest which (if either) side presents a more accurate estimate of relative pharmacoeconomic outcome. Both industry-supported and non-industry-supported researchers may be subject to forces that could potentially bias their work (Reference Yee and HillmanYee & Hillman, 1997; Reference DrummondDrummond, 1998; Reference Rennie and LuftRennie & Luft, 2000). Additionally, journal editorial processes can result in a biased sample of studies being published. It has been observed that journals tend to publish studies with ‘positive’ rather than ‘negative’ results (Reference Freemantle and MasonFreemantle & Mason, 1997).

Causes of bias

Many ideas have been offered to explain how sponsorship could result in biased reported outcomes (Reference Udrarhelyi, Graham and RaiUdrarhelyi et al, 1992; Reference Freemantle and MasonFreemantle & Mason, 1997; Reference DrummondDrummond, 1998; Reference CookCook, 1999; Reference Neumann, Sandberg and BellNeumann et al, 2000a ; Reference Rennie and LuftRennie & Luft, 2000). Industry, motivated to enhance sales of its products, might only pursue studies on products and select comparators that would yield favourable results. They might select biased populations within administrative data-sets, overtly or subtly influence analytical methods or models, or veto submission for publication of studies yielding unfavourable results. Non-industry-sponsored researchers might bias the studies submitted for publication in similar ways, although perhaps from different motivations such as controlling formulary costs, personal or academic rivalries, or career promotion.

We are unable to pinpoint the causes of bias among the reports analysed here. Examination of the individual studies does not reveal a common element that differ differentiates industry-sponsored from non-industry-sponsored studies; rather, the methodological limitations in the studies vary widely. These limitations have been discussed extensively elsewhere (Reference Hotopf, Lewis and NormandHotopf et al, 1996; Woods & Baker, Reference Woods and Baker1997, Reference Woods, Baker, Davis, Coyle and Charney2002). However, at least two suggested causes seem unlikely. First, some commentators have noted the potential role of selection bias - i.e. the tendency of researchers not to submit and of journals not to publish small studies or studies with negative statistical outcomes (Reference Freemantle and MasonFreemantle & Mason, 1997; Reference NeumannNeumann, 1998). This would help to explain how an overall preponderance of statistically positive studies could exist even if there were true uncertainty about alternative medications (Reference Djulbegovic, Lacevic and CantorDjulbegovic et al, 2000). The difference we have shown between industry-sponsored and non-industry-sponsored studies suggests that submission or editorial selection bias based on statistical significance alone does not adequately explain the bias in the present case. Second, it has been suggested that a particular sponsor weeds out weak alternatives among its drugs in early preliminary processes; therefore, drugs that reach the stage of being marketed are strong competitors and likely to yield analyses that favour the sponsor's drug (Reference GagnonGagnon, 2000). However, these same strong competitors performed less well in non-industry-sponsored studies, as shown clearly in our analysis of outcomes favouring either SSRIs or TCAs. Moreover, it should be noted that in the 18 studies with head-to-head comparisons among such strong competitors, the sponsor's drug lost only once (Reference Einarson, Arikian and SweeneyEinarson et al, 1995).

Bias in efficacy v. pharmacoeconomic studies

It is not possible to comment about whether the bias revealed in the current study of pharmacoeconomic reports of antidepressants is any greater or less than the sponsorship bias that may exist in efficacy studies of antidepressants. There is no published report on sponsorship bias in efficacy studies in any medication category within psychiatry. The only published report devoted to such quantitative analysis of psychiatric medications is a letter reviewing efficacy studies of any psychiatric medication in one journal over a 1-year period (Reference MandelkernMandelkern, 1999). This author reported a tally for industry-supported studies of 16 favourable to the manufacturer's drug and none unfavourable, and for unsupported studies 10 favourable and 6 unfavourable, concluding that there was a correlation between source of support and efficacy outcome. In other areas of medicine, bias has been demonstrated repeatedly in efficacy studies (Reference DavidsonDavidson, 1986; Reference Rochon, Gurwitz and SimmsRochon et al, 1994; Reference Stelfox, Chua and O'RourkeStelfox et al, 1998; Reference Djulbegovic, Lacevic and CantorDjulbegovic et al, 2000). A study of sponsorship bias in efficacy trials of antidepressants would provide a useful comparison for our study.

It is important for pharmacoeconomic studies to attempt to give estimates that are as accurate and uninfluenced by bias as possible, given the large and growing number of health care dollars spent on medications. Pharmaceutical sales for North America were reported to be US$153 billion in 2000, representing a 14% growth over the previous year (IMS Health, 2001). Owing to the importance of cost constraint in medicine the volume of pharmacoeconomic research has been growing (Reference DetskyDetsky, 1994) and is linked to governmental purchasing decisions in some jurisdictions (Canadian Coordinating Office for Health Technology Assessment, 1994; Ontario Ministry of Health, 1994; Australian Government, 1995). However as we noted previously, financial and other incentives create strong motives for bias. Our results for antidepressants suggest that actual bias related to sponsorship appears to exist, although whether or how the bias and specific motives are related cannot be determined. Until the mechanisms producing the bias are better understood, interpretation of results from pharmacoeconomic studies should take sponsorship into account.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Evaluation of pharmacoeconomic studies of antidepressants for treatment of major depression should take into account the source of study funding and of authorship.

  2. Evaluation of modelling studies in this area may warrant additional scrutiny.

  3. Evaluation of pharmacoeconomic studies should be informed by a thorough understanding of their methodology.

LIMITATIONS

  1. Randomised pharmacoeconomic trials could not be compared because of their scarcity.

  2. Few non-industry-sponsored trials were available.

  3. Only one class of medication was examined.

Acknowledgements

Consultancies, research grants or unrestricted grants were received from. Pfizer, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly & Co. and Forest Laboratories. The. work was supported in part by PHS KO8 MH-01718 (C.B.B.), NARSAD R03051 (C.B.B.), Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (M.L.C.), Substance Abuse and Mental. Health Services Administration (M.L.C.), Meadows Foundation (M.L.C.), Houston. Endowment (M.L.C.), Hogg Foundation (M.L.C.), Texas Department of Mental. Health and Mental Retardation (M.L.C.) and PHS MH-54446 (S.W.W.).

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

Range of industry and non-industry funding received, detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Anton, S. F. & Revicki, D. A. (1995) The use of decision analysis in the pharmacoeconomic evaluation of an antidepressant: a cost-effectiveness study of nefazodone. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 31, 249258.Google Scholar
Australian Government (1995) Guidelines for the Pharmaceutical Industry on Preparation of Submissions to the Pharmaceutical Benefits Advisory Committee Including Major Submissions Involving Economic Analysis. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.Google Scholar
Azimi, N. A. & Welch, G. H. (1998) The effectiveness of cost-effectiveness analysis in containing costs. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 13, 664669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentkover, J. D. & Feighner, J. P. (1996) Cost analysis of paroxetine vs imipramine in major depression. Pharmacoeconomics, 8, 223232.Google Scholar
Borghi, J. & Guest, J. F. (2000) Economic impact of using mirtazapine compared to amitriptyline and fluoxetine in the treatment of moderate and severe depression in the UK. European Psychiatry, 15, 378387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyer, P., Danion, J. M., Bisserbe, J. C., et al (1998) Clinical and economic comparison of sertraline and fluoxetine in the treatment of depression. Pharmacoeconomics, 13, 157169.Google Scholar
Brown, M. C., Nimmerrichter, A. A. & Guest, J. F. (1999a) Cost-effectiveness of mirtazapine compared to amitriptyline and fluoxetine in the treatment of moderate and severe depression in Austria. European Psychiatry: Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 14, 230244.Google Scholar
Brown, M. C., van Loon, J. M. T. & Guest, J. F. (1999b) Cost-effectiveness of mirtazapine relative to amitriptyline in the treatment of moderate and severe depression in France. European Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 197208.Google Scholar
Canadian Coordinating Office for Health Technology Assessment (1994) Guidelines for Economic Evaluation of Pharmaceuticals. Ottawa: CCOHTA.Google Scholar
Canadian Coordinating Office for Health Technology Assessment (1998) Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) for major depression. Part II: The cost effectiveness of SSRIs in treatment of depression. August 1997. Report 4E. Evidence-Based Medicine, 3, 87.Google Scholar
Casciano, J., Doyle, J., Arikian, S., et al (2001) The health economic impact of antidepressant usage from a payer's perspective: a multinational study. International Journal of Clinical Practice, 55, 292299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, J. R. (1999) Challenges in economic outcomes research. Hepatology, 29, 23S25S.Google Scholar
Croghan, T. W., Lair, T. J., Engelhart, L., et al (1997) Effect of antidepressant therapy on health care utilization and costs in primary care. Psychiatric Services, 48, 14201426.Google Scholar
Croghan, T. W., Kniesner, T. J., Melfi, C. A., et al (2000) Effect of antidepressant choice on the incidence and economic intensity of hospitalization among depressed individuals. Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 27, 183195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crown, W. H., Hylan, T. R. & Meneades, L. (1998) Antidepressant selection and use and healthcare expenditures – an empirical approach. Pharmacoeconomics, 13, 435448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. A. (1986) Source of funding and outcome of clinical trials. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 1, 155158.Google Scholar
Detsky, A. S. (1994) Using cost-effectiveness analysis for formulary decision making: from theory into practice. Pharmacoeconomics, 6, 281288.Google Scholar
Djulbegovic, B., Lacevic, M., Cantor, A., et al (2000) The uncertainty principle and industry-sponsored research. Lancet, 356, 635638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyle, J. J., Casciano, J., Arikian, S., et al (2001) A multinational pharmacoeconomic evaluation of acute major depressive disorder (MDD); a comparison of cost-effectiveness between venlafaxine, SSRIs, and TCAs. Value in Health, 4, 1631.Google Scholar
Drummond, M. F. (1998) A reappraisal of economic evaluation of pharmaceuticals: science or marketing? Pharmacoeconomics, 14, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einarson, T. R., Arikian, S., Sweeney, S., et al (1995) A model to evaluate the cost-effectiveness of oral therapies in the management of patients with major depressive disorders. Clinical Therapeutics, 17, 136153.Google Scholar
Einarson, T. R., Addis, A. & Iskedjian, M. (1997) Pharmacoeconomic analysis of venlafaxine in the treatment of major depressive disorder. Pharmacoeconomics, 12, 286296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forder, J., Kavanagh, S. & Fenyo, A. (1996) A comparison of the cost-effectiveness of sertraline versus tricyclic antidepressants in primary care. Journal of Affective Disorders, 38, 97111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freemantle, N. & Mason, J. (1997) Publication bias in clinical trials and economic analyses Pharmacoeconomics, 12, 1016.Google Scholar
Friedberg, M., Saffran, B., Stinson, T. J., et al (1999) Evaluation of conflict of interest in economic analyses of new drugs used in oncology. JAMA, 282, 14531475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gagnon, J. P. (2000) Sources of bias in the economic analysis of new drugs (letter). JAMA, 283, 1423.Google Scholar
Griffiths, R. I., Sullivan, E. M., Frank, R. G., et al (1999) Medical resource use and cost of venlafaxine or tricyclic antidepressant therapy. Pharmacoeconomics, 15, 495505.Google Scholar
Gulati, S. C. & Bitran, J. D. (1995) Cost-effectiveness analysis: sleeping with an enemy or a friend? Journal of Clinical Oncology, 13, 21522154.Google Scholar
Hatziandreu, E. J., Brown, R. E., Revicki, D. A., et al (1994) Cost utility of maintenance treatment of recurrent depression with sertraline versus episodic treatment with dothiepin. Pharmacoeconomics, 5, 249264.Google Scholar
Hill, S. R., Mitchell, A. S. & Henry, D. A. (2000) Problems with the interpretation of pharmacoeconomic analyses: a review of submissions to the Australian Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme. JAMA, 283, 21162121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillman, A. L., Eisenberg, J. M., Pauly, M. V., et al (1991) Avoiding bias in the conduct and reporting of cost-effectiveness research sponsored by pharmaceutical companies New England Journal of Medicine, 324, 13621365 Google Scholar
Hillman, B. J., Joseph, C. A., Magel, M. R., et al (1990) Frequency and costs of diagnostic imaging in office practice: a comparison of self referring and radiologist referring physicians New England Journal of Medicine, 323, 15041508 Google Scholar
Hotopf, M., Lewis, G. & Normand, C. (1996) Are SSRIs a cost-effective alternative to tricyclics? British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 404409 Google Scholar
Hylan, T. R., Kotsanos, J. G., Andersen, J. S., et al (1996) Comparison of a decision analytic model with results from a naturalistic economic clinical trial: an application of evaluating alternative antidepressants American Journal of Managed Care, 2, 12111223 Google Scholar
Hylan, T. R., Crown, W. H., Meneades, L., et al (1998) Tricyclic antidepressant and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors antidepressant selection and health care costs in the naturalistic setting: a multivariate analysis Journal of Affective Disorders, 47, 7179 Google Scholar
IMS Health (2001) The global pharmaceutical market in 2000. www.ims-global.com/insight/news_story/0103/news=story=010314.htm.Google Scholar
Jones, M. T. & Cockrum, P. C. (2000) A critical review of published economic modelling studies in depression. Pharmacoeconomics, 17, 555583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonsson, B. & Bebbington, P. (1993) Economic studies of the treatment of depressive illness. In Health Economics of Depression (eds Jonsson, B. & Rosenbaum, J.), pp. 3548. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Jonsson, B. & Bebbington, P. (1994) What price depression? The cost of depression and the cost-effectiveness of pharmacological treatment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 665673.Google Scholar
Lapierre, Y., Bentkover, J., Schainbaum, S., et al (1995) Direct costs of depression: analysis of treatment costs of paroxetine versus imipramine in Canada. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 370377.Google Scholar
Laupacis, A., Feeny, D., Detsky, A. S., et al (1992) How attractive does a new technology have to be to warrant adoption and utilization? Canadian Medical Association Journal, 146, 473481.Google Scholar
Le Pen, C., Levy, E., Ravily, V., et al (1994) The cost of treatment dropout in depression: a cost-benefit analysis of fluoxetine vs. tricyclics. Journal of Affective Disorders, 31, 118.Google Scholar
Luce, B. R. (1995) Policy implications of modelling the cost-effectiveness of health care technologies. Drug Information Journal, 29, 14691475.Google Scholar
Mandelkern, M. (1999) Manufacturer support and outcome (letter). Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard, A. & Cookson, R. F. (1998) Computer modelling: the need for careful evaluation and public audit. Pharmacoeconomics, 14, 6772.Google Scholar
McCabe, C. & Dixon, S. (2000) Testing the validity of cost-effectiveness models. Pharmacoeconomics, 17, 501513.Google Scholar
McFarland, B. H. (1994) Cost-effectiveness considerations for managed care systems: treating depression in primary care. American Journal of Medicine, 97(suppl. 6A), 47S58S.Google Scholar
Melton, S. T., Kirkwood, C. K., Farrar, T. W., et al (1997) Economic evaluation of paroxetine and imipramine in depressed outpatients. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 33, 93100.Google Scholar
Montgomery, S. A., Brown, R. E. & Clark, M. (1996) Economic analysis of treating depression with nefazodone v. imipramine. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 768771.Google Scholar
Neumann, P. J. (1998) Paying the piper for pharmacoeconomic studies. Medical Decision Making, 18, S2326.Google Scholar
Neumann, P. J., Sandberg, E. A., Bell, C., et al (2000a) Are pharmaceuticalls cost-effective? A review of the evidence. Health Affairs, 19, 92109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, P. J., Stone, P. W., Chapman, R. H., et al (2000b) The quality of reporting in published cost–utility analyses, 1976–1997. Annals of Internal Medicine, 132, 964972.Google Scholar
Nuijten, M. J. C., Hardens, M. & Soutre, E. (1995) A Markov process analysis comparing the cost effectiveness of maintenance therapy with citalopram versus standard therapy in major depression. Pharmacoeconomics, 8, 159168.Google Scholar
Nurnberg, G. H., Hensley, P. L., Thompson, P. M., et al (1999) Modeling the pharmacoeconomic cost of three selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Psychiatric Services, 50, 13511353.Google Scholar
Obenchain, R. L., Melfi, C. A., Croghan, T. W., et al (1997) Bootstrap analyses of cost effectiveness in antidepressant pharmacotherapy. Pharmacoeconomics, 11, 464472.Google Scholar
O'Brien, B. (1996) Economic evaluation of pharmaceuticals: Frankenstein's monster or vampire of trials? Medical Care, 34, DS99–108.Google Scholar
Ontario Ministry of Health (1994) Ontario Guidelines for Economic Analysis of Pharmaceutical Products. Toronto: Ontario Ministry of Health, Drug Programs Branch.Google Scholar
Poret, A. W., Neslusan, C., Ricci, J., et al (2001) Retrospective analysis of the health-care costs of bupropion sustained release in comparison with other antidepressants. Value in Heallth, 4, 362369.Google Scholar
Rennie, D. & Flanagin, A. (1992) Publication bias: the triumph of hope over experience. JAMA, 267, 411412.Google Scholar
Rennie, D. & Luft, H. S. (2000) Pharmacoeconomic analyses: making them transparent, making them credible. JAMA, 283, 21582160.Google Scholar
Revicki, D. A., Brown, R. E., Palmer, W., et al (1995) Modelling the cost effectiveness of antidepressant treatment in primary care. Pharmacoeconomics, 8, 524540.Google Scholar
Revicki, D. A., Brown, R. E., Keller, M. B., et al (1997) Cost-effectiveness of newer antidepressants compared with tricyclic antidepressants in managed care settings. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 58, 4758.Google Scholar
Rochon, P. A., Gurwitz, J. H., Simms, R. W., et al (1994) A study of manufacturer-supported trials of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs in the treatment of arthritis. Archives of Internal Medicine, 154, 157163.Google Scholar
Russell, J. M., Berndt, E. R., Miceli, R., et al (1999) Course and cost of treatment for depression with fluoxetine, paroxetine, and sertraline. American Journal of Managed Care, 5, 597606.Google Scholar
Sacristan, J. A., Bolanos, E., Soto, J., et al (1997) Publication bias in health economic studies (letter). Pharmacoeconomics, 11, 289290.Google Scholar
Sclar, D. A., Robison, L. M., Skaer, T. L., et al (1994) Antidepressant pharmacotherapy: economic outcomes in a health maintenance organization. Clinical Therapeutics, 16, 715730.Google Scholar
Sclar, D. A., Robison, L. M., Skaer, T. L., et al (1995) Antidepressant pharmacotherapy: economic evaluation of fluoxetine, paroxetine, and sertraline in a health maintenance organization. Journal of International Medical Research, 23, 395412.Google Scholar
Sclar, D. A., Skaer, T. L., Robison, L. M., et al (1998) Economic outcomes with antidepressant pharmacotherapy: a retrospective intent-to-treat analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 1317.Google Scholar
Sclar, D. A., Skaer, T. L., Robison, L. M., et al (1999) Economic appraisal of citalopram in the management of single-episode depression. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19, 47S54S.Google Scholar
Sheldon, T. A. (1996) Problems of using modelling in the economic evaluation of health care. Health Economics, 5, III.3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, J. E., Weinstein, M. C., Russell, L. B., et al (1996) Recommendations for reporting cost-effectiveness analyses. JAMA, 276, 13391341.Google Scholar
Simon, G. E. & Fishman, P. (1998) Cost implications of initial antidepressant selection in primary care. Pharmacoeconomics, 13, 6170.Google Scholar
Simon, G. E., Heilegenstein, J. H., Revicki, D. A., et al (1999) Long-term outcomes of initial antidepressant drug choice in a ‘real world’ randomized trial. Archives of Family Medicine, 8, 319325.Google Scholar
Skaer, T. L., Sclar, D. A., Robison, L. M., et al (1995) Economic evaluation of amitriptyline, desipramine, nortriptyline, and sertraline in the management of patients with depression. Current Therapeutic Research, 56, 556567.Google Scholar
Smith, W. & Sherrill, A. (1996) A pharmacoeconomic study of the management of major depression: patients in a TennCare HMO. Medical Interface, 9, 8892.Google Scholar
Stelfox, H. T., Chua, G., O'Rourke, K., et al (1998) Conflict of interest in the debate over calcium-channel antagonists. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 101106.Google Scholar
Stewart, A. (1994) Anti-depressant pharmacotherapy: cost comparison of SSRIs and TCAs. British Journal of Medical Economics, 7, 6779.Google Scholar
Sullivan, E. M., Griffiths, R. I., Frank, R. G., et al (2000) One-year costs of second-line therapies for depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61, 290298.Google Scholar
Thompson, D., Hylan, T. R., McMullen, W., et al (1998) Predictors of a medical-offset effect among patients receiving antidepressant therapy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 824827.Google Scholar
Udrarhelyi, I. S., Graham, A., Rai, A., et al (1992) Cost-effectiveness and cost-benefit analyses in the medical literature: are the methods being used correctly? Annals of Internal Medicine, 116, 238244.Google Scholar
Wan, G. J., Crown, W. H., Berndt, E. R., et al (2002) Treatment costs of venlafaxine and selective serotoninreuptake inhibitors for depression and anxiety. Managed Care Interface, 15, 2430.Google Scholar
Woods, S. W. & Baker, C. B. (1997) Cost-effectiveness of newer antidepressants. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 10, 95101.Google Scholar
Woods, S. W. & Baker, C. B. (2002) Cost effectiveness of the newer generation of antidepressants. In Psychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress (eds Davis, K. L., Coyle, J., Charney, D. S., et al), pp. 11191138. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Woods, S. W. & Rizzo, J. A. (1997) Cost-effectiveness of antidepressant treatment reassessed. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 257263.Google Scholar
Yee, G. C. & Hillman, A. L. (1997) Applied pharmacoeconomics: when can publication be legitimately withheld? Pharmacoeconomics, 12, 511516.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Economic outcome studies of antidepressant therapy sponsored by manufacturers of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors

Figure 1

Table 2 Economic outcome studies of antidepressant therapy sponsored by manufacturers of atypical antidepressants

Figure 2

Table 3 Non-industry-sponsored economic outcome studies of antidepressant therapy

Figure 3

Table 4 Fisher's exact test: distributions and probabilities

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.