Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:48:57.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychiatry does need more randomised controlled trials

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 December 2018

Tom Burns*
Affiliation:
Professor of Social Psychiatry Emeritus, Oxford University, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2018 

In their editorial, Duncan et al claim that ‘Conventional approaches to evidence that prioritise randomised controlled trials appear increasingly inadequate for the evaluation of complex mental health interventions’.Reference Duncan, Weich, Fenton, Twigg, Moon and Madan1 Nothing could be further from the truth. The exaggerated distinctions presented between research in psychiatry and that in the rest of medicine are in a long tradition of special pleading that does our discipline no favours.

Randomised controlled trials (RCTs) seek to identify what works for whom – careful identification of the target population and appropriate outcome measures are key to all successful trials. Their findings do, indeed, ‘apply to groups …not equally to everyone’ – clinicians are still needed to interpret and apply their findings. RCTs do not seek to substitute for the exploration of mechanisms, nor the creative development of alternative approaches to treatment. Their purpose is to reduce persisting doubts about the effectiveness or otherwise of an intervention. If there are no doubts they are not needed. But where there is doubt about treatment effects (highly likely in the long-term relapsing–remitting disorders in psychiatry with their probabilistic outcomes over extended periods) their superiority has proved itself time and time again. One simply needs to observe the staggering improvements in evidence-based medicine over the past 50 years.

The authors’ implication that in general medicine trials are so much simpler, interventions less complex, or treatments less ‘personalised’ would receive a dusty response from our colleagues in oncology or cardiology. Where interventions are complex they need to be carefully dissected to determine what is potentially effective and what is potentially redundant. Such hard-nosed examination is sorely needed in psychiatry and it can be highly productive in its own right, even without RCTs to test core components.

Psychiatry is not handicapped by the dominance of ‘positivistic’ research favouring RCTs and systematic reviews. On the contrary it is handicapped by there not being anywhere near enough of them, and not enough weight being given to their results. In their contrast between ‘realist’ and ‘positivist’ research the authors omit to acknowledge what Karl Popper considered scientific method's cardinal virtue – its ability to falsify hypotheses.Reference Popper2

Rigorously designed and conducted RCTs have an almost unique power to reverse strongly held clinical convictions. It was Acker et al’s 1957 RCT that ended insulin coma's two decades of dominance in schizophrenia treatment.Reference Ackner, Harris and Oldham3 Twice I have been forced, painfully, to abandon cherished beliefs when confronted by RCT evidence. Assertive community treatment teams did not, despite my enthusiasm and commitment to it, deliver superior care to community mental health teams,Reference Burns, Creed, Fahy, Thompson, Tyrer and White4, Reference Burns, Catty, Dash, Roberts, Lockwood and Marshall5 nor do community treatment orders stabilise severe psychosis in the community.Reference Burns, Rugkåsa, Molodynski, Dawson, Yeeles and Vazquez-Montes6, Reference Kisely and Hall7 Would the proposed realist studies have anything like the power of RCTs to achieve this?

Our current demand is for parity of esteem. We are more likely to get equal respect and funding if our practice matches that of our medical colleagues. Holding psychiatry's practice to the same rigorous standards in research will go a long way to establishing society's trust and, through that, genuine parity of esteem for our profession and patients.

References

1Duncan, C, Weich, S, Fenton, SJ, Twigg, L, Moon, G, Madan, J, et al. A realist approach to the evaluation of complex mental health interventions. Br J Psychiatry 2018; 213: 451–3.Google Scholar
2Popper, K. The Logic of Scientific Discovery. Routledge, 1959.Google Scholar
3Ackner, B, Harris, A, Oldham, A. Insulin treatment of schizophrenia; a controlled study. Lancet 1957; 272: 607.Google Scholar
4Burns, T, Creed, F, Fahy, T, Thompson, S, Tyrer, P, White, I. Intensive versus standard case management for severe psychotic illness: a randomised trial. Lancet 1999; 353: 2185–9.Google Scholar
5Burns, T, Catty, J, Dash, M, Roberts, C, Lockwood, A, Marshall, M. Use of intensive case management to reduce time in hospital in people with severe mental illness: systematic review and meta-regression. BMJ 2007; 335: 336.Google Scholar
6Burns, T, Rugkåsa, J, Molodynski, A, Dawson, J, Yeeles, K, Vazquez-Montes, M, et al. Community treatment orders for patients with psychosis (OCTET): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2013; 381: 1627–33.Google Scholar
7Kisely, S, Hall, K. An updated meta-analysis of randomized controlled evidence for the effectiveness of community treatment orders. Can J Psychiatry 2014; 59: 561–4.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.