Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:33:01.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prevalence and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms in the British population

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Louise C. Johns*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London
Mary Cannon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London
Nicola Singleton
Affiliation:
Drugs & Alcohol Research Programme, Research Development & Statistics Directorate, Home Office, London
Robin M. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London
Michael Farrell
Affiliation:
National Addiction Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, London
Traolach Brugha
Affiliation:
University of Leicester, Section of Social and Epidemiological Psychiatry, Department of Health Sciences, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester
Paul Bebbington
Affiliation:
Royal Free and University College Medical School, Department of Mental Health Sciences, London
Rachel Jenkins
Affiliation:
WHO Collaborating Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, London
Howard Meltzer
Affiliation:
Office for National Statistics, London
*
Dr L. C. Johns, PO 67 Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The psychosis phenotype is generally thought of as a categorical entity. However, there is increasing evidence that psychosis exists in the population as a continuum of severity rather than an all-or-none phenomenon.

Aims

To investigate the prevalence and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms using data from the 2000 British National Survey of Psychiatric Morbidity.

Method

A total of 8580 respondents aged 16–74 years were interviewed. Questions covered mental health, physical health, substance use, life events and socio-demographic variables. The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ) was used to identify psychotic symptoms.

Results

Of the respondents, 5.5% endorsed one or more items on the PSQ. Factors independently associated with psychotic symptoms were cannabis dependence, alcohol dependence, victimisation, recent stressful life events, lower intellectual ability and neurotic symptoms. Male gender was associated with paranoid thoughts, whereas female gender predicted hallucinatory experiences.

Conclusions

Self-reported psychotic symptoms are less common in this study than reported elsewhere, because of the measure used. These symptoms have demographic and clinical correlates similar to clinical psychosis.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2004 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

In recent years there have been suggestions that psychosis exists in the general population as a continuous phenotype rather than as an all-or-none phenomenon (Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000). The existence of a psychosis continuum has been found in several large-scale community surveys. In the US National Comorbidity Survey, 28% of individuals endorsed psychosis-screening questions although the rate of clinician-defined psychosis was only 0.7% (Reference Kendler, Gallagher and AbelsonKendler et al, 1996). In the Dunedin birth cohort, 25% of the sample at age 26 years reported at least one delusional or hallucinatory experience that was unrelated to drug use or physical illness, but only 3.7% actually fulfilled criteria for schizophreniform disorder (Reference Poulton, Caspi and MoffittPoulton et al, 2000). Data on psychotic symptoms were collected as part of the Dutch NEMESIS study (Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000). This representative general population sample of 7076 men and women was interviewed using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI), which contains 17 core positive psychosis items; 17.5% of the sample scored at least one on any type of positive psychosis rating but only 2.1% received a DSM–III–R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) diagnosis of non-affective psychosis. Together, these findings suggest that only a small part of the total phenotypic continuum of psychosis is represented by clinically verified and defined cases. Psychotic symptoms in the general population are associated with certain risk factors (e.g. urban residence or younger age group; Reference Verdoux, van Os and Maurice-TisonVerdoux et al, 1998; van Os et al, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2000, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2001). Furthermore, the risk factors for psychotic symptoms mirror those for clinical psychosis, supporting the continuum hypothesis. Individual psychotic symptoms also seem to have specific correlates, suggesting different risk factors for these symptoms. For example, it has been reported that paranoia is associated with experience of victimisation (Reference Janssen, Hanssen and BakJanssen et al, 2003) and that hallucinatory experiences are more common among people of Caribbean origin living in Britain (Reference Johns, Nazroo and BebbingtonJohns et al, 2002). We used data from a large cross-sectional survey of the British population to examine the distribution and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms. We also examined whether there were any specific demographic and clinical correlates of paranoid thoughts and hallucinatory experiences.

METHOD

Sample

The data were obtained from the second National Survey of Psychiatric Morbidity in Great Britain, conducted in 2000 by the Office for National Statistics (ONS). The survey examined the prevalence of mental health problems among adults aged 16–74 years living in private households in Great Britain. It covered mental health, physical health, drug and alcohol use, life events, service use and socio-demographic variables. The sample was drawn from the small-user Postcode Address File using a two-stage approach. Initially, postcode sectors were stratified on the basis of socio-economic status within region and 438 sectors were chosen with a probability proportional to size. Then, within each sector, 36 addresses were randomly selected for inclusion in the survey. One adult aged 16–74 years was interviewed in each household. The survey used a two-phase design. First-phase interviews were carried out by interviewers from the ONS field force. Personal interviews were carried out with 8580 adults (a response rate of 67%). Individuals who scored positively on one or more psychosis criteria were part of a sub-sample who had a follow-up second-phase interview by a clinician. Details of the survey methods can be found in Singleton et al (Reference Singleton, Bumpstead and O'Brien2001).

Assessment of psychotic symptoms

In the initial interview, the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ; Reference Bebbington and NayaniBebbington & Nayani, 1995) was used to assess psychotic symptoms in the past year. The PSQ has five probe questions (plus secondary questions) enquiring about mania, thought insertion, paranoia, strange experiences and hallucinations (see Appendix). Respondents were asked all the items from the PSQ without the usual procedure of cutting off after a section was answered positively.

For the current analyses, we selected individuals who endorsed one or more psychotic symptom (initial probe plus secondary questions) on the PSQ. Because we wanted to examine psychotic experiences in non-clinical subjects, we first excluded those people with definite or probable psychosis (defined below).

Assessment of psychosis

Four criteria from the first-phase interview were considered likely to be indicative of psychosis: a self-reported diagnosis or symptoms suggestive of psychotic disorder; taking anti-psychotic medication; a history of admission to a psychiatric hospital or ward; a positive response to question 5a of the PSQ (auditory hallucinations). Respondents who met one or more of these psychosis screening criteria were selected for a follow-up interview using the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN; World Health Organization, 1992a ). Algorithms then were used to classify respondents into ICD–10 (World Health Organization, 1992b ) psychosis categories. Some people selected for a second-phase interview could not be contacted or refused a second interview; in these cases, an assessment of ‘probable psychosis’ was assigned to those who scored positively on two or more of these psychosis criteria.

Correlates

Based on previously reported associations with psychotic symptoms (Reference Verdoux, van Os and Maurice-TisonVerdoux et al, 1998; van Os et al, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2000, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2001; Reference Johns, Nazroo and BebbingtonJohns et al, 2002; Reference Janssen, Hanssen and BakJanssen et al, 2003), the following variables were selected.

  1. (a) Socio-demographic characteristics: age, gender, ethnic origin, area of residence, educational qualifications and intellectual functioning.

  2. (b) Alcohol dependence, assessed by the Severity of Alcohol Dependence Questionnaire (SAD–Q; Reference Stockwell, Murphy and HogsonStockwell et al, 1983).

  3. (c) Drug use, drug dependence and cannabis dependence. Dependence was assessed with five specific questions, a positive response to any of which was taken to indicate dependence (Reference Singleton, Bumpstead and O'BrienSingleton et al, 2001).

  4. (d) Neurotic symptoms (anxiety/depression), measured using the revised Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS–R; Reference Lewis, Pelosi and ArayaLewis et al, 1992). Individuals with scores above 12 were classified as having neurosis.

  5. (e) Life events. Respondents were asked whether they had experienced any of 11 stressful life events taken from the List of Threatening Experiences (life events with long-term threat; Reference Brugha, Bebbington and TennantBrugha et al, 1985) in the 6 months prior to interview. The life events are: serious illness, injury or assault to you; serious illness, injury or assault to a close relative; death of a close relative; death of a close friend/other relative; separation or divorce; serious problem with a close friend, neighbour or relative; made redundant or sacked; unemployed/seeking work for more than 1 month; major financial crisis; problem with police and court appearance; something valued that is lost or stolen.

  6. (f) Victimisation events. Respondents were asked whether they had ever experienced any of the following: bullying; violence at work; violence in the home; sexual abuse; being expelled from school; running away from home; being homeless.

Analyses

The data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (version 11.0 for Windows). Binary logistic regression analyses were used to ascertain which factors were associated with the presence of psychotic symptoms. Associations were expressed as odds ratios (ORs). Sixty people (0.7%) with probable psychosis were excluded from the analyses, 27 of whom met the criteria for functional psychosis following a SCAN interview.

We examined the factors associated with the presence of any psychotic symptom (endorsement of initial plus secondary questions on one or more items of the PSQ). First, the predictor variables were entered individually to obtain unadjusted odds ratios. Age was collapsed into three age bands (16–34, 35–54, 55–74). Information about ethnic origin was divided into four groups: White, Black, South Asian and Other. Area of residence was divided into urban and rural. Educational qualifications covered three groups: none, GCSE level, A-level or above. Verbal IQ was estimated from respondents’ scores on the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Reference NelsonNelson, 1982). Alcohol dependence was classified as present or absent. Drug use was any drug used in the last month (yes/no); and drug dependence was classified as no dependence, dependent on cannabis only or dependent on other drug. Experience of life events and victimisation events was classified dichotomously (yes/no). Second, all the significant variables were entered together to obtain the relative odds of psychotic symptoms controlling for interrelationships between these variables.

We examined whether specific factors were associated with the presence of either paranoid thoughts or hallucinatory experiences. These items were chosen because previous studies have suggested that they are associated with particular risk factors (Reference Johns, Nazroo and BebbingtonJohns et al, 2002; Reference Janssen, Hanssen and BakJanssen et al, 2003). The response variables selected were ‘Have there been times when you felt that people were deliberately acting to harm you or your interests?’ and ‘Have there been times when you heard or saw things that other people couldn't?’ In order to examine specific risk factors, the analyses for each variable included individuals who had endorsed only that item and no other PSQ items.

RESULTS

Frequency of psychotic symptoms

After excluding people with probable psychosis (n=60), data were available for 8520 individuals. Of this remaining sample, 5.5% reported one or more psychotic symptom as measured by the PSQ. For each item, more people endorsed the initial probe question than the secondary question(s) (Table 1). Thus, for paranoia, 9.1% endorsed the question ‘Have there been times when you felt that people were deliberately acting to harm you or your interests?’ whereas only 1.5% endorsed ‘Have there been times when you felt that a group of people were plotting to cause you serious harm or injury?’ Similarly for hallucinations, 4.2% of the sample said that there had been times when they heard or saw things that other people could not, but only 0.7% reported hearing voices saying quite a few words or sentences when there was no-one around that might account for it.

Table I Percentage of respondents who endorsed each question on the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ)

PSQ Items Initial probe Secondary question(s) Prevalence (%) of ‘Yes’ responses (weighted) Number of respondents (unweighted)
Hypomania
    Over the past year, have there been times when you felt very happy indeed without a break for days on end? 55.6 4633
    Was there an obvious reason for this? 32.4 2710
    Did your relatives or friends think it was strange or complain about it? 0.6 45
Thought insertion
    Over the past year, have you ever felt that your thoughts were directly interfered with or controlled by some outside force or person? 9.0 790
    Did this come about in a way that many people would find hard to believe, for instance, through telepathy? 0.9 89
Paranoia
    Over the past year, have there been times when you felt that people were against you? 21.2 1769
    Have there been times when you felt that people were deliberately acting to harm you or your interests? 9.1 782
    Have there been times when you felt that a group of people were plotting to cause you serious harm or injury? 1.5 136
Strange experiences
    Over the past year, have there been times when you felt that something strange was going on? 8.9 731
    Did you feel it was so strange that other people would find it very hard to believe? 3.0 264
Hallucinations
    Over the past year, have there been times when you heard or saw things that other people couldn't? 4.2 344
    Did you at any time hear voices saying quite a few words or sentences when there was no-one around that might account for it? 0.7 57
Any psychotic symptom1
    Yes to one or more probe questions 66.1 5535
    Yes to the secondary question(s) 5.5 478
Any psychotic symptom excluding mania
    Yes to one or more probe questions 28.6 2383
    Yes to the secondary question(s) 5.0 438

Socio-demographic distribution of self-reported psychotic symptoms

The frequencies of the variables that were investigated for associations with psychotic symptoms are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Frequencies of the variables examined for associations with psychotic symptoms

Variable Categories Frequency (%) (weighted)1 No. of respondents (unweighted)
Age (years) 55-74 25.7 2696
35-54 38.9 3360
16-34 35.4 2464
Ethnic group White 92.7 7978
Black 2.2 181
South Asian 2.5 142
Other groups 2.0 156
Type of area Rural 34.1 2974
Urban 65.9 5546
Educational qualifications A level or above 36.3 2971
GCSE level 35.9 2946
None 27.1 2541
Estimated verbal IQ Above average 28.8 2610
Average 44.9 3788
Below average 19.9 1605
Alcohol dependence No dependence 92.7 7923
Mild/moderate/severe dependence 7.3 557
Used any drug in past month No 93.3 8034
Yes 6.4 449
Drug dependence No dependence 95.9 8229
Dependent on cannabis only 2.5 171
Dependent on other drug (± cannabis) 1.2 83
Life event in past 6 months No 75.0 6384
Yes 25.0 2136
Victimisation experience No 71.2 6026
Yes 28.2 2437
Neurosis (CIS-R score > 12) ≤ 12 85.1 7161
> 12 14.9 1359

Any psychotic symptom

In the initial unadjusted analysis, the following variables were associated with the presence of any self-reported psychotic symptom: drug dependence, the presence of neurotic disorder, drug use, victimisation events, alcohol dependence, recent stressful life events, non-White ethnic group, younger age, lower IQ, fewer educational qualifications and urban residence (Table 3). Gender was not a significant predictor. In the final model of adjusted odds ratios, neurotic disorder and drug dependence were the variables most strongly associated with psychotic symptoms. Individuals were 3.5 times as likely (95% CI 2.9–4.5) to experience one or more psychotic symptoms if they scored above 12 on the CIS–R, were almost three times as likely (95% CI 2.0–4.4) if they were dependent on cannabis and were just under 2.5 times as likely (95% CI 1.3–3.9) if they were dependent on any other drug (with or without cannabis). Experience of victimisation and alcohol dependence were also strongly associated with psychotic symptoms (OR=2.0, 95% CI 1.7–2.6; OR=1.8, 95% CI 1.3–2.4, respectively). After controlling for interrelationships between all the variables, young age, non-White ethnic group, urban residence and recent drug use were no longer associated significantly with psychotic symptoms.

Table 3 Associations between risk factors and psychotic symptoms on the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ)

Exposure variable Parameter coding Model I: unadjusted odds ratios Final model: adjusted odds ratios
Odds ratio 95% CI P Odds ratio 95% CI P
Age (years) 55-74 (0)
35-54 (1) 1.95 1.47-2.57 <0.001 1.52 1.11-2.09 0.008
16-34 (2) 2.18 1.65-2.88 <0.001 1.30 0.92-1.82 0.14
Ethnic group White (0)
Black (1) 2.27 1.43-3.61 0.001
South Asian (2) 1.91 1.19-3.06 0.007
Other groups (3) 1.01 0.51-1.99 NS
Type of area Rural (0)
Urban (1) 1.27 1.04-1.56 0.02
Educational qualifications A level or above (0)
GCSE level (1) 1.43 1.15-1.79 0.001 1.38 1.07-1.77 0.013
None (2) 1.21 0.94-1.55 NS 1.31 0.97-1.78 0.078
Estimated verbal IQ Above average (0)
Average (1) 1.61 1.25-2.07 <0.001 1.36 1.04-1.79 0.027
Below average (2) 2.10 1.59-2.78 <0.001 1.50 1.08-2.07 0.015
Alcohol dependence No dependence (0)
Mild/moderate/severe dependence (1) 2.97 2.31-3.83 <0.001 1.77 1.32-2.38 <0.001
Used any drug in past month No (0)
Yes (1) 3.44 2.66-4.45 <0.001
Drug dependence No depencence (0)
Dependent on cannabis only (1) 4.90 3.46-6.95 <0.001 2.94 1.98-4.37 <0.001
Dependent on other drug (±cannabis) (2) 6.94 4.43-10.89 <0.001 2.29 1.33-3.95 0.003
Life event in past 6 months No (0)
Yes (1) 2.20 1.82-2.66 <0.001 1.55 1.26-1.92 <0.001
Victimisation experience No (0)
Yes (1) 3.32 2.75-4.01 <0.001 2.06 1.66-2.55 <0.001
Neurosis (CIS-R score > 12) ≤ 12 (0)
> 12 (1) 5.06 4.17-6.14 <0.001 3.63 2.93-4.52 <0.001

Paranoid thoughts

The following factors were independently associated with paranoid thoughts in a multivariate regression analysis: neurotic disorder, victimisation experiences, younger age group, alcohol dependence, recent stressful life event(s), average IQ and male gender (see Table 4).

Table 4 Associations between risk factors and paranoid thoughts

Exposure variable Parameter coding Model I: unadjusted odds ratios Final model: adjusted odds ratios
Odds ratio 95% CI P Odds ratio 95% CI P
Age (years) 55-74 (0)
35-74 (1) 2.63 2.00-3.47 <0.001 2.04 1.51-2.76 <0.001
16-34 (2) 3.16 2.40-4.16 <0.001 2.18 1.60-2.97 <0.001
Gender Female (0)
Male (1) 1.19 1.00-1.41 0.049 1.25 1.03-1.51 0.027
Ethnic group White (0)
Black (1) 1.34 0.78-2.28 NS
South Asian (2) 0.97 0.55-1.72 NS
Other groups (3) 1.87 1.16-3.02 0.01
Type of area Rural (0)
Urban (1) 1.22 1.02-1.47 0.032
Estimated verbal IQ Above average (0)
Average (1) 1.53 1.24-1.89 <0.001 1.42 1.13-1.78 0.002
Below average (2) 1.23 0.94-1.60 NS 1.02 0.77-1.36 NS
Alcohol dependence No dependence (0)
Mild/moderate/severe dependence (1) 2.88 2.25-3.68 <0.001 1.92 1.45-2.56 <0.001
Drug dependence No dependence (0)
Dependent on cannabis only (1) 3.13 2.11-4.64 <0.001
Dependent on other drug (±cannabis) (2) 4.35 2.54-7.46 <0.001
Life event in past 6 months No (0)
Yes (1) 2.15 1.81-2.57 <0.001 1.63 1.34-1.99 <0.001
Victimisation experience No (0)
Yes (1) 3.85 3.23-4.57 <0.001 2.56 2.11-3.10 <0.001
Neurosis (CIS-R score > 12) ≤ 12 (0)
> 12 (1) 4.86 4.04-5.84 <0.001 3.72 3.02-4.58 <0.001

Hallucinatory experiences

The following factors were independently associated with self-reported hallucinatory experiences in the multivariate analysis: neurotic disorder, victimisation experiences, average or below-average IQ, alcohol dependence and female gender (see Table 5).

Table 5 Associations between risk factors and hallucinatory experiences

Exposure variable Parameter coding Model I: unadjusted odds ratios Final model: adjusted odds ratios
Odds ratio 95% CI P Odds ratio 95% CI P
Age (years) 55-74 (0)
35-54 (1) 1.09 0.76-1.57 NS
16-34 (2) 1.36 0.95-1.93 0.09
Gender Male (1)
Female (0) 1.31 1.00-1.72 0.055 1.49 1.09-2.02 0.012
Ethnic group White (0)
Black (1) 2.27 1.15-4.49 0.018 2.48 0.99-6.21 0.052
South Asian (2) 2.36 1.27-4.39 0.007 0.22 0.02-2.11 NS
Other groups (3) 0.65 0.18-2.30 NS
Estimated verbal IQ Above average (0)
Average (1) 2.19 1.48-3.23 <0.001 2.11 1.42-3.14 <0.001
Below average (2) 2.34 1.51-3.65 <0.001 2.22 1.42-3.47 <0.001
Alcohol dependence No dependence (0)
Mild/moderate/severe dependence (1) 1.73 1.09-2.73 0.019 1.85 1.14-3.00 0.012
Victimisation experience No (0)
Yes (1) 2.34 1.77-3.08 <0.001 2.12 1.57-2.87 <0.001
Neurosis (CIS-R score > 12) ≤ 12 (0)
> 12 (1) 2.91 2.13-3.98 <0.001 2.43 1.73-3.41 <0.001

DISCUSSION

This study examined the distribution and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms in the British population. Data were available for a large representative sample of the general population, and included a wealth of information on symptoms, socio-demographic factors, substance use and life events. As with any cross-sectional survey, this study lacks information on temporal relationships, and therefore it is possible to report only associations between variables.

Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in the sample

The annual prevalence of psychotic symptoms, in the absence of psychotic disorder, was 5.5%. This refers to the percentage of people who endorsed one or more items on the PSQ, including the secondary questions for the item (i.e. the more ‘psychotic’ experiences). As shown in Table 1, the prevalence was higher for the ‘less psychotic’ responses, consistent with the existence of a continuum of psychotic phenomena in the general population. The reported figure is lower than the rates of psychotic symptoms reported by other epidemiological studies (17.5%, Reference Poulton, Caspi and MoffittPoulton et al, 2000; 25%, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000). It is likely that variation in prevalence rates across studies is partly a consequence of the different instruments used (number and type of questions asked). The PSQ is a brief measure that assessed only five psychotic symptoms. One would expect higher prevalence rates with a more comprehensive measure such as the CIDI, which contains 17 psychotic symptom items. Also, as we found within the PSQ, questions probing for ‘less psychotic’ experiences are likely to be endorsed more frequently. In addition to differences in the measures, most epidemiological studies have assessed the lifetime prevalence of psychotic symptoms. This will be much greater than the annual prevalence, as measured by the PSQ.

Factors associated with any psychotic symptom

There were associations between psychotic symptoms and neurotic disorder, drug dependence, alcohol dependence, victimisation experiences, recent stressful life events, lower IQ and fewer educational qualifications. Younger age group, non-White ethnic group and urban residence no longer significantly predicted psychotic symptoms after controlling for interrelationships between predictor variables. In terms of drug dependence, the relationship between cannabis dependence and psychotic symptoms was the strongest and also may have contributed to the association between other drug dependence and psychotic symptoms.

The results from this sample replicate previous findings concerning risk factors associated with psychotic symptoms. Van Os et al (Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2000) reported an association between neurotic symptoms and all types of psychosis ratings, from ‘not clinically relevant’ symptoms to clinical psychosis. From our results, it is not possible to know whether neurotic disorder is associated with increased risk of developing psychotic symptoms, or whether it is a consequence of experiencing psychotic symptoms. In a study of adult primary care patients, Olfson et al (Reference Olfson, Lewis-Fernandez and Weissman2002) found that psychotic symptoms were associated with anxiety, depression and alcohol use disorder, and suggested that the latter were all clinical consequences. On the other hand, neurotic symptoms have been reported in excess in those children who later develop psychosis (Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994; Reference Cannon, Caspi and MoffittCannon et al, 2002), and have been identified as part of the initial prodrome in psychosis (Reference Yung and McGorryYung & McGorry, 1996). Longitudinal studies have found that adolescent males with neurotic disorders are more likely to develop schizophrenia years later (Reference Weiser, Reichenberg and RabinowitzWeiser et al, 2001), and that neuroticism (which is related to anxiety proneness) increases the risk for subsequent onset of psychotic symptoms (Reference Krabbendam, Janssen and BakKrabbendam et al, 2002). In a recent review, Freeman & Garety (Reference Freeman and Garety2003) argue that the frequent occurrence of emotional disorder prior to and accompanying psychosis suggests that neurosis contributes to the development of the positive symptoms of psychosis.

Similarly, we cannot determine the direction of the relationship between cannabis dependence and psychotic symptoms from these data. However, a number of cohort studies have shown that consumption of cannabis is a risk factor for later psychosis (e.g. Reference Andreasson, Allebeck and EngstromAndreasson et al, 1987; Reference van Os, Bak and Hanssenvan Os et al, 2002; Reference Zammit, Allebeck and AndreassonZammit et al, 2002). Thus, Arsenault et al (2002) found that cannabis use in adolescence increased the risk of experiencing schizophrenia symptoms in adulthood, indicating a causal link. Furthermore, this risk was specific to cannabis use, as opposed to the use of other drugs. The association between alcohol dependence and psychotic symptoms may be related to the occurrence of withdrawal symptoms.

Consistent with previous studies (e.g. Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000), lower educational achievement was associated with self-reported psychotic symptoms. The association with potentially threatening life events and victimisation events also corresponds with previously reported risk factors for psychosis. Using the same National Survey data, Bebbington et al (Reference Bebbington, Bhugra and Brugha2004) found a high prevalence of reported victimisation among people with psychosis, greater than that found among people with neurotic disorder or drug and alcohol dependence. The association between non-White ethnic group and psychotic symptoms in this study was no longer significant after controlling for other factors, including victimisation and stressful life events. Although it has been reported that Black and ethnic minority patients with psychosis do not experience more life events than do White British patients, they do perceive these events as more threatening (Reference Gilvarry, Walsh and SameleGilvarry et al, 1999).

Factors associated with paranoid thoughts

Paranoid thoughts were associated with neurotic symptoms, victimisation experience(s), younger age, alcohol dependence, stressful life events in the past 6 months, average IQ and male gender. The relationships between paranoia and victimisation and stressful life events are consistent with cognitive psychological theories about the development and maintenance of psychotic symptoms (Reference Garety, Kuipers and FowlerGarety et al, 2001; Reference Freeman, Garety and KuipersFreeman et al, 2002). Thus, experiences of victimisation may lead individuals to believe that they are vulnerable and to view other people and the world as hostile and threatening; and stressful events may then trigger symptoms. Janssen et al (Reference Janssen, Hanssen and Bak2003) found that perceived discrimination was associated longitudinally with onset of delusional ideation. Unfortunately, it is not possible from these data to determine the precise temporal relationships between victimisation, life events and paranoia. It could be that subjects with paranoid thoughts have a biased recall for these experiences, or that the supposedly paranoid thoughts are actually real, and that people are trying to harm them. Neurotic symptoms were strongly associated with paranoid thoughts in this sample. Again, this result is consistent with cognitive models of persecutory delusions (Reference Freeman, Garety and KuipersFreeman et al, 2002), in which anxiety and depression (particularly anxiety) are thought to play a central role in the formation of paranoid beliefs.

Factors associated with hallucinatory experiences

Neurotic disorder, victimisation experiences, average and below-average IQ, alcohol dependence and female gender were associated with hallucinatory experiences. There was a trend for an association between hallucinations and Black ethnic group, which replicates the findings of Johns et al (Reference Johns, Nazroo and Bebbington2002) from an earlier UK national survey. In that study, the prevalence of hallucinations assessed by the PSQ in a sample of people of Caribbean origin living in Britain was 2.5 times higher than that in a White sample, although no statistical comparison was reported. The finding that women were more likely to report hallucinatory experiences is consistent with the higher rates of hallucinations in females found in previous studies (Reference TienTien, 1991). As with paranoia, the association with victimisation is consistent with psychological theories of hallucinations, particularly the link between exposure to trauma and the development of hallucinations (Reference Romme and EscherRomme & Escher, 1989).

Further research

This study found that self-reported psychotic experiences in the general population were associated with risk factors similar to those commonly reported for clinical psychosis. Using the interview data from both phases of the survey, we now intend to compare the correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms and clinical psychotic disorder in this sample.

APPENDIX

The Psychosis Screening

Questionnaire (PSQ; Reference Bebbington and NayaniBebbington and Nayani, 1995; reproduced by permission of the authors)

Hypomania

  • Probe: Over the past year, have there been times when you felt very happy indeed without a break for days on end?

If yes,

  • Was there an obvious reason for this?

  • Did your relatives or friends think it was strange or complain about it?

Thought insertion

  • Probe: Over the past year, have you ever felt that your thoughts were directly interfered with or controlled by some outside force or person?

If yes,

  • Did this come about in a way that many people would find hard to believe, for instance, through telepathy?

Paranoia

  • Probe: Over the past year, have there been times when you felt that people were against you?

If yes,

  • Have there been times when you felt that people were deliberately acting to harm you or your interests?

  • Have there been times when you felt that a group of people were plotting to cause you serious harm or injury?

Strange experiences

  • Probe: Over the past year, have there been times when you felt that something strange was going on?

If yes,

  • Did you feel it was so strange that other people would find it very hard to believe?

Hallucinations

  • Probe: Over the past year, have there been times when you heard or saw things that other people couldn't?

If yes,

  • Did you at any time hear voices saying quite a few words or sentences when there was no-one around that might account for it?

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. The psychological factors associated with psychotic symptoms in the general population are consistent with cognitive psychological models of psychotic symptoms in patient samples.

  2. Neurotic symptoms may contribute to the development of psychotic symptoms, and offer a target for intervention in people with prodromal or early warning signs of psychosis.

  3. Cannabis dependence may increase the risk of experiencing psychotic symptoms.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The PSQ items are intended to screen for psychotic symptoms, and do not provide data on the precise nature of the experiences.

  2. Because the study is cross-sectional, causal inferences cannot be drawn clearly for the associations. Longitudinal studies are needed.

  3. The number of interviewees with psychotic experiences was modest, particularly in the analyses of factors associated with hallucinations.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Andreasson, S., Allebeck, P., Engstrom, A., et al (1987) Cannabis and schizophrenia. A longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. Lancet, ii, 14831486.Google Scholar
Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Poulton, R., et al (2002) Cannabis use in adolescence and risk for adult psychosis: longitudinal prospective study. BMJ, 325, 12121213.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. E. & Nayani, T. (1995) The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 1119.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P E., Bhugra, D., Brugha, T., et al (2004) Psychosis, victimisation and childhood disadvantage. Evidence from the Second British National Survey of Psychiatric Epidemiology. British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 220226.Google Scholar
Brugha, T., Bebbington, P. E., Tennant, C., et al (1985) The List of Threatening Experiences: a subset of 12 life event categories with considerable long-term contextual threat. Psychological Medicine, 15, 189194.Google Scholar
Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., et al (2002) Evidence for early-childhood pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 449457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D. & Garety, P. A. (2003) Connecting neurosis and psychosis: the direct influence of emotion on delusions and hallucinations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 923947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., et al (2002) A cognitive model of persecutory delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 331347.Google Scholar
Garety, P., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., et al (2001) A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189195.Google Scholar
Gilvarry, C. M., Walsh, E., Samele, C., et al (1999) Life events, ethnicity and perceptions of discrimination in patients with severe mental illness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 600608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janssen, I., Hanssen, M., Bak, M., et al (2003) Discrimination and delusional ideation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 7176.Google Scholar
Johns, L. C., Nazroo, J. Y., Bebbington, P., et al (2002) Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 174178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., et al (1994) Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet, 344, 13981402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Gallagher, T. J., Abelson, J. M., et al (1996) Lifetime prevalence, demographic risk factors, and diagnostic validity of nonaffective psychosis as assessed in a US community sample. The National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 10221031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krabbendam, L., Janssen, I., Bak, M., et al (2002) Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for psychosis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, G., Pelosi, A. J., Araya, R., et al (1992) Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standard assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychological Medicine, 22, 465486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H. E. (1982) National Adult Reading Test (NART) (2nd edn). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Olfson, M., Lewis-Fernandez, R., Weissman, M. M., et al (2002) Psychotic symptoms in an urban general medicine practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 14121419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulton, R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., et al (2000) Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 10531058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romme, M. A. & Escher, A. D. (1989) Hearing voices. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 209216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singleton, N., Bumpstead, R., O'Brien, M., et al (2001) Psychiatric Morbidity among Adults Living in Private Households, 2000. London: Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Stockwell, T., Murphy, D. & Hogson, R. (1983) The Severity of Alcohol Dependence Questionnaire: its use, reliability and validity. British Journal of Addiction, 78, 145155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tien, A. Y. (1991) Distributions of hallucinations in the population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 26, 287292.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., et al (2000) Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophrenia Research, 45, 1120.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., et al (2001) Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms: an urban-rural comparison. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 663668.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Bak, M., Hanssen, M., et al (2002) Cannabis use and psychosis: a longitudinal population-based study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156, 319327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdoux, H., van Os, J., Maurice-Tison, S., et al (1998) Is early adulthood a critical developmental stage for psychosis proneness? A survey of delusional ideation in normal subjects. Schizophrenia Research, 29, 247254.Google Scholar
Weiser, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., et al (2001) Association between nonpsychotic psychiatric diagnoses in adolescent males and subsequent onset of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 959964.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992a) SCAN Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992b) Tenth Revision of the International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Yung, A. R. & McGorry, P. D. (1996) The initial prodrome in psychosis: descriptive and qualitative aspects. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 30, 587599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, S., Allebeck, P., Andreasson, S., et al (2002) Self reported cannabis use as a risk factor for schizophrenia in Swedish conscripts of 1969: historical cohort study. BMJ, 325, 11991201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table I Percentage of respondents who endorsed each question on the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ)

Figure 1

Table 2 Frequencies of the variables examined for associations with psychotic symptoms

Figure 2

Table 3 Associations between risk factors and psychotic symptoms on the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ)

Figure 3

Table 4 Associations between risk factors and paranoid thoughts

Figure 4

Table 5 Associations between risk factors and hallucinatory experiences

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.