Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T10:12:42.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of response to treatment for chronic fatigue syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Richard P. Bentall*
Affiliation:
University of Manchester
Pauline Powell
Affiliation:
Royal Liverpool Broadgreen Trust
Fred J. Nye
Affiliation:
Royal Liverpool Broadgreen Trust
Richard H. T. Edwards
Affiliation:
Formerly University of Liverpool, UK
*
Professor Richard Bentall, Department of Psychology, University of Manchester, Coupland 1 Building, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PL, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Controlled trials have shown that psychological interventions designed to encourage graded exercise can facilitate recovery from chronic fatigue syndrome.

Aims

To identify predictors of response to psychological treatment for chronic fatigue syndrome.

Method

Of 114 patients assigned to equally effective treatment conditions in a randomised, controlled trial, 95 completed follow-up assessments. Relationships between variables measured prior to randomisation and changes in physical functioning and subjective handicap at 1 year were evaluated by multiple regression.

Results

Poor outcome was predicted by membership of a self-help group, being in receipt of sickness benefit at the start of treatment, and dysphoria as measured by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale. Severity of symptoms and duration of illness were not predictors of response.

Conclusions

Poor outcome in the psychological treatment of chronic fatigue syndrome is predicted by variables that indicate resistance to accepting the therapeutic rationale, poor motivation to treatment adherence or secondary gains from illness.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2002 

Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) is associated with muscular and cardiovascular deconditioning (Reference De Lorenzo, Xiao and MukherjeeDe Lorenzo et al, 1998), circadian desynchrony (Reference Williams, Piromohamed and MinorsWilliams et al, 1996), sleep abnormalities (Reference Morriss, Wearden and BattersbyMorriss et al, 1997) and cortisol deficiency (Reference Cleare, Bearn and AllainCleare et al, 1995). It has been proposed that these signs are consequences of disrupted cycles of activity, and that CFS is maintained by illness beliefs that encourage activity avoidance (Reference Wessely, Hotopf and SharpeWessely et al, 1998). Psychological interventions that encourage exercise are effective treatments for CFS (Reference Sharpe, Hawton and SimkinSharpe et al, 1996; Reference Deale, Chalder and MarksDeale et al, 1997; Reference Powell, Bentall and NyePowell et al, 2001). We tested the hypotheses that variables indicative of rigid illness beliefs (duration of untreated illness, membership of a support group advocating exercise avoidance, belief in physical illness), limited emotional resources (concurrent dysphoria, receipt of antidepressant medication, sleep problems), poor social support (living alone or as a lone parent), or probable loss of benefits on recovery, would predict poor response to this kind of treatment.

METHOD

Participants

Participants were 114 patients recruited from a chronic fatigue clinic and an infectious diseases out-patient clinic and assigned to three treatment groups in a randomised, controlled trial of a brief psychological intervention of chronic fatigue syndrome (Reference Powell, Bentall and NyePowell et al, 2001). Thirty-four patients assigned to a no-treatment control group were not included in this study, and a further 36 patients eligible for the trial did not take part — 16 because they were unable to attend, 8 because they were in receipt of other therapy and 12 because they refused. All participants fulfilled the Oxford criteria for CFS (Reference Sharpe, Archard and BanarvalaSharpe et al, 1991) and scored less than 25 on the physical functioning sub-scale of the 36-item short form health survey (SF36; Reference Ware and SherbourneWare & Sherbourne, 1992). This sub-scale has a scoring range of 10 to 30, where 10 indicates maximum physical limitation in self-care and 30 indicates the ability to do vigorous sports. The following exclusion criteria were employed:

  1. (a) further physical investigations or other treatments including antidepressant therapy (unless the same dose had been taken for at least 3 months without improvement);

  2. (b) psychotic illness, somatisation disorder, eating disorder or a history of substance misuse;

  3. (c) confinement to wheelchair or bed.

Treatment methods

The trial compared three different ‘dosages’ (defined in terms of therapist time and follow-up telephone contacts with the therapist) of an educational intervention in which patients were taught evidence-based physiological explanations of their symptoms and were encouraged to undertake a home-based graded exercise programme. The ‘minimum intervention’ patients received two individual treatment sessions and two telephone follow-up calls, plus access to a telephone helpline. The ‘telephone intervention’ patients received an additional seven planned follow-up calls, and the ‘maximum intervention’ patients received an additional seven face-to-face sessions over 4 months.

Assessments

Clinical and demographic data collected from the patients prior to recruitment to the trial included duration of illness, gender, receipt of antidepressant therapy and sickness benefits, membership of a self-help group (all assessed as yes/no variables) and belief in a physical cause of CFS assessed by means of a four-point scale (‘definitely physical’, ‘mainly physical’, ‘equally physical and psychological/stress-related’ and ‘mainly psychological/stress-related’) derived from a measure of CFS illness beliefs developed by Wood et al (Reference Wood, Bentall and Gopfert1991). Self-rated validated outcome measures sent by post were assessed before randomisation, and 3, 6 and 12 months after the beginning of treatment. Primary outcomes were measured on the physical functioning sub-scale of the SF36, and on the fatigue scale (Reference Chalder, Berelowitz and PawlikowskaChalder et al, 1993: range 0-11, scores greater than 3 indicate excessive fatigue). At baseline the mean score for physical functioning of the treated patients was 16.0 and their mean score on the fatigue scale was 10.28. Secondary outcome measures assessed before treatment and at follow-up included the London Handicap Scale (Reference Harwood, Rogers and DickinsonHarwood et al, 1994), a multi-dimensional assessment of mobility, physical independence, occupational functioning, social integration and economic self-sufficiency which yields a global handicap score; the Hospital Anxiety and Depression (HAD) scale (Reference Zigmond and SnaithZigmond & Snaith, 1983; scores above 10 indicate caseness); and a four-item sleep problem questionnaire (Reference Jenkins, Stanton and NiemcrykJenkins et al, 1988).

Statistical analysis

Outcome data were available for 95 of the treated patients, representing loss from the study of 17% of the sample. For those who dropped out, the last recorded data point was carried forward prior to analysis. We analysed predictors of improvement by multiple regression, entering all variables at once, a method which is more conservative and less prone to type 1 errors than the more commonly used stepwise method. As multiple regression allows one variable to be included for every 10 participants, we had sufficient power to include nine predictors (Reference Kleinbaum, Kupper and MullerKleinbaum et al, 1988). Although treatment dose bore no relation to outcome in our main outcome analysis at 1 year (Reference Powell, Bentall and NyePowell et al, 2001) we included it as a predictor in all analyses by coding dose as 1 (minimum treatment), 2 (telephone treatment) and 3 (maximum treatment). Initial scores on the dependent variables were included in the regression equations to control for systematic relationships between initial scores and outcome.

Predictors were chosen on the basis of the hypotheses listed in the introduction. We therefore included the following binary variables: membership of a self-help group, living alone or as a lone parent, being in receipt of sickness benefits, and antidepressant therapy. Continuous variables included in the analysis were duration of illness, illness beliefs, and sleep disturbance scores. Depression and anxiety scores from the HAD scales were also included, but because these were highly correlated (Pearson r=0.56, P<0.001, one-tailed), they were summed into a single dysphoria variable to avoid the problem of multi-collinearity. Initial scores on these variables are shown in Tables 1 and 2. The mean physical functioning score of the members of the self-help groups was 14.29 compared with 16.38 for non-members, indicating that members were more impaired at the outset (t=2.80, P<0.01, 112 d.f.).

Table 1 Initial scores on predictor variables: binary variables (total n=95)

Predictor variable n (%)
Membership of self-help group 17 18
Living alone or lone parent 19 20
In receipt of antidepressant therapy 14 15
In receipt of sickness benefit 37 39

Table 2 Initial scores on predictor variables: continuous variables

Predictor variable Mean s.d.
HAD depression score (range 0-21, > 10 indicates depression) 8.82 3.72
HAD anxiety score (range 0-21, > 10 indicates anxiety) 20.28 4.63
Sleep score (range 0-20, where 20 indicates maximum disturbance) 12.83 4.76
Cause of illness (range 1-4, where I indicates definitely physical and 4 indicates mainly psychological) 2.15 0.84

RESULTS

The outcome of these interventions at 1-year follow-up has been presented elsewhere (Reference Powell, Bentall and NyePowell et al, 2001). Patients in the three treatment groups showed significantly greater improvement on all primary and secondary outcome variables in comparison with patients in the control group. However, no significant difference was found between the outcomes associated with the three treatment dosages. Only two of the 34 patients in the control group met our predetermined criteria for a clinically significant improvement, which was a final score of 25 or more or an increase of 10 or more on the physical functioning scale (virtually equivalent to normal functioning). However, 26 of 37 patients in the ‘minimum intervention’ group, 27 of 39 in the ‘telephone intervention’ group and 26 of 38 in the ‘maximum intervention’ group improved.

Predictors of change in physical functioning scores

Our main outcome measure in the trial was change on the SF36 physical functioning scale, which was approximately normally distributed, with an average improvement of 8.83 points (s.d.=5.23, skewness—0.557), indicating that most of the patients who were followed-up improved. When this was used as the dependent variable in the regression model, the equation was highly significant (r 2=0.38, F=6.29, P<0.001). Standardised regression coefficients and significance tests for each of the predictors are given in Table 3. Apart from initial physical functioning scores, poor response to treatment was predicted by (in order of predictive power) dysphoria scores, membership of a self-help group, and receipt of sickness benefit. Living arrangements (alone or as a single parent), receipt of antidepressant therapy, sleep disturbance, duration of illness, dose of treatment and — surprisingly — initial illness beliefs did not add significantly to the model.

Table 3 Standardised regression coefficients, t and significance values for predictor variables in regression equations predicting changes in SF36 physical functioning scores and London Handicap Scale scores at 1-year follow-up

Dependent variable and predictors β t P
Change in physical functioning (SF36)
Membership of support group -0.28 -3.45 0.001
Living alone or as lone parent 0.12 1.52 0.13
Receipt of sickness benefit -0.21 -2.57 0.01
Duration of illness 0.04 0.42 0.68
Receipt of antidepressant medication 0.11 1.36 0.18
Dysphoria (HAD) -0.34 -3.75 0.001
Belief in physical cause -0.08 -0.88 0.38
Sleep problems 0.12 1.31 0.38
Treatment dose -0.06 -0.70 0.49
Initial physical functioning -0.53 -5.84 0.001
Change in London Handicap Scale score
Membership of support group -0.30 -3.79 0.001
Living alone or as lone parent 0.13 1.66 0.10
Receipt of sickness benefit -0.22 -2.45 0.02
Duration of illness -0.04 -0.47 0.64
Receipt of antidepressant medication 0.11 1.41 0.16
Dysphoria (HAD) -0.20 -2.13 0.04
Belief in physical cause -0.19 -2.28 0.03
Sleep problems -0.08 -0.86 0.39
Treatment dose -0.04 -0.53 0.60
Initial handicap score -0.60 -5.86 0.001

Predictors of change in London Handicap Scale scores

Fatigue scores (both change and final), our second primary outcome measure, deviated severely from the normal distribution and were unsuited to regression analysis. Therefore, to assess the validity of the above findings, the analyses were repeated using change scores on the London Handicap Scale, which were normally distributed. The regression equation was again highly significant (r 2=0.38, F=6.13, P<0.001). In addition to initial scores on the scale, predictors of poor response on this scale were membership of a self-help group, receipt of sickness benefit, belief in a physical cause of illness, and dysphoria (Table 3).

Analyses excluding initial scores

It is difficult to interpret the theoretical significance of the observed relationships between initial and change scores as these may simply reflect the fact that those who were most ill had most opportunity to change. The analyses were therefore repeated with final scores as the dependent variables. These were less than ideal for analysis because they had highly non-normal distributions with scores highly skewed towards maximum values (physical functioning mean 24.74, skewness -0.96; London Handicap Scale mean 76.27, skewness -0.22). None the less, the regression equation for final physical functioning was significant (r 2=0.34, F=5.16, P < 0.001), the same three predictors were retained in the equation, but initial physical functioning scores no longer predicted outcome. Similarly, for final London Handicap Scale scores, a significant regression equation was calculated (r 2=0.39, F=6.59, P < 0.001), with the same four predictors retained, and no significant contribution to the model from initial London Handicap Scale scores.

Supplementary analyses

In the above analyses we assumed that the scores of those who dropped out had not changed from their last recorded data points. To test the impact of this assumption, we repeated the main analyses assuming that all of those who dropped out had done poorly by assigning them the relevant mean scores of the untreated control group. For changes in physical functioning, dysphoria, membership of a support group and receipt of state benefits remained predictors. For changes in the London Handicap Scale scores, dysphoria, membership of a support group, receipt of benefits, and physical illness attributions were all retained. As a further check for the validity of our assumptions, we repeated the analyses with all data from those who dropped out excluded (leaving a sample size of 95). In this case, dysphoria and membership of a support group remained predictors of changes in physical functioning, but receipt of benefits was no longer a predictor. However, for changes in scores on the London Handicap Scale, only membership of a support group remained as a predictor.

DISCUSSION

Variables that did not predict treatment response

Variables measured on admission to the trial predicted a sizeable proportion of the variance in outcomes — nearly 40% in most of the analyses. Variables that failed to predict response to treatment were, in many ways, as interesting as those that did. Our supplementary analyses of raw outcome scores indicated that severity of initial scores on these measures did not predict improvement. Duration of illness, receipt of antidepressant therapy, living circumstances and (as we expected from our main trial data) treatment dose also failed to predict outcome. The implication of these negative findings is that severity and chronicity of core CFS symptoms are not impediments to clinical response, other things being equal, and that psychological treatment should therefore be offered to patients whose symptoms are fairly severe. Although non-ambulatory patients were not included in this study, we have elsewhere reported the successful treatment of wheelchair-bound patients using an extended version of our treatment programme (Reference Powell, Edwards and BentallPowell et al, 1999).

Variables predicting treatment response

The most robust predictors of poor treatment response were membership of a support group and concurrent emotional difficulties. Although we found that support group members were more ill than non-members on admission to the study, this was controlled for in our regression analyses, and therefore cannot explain the impact that membership had on outcomes. Receipt of benefits and belief in exclusively physical causes of CFS were also predictors of poor response in some of the analyses. These variables may be indicators of poor motivation for treatment adherence. Individuals who are emotionally distressed, who will experience financial difficulties when improving, and who are initially sceptical about the value of psychological approaches, may have difficulty persisting with a graded exercise programme.

Consistency with previous findings

Consistent with our findings, Sharpe et al (Reference Sharpe, Hawton and Seagrott1992) followed-up people with CFS attending an infectious disease clinic and found that poor outcome was associated with emotional disorder, belief in a viral illness and membership of a self-help organisation (other predictors of poor response were limiting exercise, avoiding alcohol and changing employment). In a 4-year follow-up of patients taking part in an open, uncontrolled trial of psychological treatment for CFS, Bonner et al (Reference Bonner, Ron and Chalder1994) found that poor response was predicted by emotional disorder and that there was also a trend for physical illness beliefs to predict a poor response; however, no relationship was found between response and membership of a support group. The current finding that receipt of sickness benefit at the start of treatment is associated with poor outcome is consistent with a report by Barsky & Borus (Reference Barsky and Borus1999) that disability payments are associated with worse symptomatic outcome after medical treatment.

Mechanic (Reference Mechanic1986) reported that interest groups who adhere to particular theories of illness may play a part in reinforcing illness beliefs. In the presence of such a group, a personal problem becomes a shared social problem. Although there are undoubted psychological benefits from this kind of support (Reference McCully, Sisto and NatelsonMcCully et al, 1996), there have been reports that the advice dispensed by some CFS groups may have a negative effect on recovery, for example by advocating the avoidance of activity (Reference Abbey and StrausAbbey, 1993; Reference Surawy, Hackmann and HawtonSurawy et al, 1995). It may be counterproductive to discourage patients from joining such groups, however; instead, clinicians and researchers should forge constructive alliances with support groups, to ensure that patients receive advice that is consistent and evidence-based. In this context, we should like to acknowledge that our work has been assisted by support groups in the Merseyside area, who have had an active role in ensuring that our treatment programme is now being offered as a routine National Health Service therapy.

Limitations of this study

The randomised clinical trial from which these data were taken had several limitations, which should be noted. The Oxford criteria for CFS, used as an entry criterion, are broader than the now widely used Centers for Disease Control definition (Reference FukudaFukuda, 1994). More importantly, outcome was measured by self-report. Although this had the advantage of minimising the possibility of researcher bias in assessing outcome, it would have been better to have included an objective measure of exercise capacity. None the less, it seems implausible that these limitations can account for the results discussed here. In general, the observation that an intervention designed to affect illness beliefs had positive effects, coupled with the observation that variables indicative of poor treatment motivation predicted poor outcome, is consistent with our overall hypothesis that attitudinal factors play a part in maintaining CFS symptoms.

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. The severity and chronicity of chronic fatigue syndrome symptoms do not predict response to psychological treatment aimed at encouraging graded exercise. This kind of treatment should therefore be available to all patients.

  2. Treatment response is predicted by concurrent emotional difficulties and receipt of state benefits, factors that may undermine motivation to persist with treatment.

  3. Membership of a support group is also associated with poor treatment response, probably because such groups sometimes advise against graded exercise interventions. This observation highlights the importance of building a constructive alliance between services and support groups.

LIMITATIONS

  1. A broad definition of chronic fatigue syndrome was used as an entry criterion.

  2. Seventeen per cent of patients were lost to follow-up.

  3. Only subjective measures of outcome were recorded; there was no objective assessment of exercise capacity.

Acknowledgement

This research was supported by a grant from the Linbury Trust.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Financial support from the Linbury Trust.

References

Abbey, S. (1993) Somatization, illness attribution and the sociocultural psychiatry of chronic fatigue syndrome. In Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (ed. Straus, S.), pp. 238261. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Barsky, A. & Borus, J. (1999) Functional somatic symptoms. Annals of Internal Medicine, 130, 910921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonner, D., Ron, M., Chalder, T., et al (1994) Chronic fatigue syndrome: a follow up study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57, 617621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chalder, T., Berelowitz, G., Pawlikowska, T., et al (1993) Development of a fatigue scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37, 147153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleare, A., Bearn, J., Allain, T., et al (1995) Contrasting neuroendocrine responses in depression and chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Affective Disorders, 34, 283289.Google Scholar
Deale, A., Chalder, T., Marks, I., et al (1997) Cognitive behavioural therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 408414.Google Scholar
De Lorenzo, F., Xiao, H., Mukherjee, M., et al (1998) Chronic fatigue syndrome: physical and cardiovascular deconditioning. Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 91, 475481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukuda, K. (1994) The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach towards its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121, 953959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harwood, R., Rogers, A., Dickinson, E. et al (1994) Measuring handicap: methods, and a model. Quality in Health Care, 3, 5357.Google Scholar
Jenkins, D., Stanton, B., Niemcryk, S., et al (1988) A scale for the estimation of sleep problemsin clinical research. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 41, 313321.Google Scholar
Kleinbaum, D. G., Kupper, L. L. & Muller, K. E. (1988) Applied Regression Analysis and Other Multivariable Methods (2nd edn). Boston, MA: PWS-Kent.Google Scholar
McCully, K., Sisto, S. & Natelson, B. (1996) Use of exercise for treatment of chronic fatigue syndrome. Sports Medicine, 1, 3548.Google Scholar
Mechanic, D. (1986) The concept of illness behaviour: culture, situation and personal predisposition. Psychological Medicine, 16, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morriss, R., Wearden, A. & Battersby, L. (1997) The relation of sleep difficulties to fatigue, mood and disability in chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42, 597605.Google Scholar
Powell, P., Edwards, R. & Bentall, R. (1999) The treatment of wheelchair-bound chronic fatigue syndrome patients: two case studies of a pragmatic treatment approach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 27, 249260.Google Scholar
Powell, P., Bentall, R., Nye, F., et al (2001) Randomised controlled trial of patient education to encourage graded exercise in chronic fatigue syndrome. BMJ, 322, 387390.Google Scholar
Sharpe, M., Archard, L., Banarvala, J., et al (1991) A report – chronic fatigue syndrome: guidelines for research. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 84, 118121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, M., Hawton, K., Seagrott, V., et al (1992) Followup of patients presenting with fatigue to an infectious diseases clinic. BMJ, 305, 147152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharpe, M., Hawton, K., Simkin, S., et al (1996) Cognitive behaviour therapy for the chronic fatigue syndrome: arandomised controlled trial. BMJ, 321, 2226.Google Scholar
Surawy, C., Hackmann, A., Hawton, K., et al (1995) Chronic fatigue syndrome: a cognitive approach. Behavioural Research Therapy, 33, 535544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, J. & Sherbourne, C. (1992) The MOS 36-item short form health survey (SF36). Medical Care, 30, 473483.Google Scholar
Wessely, S., Hotopf, M. & Sharpe, M. (1998) Chronic Fatigue and Its Syndromes. Oxfford: Oxford Uniiversity Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, G., Piromohamed, J., Minors, D., et al (1996) Dissociation of body-temperature and melatonin secretion circadian rhythms in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Clinical Physiology, 16, 327337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, G., Bentall, R., Gopfert, M., et al (1991) A comparative psychiatric assessment of patients with chronic fatigue syndrome and muscle disease. Psychological Medicine, 21, 619628.Google Scholar
Zigmond, A. & Snaith, R. (1983) The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361370.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Initial scores on predictor variables: binary variables (total n=95)

Figure 1

Table 2 Initial scores on predictor variables: continuous variables

Figure 2

Table 3 Standardised regression coefficients, t and significance values for predictor variables in regression equations predicting changes in SF36 physical functioning scores and London Handicap Scale scores at 1-year follow-up

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.