Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T17:16:15.571Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Population research and mental health policy

Bridging the gap

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Brian Cooper*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Bruce Singh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
*
Dr Brian Cooper, Section of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © 2000 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Twenty years ago a World Psychiatric Association (WPA) international symposium under the title ‘What is a case?’ (Reference Wing, Bebbington and RobinsWing et al, 1981) served to highlight a central methodological problem of psychiatric epidemiology. Since then, incorporation in the main classification systems of glossaries and operational guidelines (World Health Organization, 1993; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), together with advances in standardised interview techniques, seemed to have so far underpinned diagnostic assessment that this issue, if not fully resolved, had at any rate lost much of its immediacy. However, application of the concept of ‘medical necessity’ in managed-care programmes (Reference FordFord, 1998) has led to a critical reappraisal in the USA. Regier et al (Reference Regier, Kaelber and Rae1998), in a review of estimates from two large-scale USA projects, the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) studies and the National Comorbidity Survey (NCS), noted that these differed widely. Thus, for the age range 18-54 years, the mean annual percentage prevalence rates for alcohol dependence, major depressive disorder, anxiety disorder, panic disorder and social phobia were 4.1, 4.2, 9.9, 1.1 and 1.6, respectively, in the ECA studies (one-wave estimates) and 7.4, 10.1, 15.3, 2.2 and 7.4, respectively, in the NCS. Corresponding differences were found between ‘lifetime’ prevalence rates for these categories. Such large disparities, in estimates derived by similar methods in the same country only a few years apart, are unlikely to be due to real differences in morbidity experience, and must raise doubts about the accuracy of case reporting.

Apart from diagnostic inconsistency, total prevalence rates in the two projects were deemed too high to provide useful guidance to the extent of need as gauged by medical necessity. In the ECA first wave, one-year psychiatric prevalence was 20% and lifetime prevalence 32%, and following a second wave of interviewing these figures rose to 28% and 44%, respectively. The single-wave NCS yielded for the age range 18-54 years a mean one-year prevalence of 29% and a lifetime prevalence of 48%. All these rates are so much higher than the annual estimate of 2.8% for severe mental disorders in the USA (National Advisory Mental Health Council, 1993) as to raise doubts about the relevance of such data in gauging service needs. Regier et al (Reference Regier, Kaelber and Rae1998) considered it unlikely, given current USA preoccupation with cost-containment and managed-care plans, that mental health care could be provided for 28% or 29% of the population in any one year, and went on to speculate that in many of the reported cases this might not in fact be required:

“…It is possible that many people with currently defined mental syndromes (in particular among the affective and anxiety disorders) not brought to medical attention may be having appropriate homeostatic responses that are neither pathologic nor in need of treatment” (Reference Regier, Kaelber and RaeRegier et al, 1998).

These remarks, coming from workers who themselves were involved in planning and coordinating the research projects in question, deserve to be taken seriously, particularly in view of current plans for a World Mental Health (WMH 2000) Initiative, involving a series of parallel community surveys in countries across the globe (Reference KesslerKessler, 1999). If one accepts that prevalence statistics are of limited value in causal inquiry, and that the main value of cross-sectional surveys (other than as a baseline for prospective studies) lies in providing the descriptive statistics required for health care policy and planning, it follows that their findings must be capable of evaluation within that frame of reference. The broad argument, however, condenses a number of distinct points: (a) that the validity of case identification and diagnosis in community surveys still needs improvement; (b) that such research should provide information on unmet needs for specialist treatment and care; and (c) that there are wider implications for health policy if a high prevalence of clinically significant disorders - including milder forms - is indeed confirmed.

DIAGNOSTIC VALIDITY IN FIELD STUDIES

The historical background has been reviewed by Dohrenwend (Reference Dohrenwend1990), and more recent developments by Brugha et al (Reference Brugha, Bebbington and Jenkins1999a ). In first-generation surveys prior to the Second World War, most investigators relied on key informants and agency records, while the handful of studies based on direct (unstructured) interviewing by psychiatrists yielded low prevalence estimates, with a mean of under 4%. From the Second World War to around 1980, many second-generation surveys used clinical assessment by psychiatrists, based on personal examination or evaluation of interview protocols, and sometimes on collateral information. A number of standardised interviews, such as the Psychiatric Status Schedule (PSS; Reference Spitzer, Endicott and FleissSpitzer et al, 1970) and the Present State Examination (PSE; Reference Wing, Cooper and SartoriusWing et al, 1974) were developed and found on testing to provide good interrater agreement. A review of research in this period revealed a general increase in reported prevalence, the average being now around 20% (Reference Dohrenwend and DohrenwendDohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1974).

In the third-generation period of the past 20 years, reported case rates have further increased, as the above-cited ECA and NCS survey data suggest. During these years there has been a shift away from expert assessment, towards reliance on lay interviewers and questionnaire techniques: a trend encouraged by the use of lifetime measures, derived from self-reporting of symptoms at any time in the past. Semi-structured or guided interviews have been largely replaced by tightly structured schedules with closed questions, and clinical evaluation by computerised diagnosis. Such techniques can be, and have been, applied to large population samples (in some instances upward of 10 000 persons) but must, by virtue of the interviewers' lack of experience, omit the elements of flexible probing and of clinical judgement in interpreting responses, previously thought necessary for diagnosis. The best-known packages, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS; Reference Robins, Helzer and CroughlanRobins et al, 1981) and its successor, the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI; Reference Robins, Wing and WittchenRobins et al, 1988), were developed explicitly as a response to the need for an instrument that would as closely as possible replicate a psychiatrist's diagnosis in situations where diagnosis by clinicians was not feasible (Reference Helzer, Robins and McEvoyHelzer et al, 1985; Reference KesslerKessler, 1999).

The same practical necessity has decreed that a number of instruments originally intended for clinicians should either by adapted for use by lay interviewers (Reference Rodgers and MannRodgers & Mann, 1986; Reference Lewis, Pelosi and ArayaLewis et al, 1992), or else incorporated into fully-structured schedules (Reference Robins, Wing and WittchenRobins et al, 1988). Even the most comprehensive ‘clinical’ system, the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN; Reference Wing, Babor and BrughaWing et al, 1990) in its survey format (SCAN-SF) is now being administered by lay interviewers, following a four-week training period (Reference Brugha, Nienhuis and BagchiBrugha et al, 1999b ). One consequence of the general trend has been a marked divergence between survey assessments and those made when psychiatrists undertake a clinical reappraisal of the same individuals (Reference Anthony, Folstein and RomanowskiAnthony et al, 1985; Reference Helzer, Robins and McEvoyHelzer et al, 1985; Reference Kendler, Gallagher and AbelsonKendler et al, 1996). This lack of congruence strongly suggests that reliance on fully-standardised survey techniques has not in fact settled the question of diagnostic validity.

What steps can be taken so that psychiatric field-studies may in future contribute more usefully to health-service planning and provision? A number of different strategies have been proposed, though it is as yet unclear what impact each of these would be likely to have in practice.

Selecting the right measures of morbidity

A remarkable feature of research in this field over the past 20 years has been its abiding preoccupation with lifetime measures of morbidity, despite their inaccuracy and the attendant problems of interpretation (Reference ParkerParker, 1987). Kessler (Reference Kessler1999) points out, for instance, that the focus of almost all CIDI-based surveys to date has been on lifetime prevalence, and that to get reliable one-year estimates in the planned WMH 2000 project, a special modification of this instrument will be required. Inflation of reporting rates might therefore be most simply reduced by dispensing with lifetime measures and concentrating on point- and period-prevalence. Since, however, the comments of Regier et al (Reference Regier, Kaelber and Rae1998) already mentioned refer explicitly to one-year estimates, this improvement alone would not resolve the basic problem.

Identifying clinical syndromes in community populations

Psychiatric symptoms reported by field-study respondents can be analysed to see how frequently they occur in combinations resembling the distinctive syndromes of hospital-treated patients, and hence to gauge the confidence with which individuals can be assigned to diagnostic categories such as schizophrenia, depressive disorder or anxiety state. This approach, exemplified by the ‘Index of Definition’, which Wing and his colleagues developed in conjunction with the PSE-CATEGO classification system (Reference Wing, Mann and LeffWing et al, 1978), has yielded fairly conservative prevalence estimates in community surveys (Reference BebbingtonBebbington, 1990). Diagnostic confidence cannot, however, be equated with clinical severity or need for treatment. Indeed, Wing (Reference Wing1976) was careful to stress that non-specific symptoms such as worrying, sleeplessness and irritability, although not of much help in differential diagnosis, may be very severe in terms of personal distress or social impairment. Hence, an undeniable need for medical treatment can exist among many persons who score below threshold on the Index of Definition.

Adding criteria for severity and functional impairment

Regier et al (Reference Regier, Kaelber and Rae1998), arguing that self-reported symptoms alone are inadequate as case criteria, recommend that these should be supplemented by data on illness severity and duration, comorbidity and associated functional impairment. If such variables, as well as the clinical symptoms of illness, could be rated reliably at a single interview, this would constitute a real advance in public-health knowledge. There are, however, no grounds a priori to suppose that such information is any simpler to obtain than that relating to diagnosis. In particular the rating of functional impairments and disabilities, which lies at the core of individual needs-assessment, calls for detailed inquiry and, in respect of those with severe mental illness, for questioning of health care professionals or other informants (Reference Brewin and WingBrewin & Wing, 1993; Reference Phelan, Slade and ThornicroftPhelan et al, 1995). The proposal thus raises questions with regard to research manpower and costs, as well as to respondents' adherence.

Two-stage procedures of case ascertainment

The solution preferred by Dohrenwend (Reference Dohrenwend1990) is a two-stage procedure, in which initial screening by means of a short rating scale is followed by a more detailed examination of all persons with abnormal scores, to weed out ‘false positives’. This approach, in his view:

“…capitalizes on the ability of a psychometric instrument to provide reliable measurement over the full range of dimensions of psychopathology and on the ability of a clinical examination to provide reliable diagnoses in groups where the types of symptomatology are not rare” (Reference DohrenwendDohrenwend, 1990).

The point is well taken, and indeed the two-stage approach is now firmly established in psychiatric epidemiology. One cannot, however, assume that it will sharpen the focus on persons with the greatest need for treatment. In the Israeli survey that Dohrenwend cites as an example, nearly half a cohort of adults were rated positively at first-stage screening, and after interview by psychiatrists using a short form of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS-L) about one-fifth were judged to meet Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for “a disorder at the definite level” (Reference Levav, Kohn and DohrenwendLevav et al, 1993). Making allowance for age differences, this result is not much different from those of the ECA and NCS projects. Even community surveys in which a more rigorous second-stage examination such as SCAN was used have indicated levels of treatment need in populations on a scale that could not be met by specialist services (Reference Bebbington, Marsden and BrewinBebbington et al, 1997).

Restoring a place for clinical judgement

It is not generally appreciated how far most of the standardised interviews have diverged from clinical ‘case taking’ methods in psychiatry. As set out, for example, in the Maudsley Handbook of Practical Psychiatry (Reference GoldbergGoldberg, 1997), these do not include systematic checking of a symptom inventory, but start from the presenting condition, and proceed to assessment of the patient's mental status from his or her appearance and general behaviour, talk, mood, attention and thought content; any evidence suggesting abnormal beliefs, interpretation of events or experiences referred to one's environment, body or person, and the cognitive status in terms of memory, orientation and general knowledge.

A number of the earlier semi-structured interviews, including the Mental Status Schedule (Reference Spitzer, Fleiss and BurdockSpitzer et al, 1964) and the Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS; Reference Goldberg, Cooper and EastwoodGoldberg et al, 1970) did respect the need for reported symptoms and abnormalities observed at interview to be rated separately, but in the 1980s this distinction was largely abandoned as reliance on lay interviewers increased. Thus, a revision of the CIS at that time was undertaken on the grounds that:

“…ratings requiring clinical judgement add little information to those based on self-report, may be less reliable and may lead to the biassed assessment of anxiety and depression” (Reference Lewis and WilliamsLewis & Williams, 1989).

This conclusion, being based not on psychiatric material, but on various samples (general practice patients, antenatal clinical attenders, people with diabetes and Home Office civil servants) whose mean symptom scores were all low, cannot be generalised to cases of serious mental disorder. There is now growing agreement that manifest abnormalities should also be rated, although not as to the best way of going about it. Brugha et al (Reference Brugha, Bebbington and Jenkins1999a ), from their experience with SCAN, argue that diagnostic validity can be ensured in large-scale surveys if non-medical interviewers are trained to make basic clinical judgements, but how successfully this can be achieved within a brief period is as yet unclear. Kessler (Reference Kessler1999), working with the CIDI, has proposed a number of strategies including use of audio-tapes and correction of interview ratings by means of comparison with a so-far undefined clinical ‘gold standard’.

COVERAGE OF SEVERE MENTAL DISORDERS

Community prevalence surveys have proved most effective in the enumeration of common, non-psychotic conditions; notably, anxiety and depressive states and their congeners among adults, learning disability and behavioural disorders in school-age children and cognitive impairment in the elderly. Their contribution to the epidemiology of psychotic illness, in contrast, has been distinctly modest. Low base rates in the general population, together with problems of non-response and difficulties in contacting high-risk groups of homeless persons and transients, dictate that case numbers will be small, and frequency estimates unreliable, except in the most ambitious projects. In area surveys listed by Goldberg & Huxley (Reference Goldberg and Huxley1992) as having employed standardised diagnostic interviews, some 250 cases of functional psychosis (schizophrenia, paranoid states and bipolar affective disorders) were found among a total of 23 000 respondents who could be fully examined, the mean one-month prevalence rate being 11 per 1000. Such low case-yields imply that community surveys are an uneconomical way to study the psychoses, unless their detection is in effect a ‘spin off’ from the enumeration of common, non-psychotic syndromes - in which case prevalence estimates are likely to be inaccurate.

One way of resolving the dilemma might be to undertake a first-stage screening targeted on the psychoses, and thus to reduce the numbers requiring a full diagnostic assessment. The British Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (Reference Jenkins, Bebbington and BrughaJenkins et al, 1998) employed in its household sampling survey a battery of items from the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ), as well as collecting information from key informants (community mental health teams and general practitioners (GPs)) on persons with psychotic illness known to them. On previous testing the PSQ had performed extremely well, with a sensitivity of 96.9% and a specificity of 95.3% (Reference Bebbington and NayaniBebbington & Nayani, 1995). Since, however, the positive predictive value of a test is directly correlated with prevalence of the target group, it will tend to be low for rare disorders. To find each case of psychosis, assuming the screen maintained its performance level under field-study conditions, an average of six screen-positives would have to be examined if the true prevalence were 1%, but many more given the one-year prevalence of 4.4 per 1000 estimated in the household survey, and since many of these cases were identified by respondents' self-reports of mental illness or anti-psychotic medication, rather than by screening for psychotic symptoms (H. Meltzer, personal communication, 1999). Such a result might still signify advantage over a single-stage design (Reference Shrout and NewmanShrout & Newman, 1989) in a project concerned only with psychoses, but as in practice these disorders are almost always studied jointly with more common conditions, comparing the relative efficiency of one- and two-stage survey designs becomes a complex task.

Much larger case numbers, and hence more robust and reliable prevalence estimates, have come from key-informant studies of cases of psychosis known to local service agencies; whether only the specialist services covered by cumulative case registers (Reference Freeman and AlpertFreeman & Alpert, 1986), or all relevant medical and social agencies co-opted as information sources in ad hoc surveys (Reference Jeffreys, Harvey and McNaughtJeffreys et al, 1997). Informant surveys relying on second-hand data cannot, however, resolve all questions of method. Problems of clinical validity will remain, while a variable proportion of cases present in the population will be unknown to, or out of contact with, all service agencies, and identifiable only with the aid of special searches (Reference Von Korff, Nestadt and RomanskiVon Korff et al, 1985).

A systematic attempt to overcome these difficulties was made as part of the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing, in which a key informant study of ‘low prevalence’ disorders (i.e. functional psychoses) in four widely separated, predominantly urban areas was carried out independently of the main community-sample survey (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 1999). All individuals in contact with ‘main-stream’ mental health services were examined using an instrument (Diagnostic Interview for Psychosis; Reference Jablensky, McGrath and HerrmanJablensky et al, 1999) specially developed for the survey and, in addition, persons with suspected psychotic illness were screened by the same technique in hostels, boarding houses, drop-in centres, refuges and shelters, as well as by office psychiatrists and GPs who agreed to participate.

According to the recent report (Reference Jablensky, McGrath and HerrmanJablensky et al, 1999), the prevalence of psychotic illness among adults in these areas lay in the range 4-7 per 1000 with a weighted mean of 4.7 per 1000, 60% of cases being accounted for by schizophrenia. Of a total of 980 persons in contact with service agencies during the census month, 26.7% were identified in psychiatric in-patient units, 43.4% in psychiatric out-patient departments or community clinics, 8.9% by private psychiatrists, 8.8% by GPs, and 12.2% in hostels, shelters for the homeless or other ‘marginal’ sites. An additional 146 persons were identified who were known to services but had not made contact in the census month. Since the parallel nationwide community survey employed both a different sampling method and different instruments, and moreover did not attempt to cover all diagnostic categories (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 1998), it is hardly possible to estimate what proportion of ‘low prevalence’ disorders could have been picked up by direct population screening. How the results of these different research strategies can best be combined to yield integrated prevalence estimates for both low-prevalence and common mental disorders remains an open question.

IMPLICATIONS OF GENUINELY HIGH PSYCHIATRIC PREVALENCE

Many who agree with Regier and his colleagues, that psychiatric field studies should in future pay closer regard to the realities of health care provision and unmet need, may baulk at the implied corollary that research findings should be constrained by the strait-jacket of ‘medical necessity’ as this is interpreted in health insurance schemes and managed care programmes. Epidemiology, after all, is a public health discipline, not an instrument of cost-containment. Distress and functional impairment are widespread among psychologically-disturbed individuals who would not satisfy the criteria imposed to justify specialist treatment costs, yet none the less require medical help.

A better basis for service planning may be a needs-assessment approach in which the appropriate care-packages can be specified in terms of psychiatric in-patient or extra-mural treatment, community support, primary health care, or some combination of these. The hierarchy of ‘levels and filters’ proposed by Goldberg & Huxley (Reference Goldberg and Huxley1992) derives from a model in which referral to specialist services is usually via the GP, and the term filter refers to each of the selective stages at which a person in the community (Level 1), consults his or her doctor (Level 2), the doctor recognises a mental disturbance (Level 3), the patient may then be referred to a psychiatric agency (Level 4) and, if necessary, admitted to in-patient care (Level 5).

That a majority of psychiatric disorders at any point in time will be under the sole care of GPs was demonstrated in the UK over 30 years ago (Reference Shepherd, Cooper and BrownShepherd et al, 1966), and has since been substantiated in many other Western countries (Reference Cooper and EastwoodCooper & Eastwood, 1992; Reference Üstün and SartoriusÜstün & Sartorius, 1995). Goldberg & Huxley (Reference Goldberg and Huxley1992), using compiled research data, estimated the one-year prevalence of ‘conspicuous’ cases (i.e. those identified by doctors) as 101.5 per 1000, and contrasted this rate with the 23.5 per 1000 referred to mental health services. Even in the USA, general practice forms a major part of the de facto mental health system, providing care each year for an estimated 43% of people with mental or addictive disorders compared with 40% treated by specialist agencies (Reference Regier, Narrow and RaeRegier et al, 1993). High total prevalence estimates may thus have stronger implications for the primary health care system than for specialist services. Treatment and management of most common mental disorders is best regarded as an integral part of the continuing, whole-patient care provided by the general practice team.

If many cases diagnosed in community surveys were actually transient, homoeostatic reactions, as suggested by Regier et al (Reference Regier, Kaelber and Rae1998), one would expect to find these mainly represented at the lower levels of the model. Goldberg & Huxley (Reference Goldberg and Huxley1992) noted inferential evidence for a high rate of short-term, apparently spontaneous remission in both community and general practice studies, but commented that this might be partly explained by false-positive results at initial screening. Longer-term follow-up is in any case necessary to examine the patterns of temporary remission and recurrence among persons who are vulnerable to minor stress.

Cooper et al (Reference Cooper, Fry and Kalton1969), in a general-practice study, found that of patients who consulted with new psychiatric episodes during one year, a steady proportion presented with similar complaints in each of the five subsequent years. Forty-eight per cent were treated for psychiatric illness in two or more of these five years, and only 28% remained apparently well for the whole period. This proportion represented in effect the maximum for new cases with complete and lasting remission, since some of the patients concerned might have experienced further episodes that were not seen, or not diagnosed, by the practice doctors. Discarding 28% of the Goldberg & Huxley ‘conspicuous morbidity’ estimate would still leave an annual rate of 73 patients per 1000 with chronic or recurrent conditions, of whom on average only one-third would be referred to mental health agencies each year. In addition, a substantial proportion of unrecognised Level 2 cases might benefit from appropriate treatment, although it seems that improving medical recognition of psychological disorders does not by itself ensure a better outcome (Reference Tiemens, Ormel and SimonTiemens et al, 1996).

DISCUSSION

Although the justification for descriptive prevalence surveys of mental illness must ultimately rest on their utility for health care policy and planning, third-generation survey researchers in their pursuit of formal reliability have long marched to a different drummer. A careful reappraisal is now called for to achieve a better balance of priorities in the future. Before embarking on new large-scale projects, it would seem prudent at least to explore ways of ensuring greater clinical and public health relevance for the research findings. The various proposals for improving case-assessment procedures outlined above deserve systematic testing.

Yet refinement of the standardised techniques alone may not be sufficient: the underlying assumption that mental health care needs can be satisfactorily gauged by community surveys, designed to net all ICD- or DSM-classified conditions, is itself open to challenge. The complementary roles of population-based case registers, key-informant surveys and general practice studies should also be carefully assessed. If service agency collaboration can be most readily secured at local area level, not least because of data protection requirements, a question arises with regard to the relative merits of national population surveys and alternative research designs, such as investigation of selected, contrasting area populations. The latter approach, already employed in the ECA Study and the Australian low prevalence survey, has the potential advantage that it can provide a starting point for population-based evaluative research.

To link epidemiology to controlled intervention studies an appropriate service frame-work is necessary, and in many societies the most promising is that of primary health care. But because the milder forms of mental disorder fit a dimensional model at least as readily as a categorical one (Reference Goldberg and HuxleyGoldberg & Huxley, 1992), a rigid dichotomy into cases and normals could here be inappropriate, and even detrimental in situations where health insurers are seeking to ‘carve out’ the mental health component from general medical cover. If a monaxial disease classification cannot do justice to the close intertwining of physical and mental ill-health so often encountered in general practice (Reference CrombieCrombie, 1963), the working definition of prevalence may have to be adapted to suit a bi- or multi-axial classification of health problems. That, however, is a matter to be decided on clinical and scientific grounds, not simply in conformity with managed care plans.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful to Scott Henderson and Ailsa Korten, Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Centre, Canberra, for supplying material from the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing, and Howard Meltzer, Office for National Statistics Social Survey Division, London, for information on the British National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Anthony, J. C., Folstein, M., Romanowski, A. J., et al (1985) Comparison of the lay diagnostic interview schedule and a standardized psychiatric diagosis: experience in Eastern Baltimore. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 667675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (1998) Mental Health and Wellbeing Profile of Adults. Canberra: ABS.Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (1999) People with Psychotic Illness – A Report from the National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing, Australia 1997–1998. Report 3. Canberra: ABS.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. E. (1990) Population surveys of psychiatric disorder and the need for treatment. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 3340.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. E. & Nayani, T. (1995) The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 1119.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. E., Marsden, L. & Brewin, C. R. (1997) The need for psychiatric treatment in the general population: the Camberwell Needs for Care Survey. Psychological Medicine, 27, 821834.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. & Wing, J. K. (1993) The MRC Needs for Care Assessment: progress and controversies. Psychological Medicine, 23, 837841.Google Scholar
Brugha, T. S., Bebbington, P. & Jenkins, R. (1999) A difference that matters: comparisons of structured and semi-structured psychiatric diagnostic interviews in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 29, 10131020.Google Scholar
Brugha, T. S., Nienhuis, F. J., Bagchi, D., et al (1999) The survey form of SCAN: the feasibility of using experienced lay survey interviewers to administer a semi-structured systematic clinical assessment of psychotic and non-psychotic disorders. Psychological Medicine, 29, 703711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, B., Fry, J. & Kalton, G. (1969) A longitudinal study of psychiatric morbidity in a general practice population. British Journal of Preventive and Social Medicine, 23, 210217.Google Scholar
Cooper, B., & Eastwood, R. (eds) (1992) Primary Health Care and Psychiatric Epidemiology. London: Tavistock Google Scholar
Crombie, D. L. (1963) The Procrustean bed of medical nomenclature. Lancet, i, 12051206.Google Scholar
Dohrenwend, B. P. (1990) “The problem of validity in field studies of psychological disorders” revisited. Psychological Medicine, 20, 195208.Google Scholar
Dohrenwend, B. P. & Dohrenwend, B. S. (1974) Social and cultural influences on psychopathology. Annual Review of Psychology, 25, 417452.Google Scholar
Ford, W. E. (1998) Medical necessity: its impact in managed mental health care. Psychiatric Services, 49, 183184.Google Scholar
Freeman, H. & Alpert, M. (1986) Prevalence of schizophrenia in an urban population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 603611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, D., (ed.) (1997) The Maudsley Handbook of Practical Psychiatry. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, D., Cooper, B., Eastwood, M. R., et al (1970) A standardised psychiatric interview for use in community surveys. British Journal of Preventive and Social Medicine, 24, 1823.Google ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D. & Huxley, P. (1992) Common Mental Disorders: a Bio-Social Model. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Helzer, J. E., Robins, L. N., McEvoy, L. T., et al (1985) A comparison of clinical and diagnostic interview schedule diagnoses. Physician re-examination of lay-interviewed cases in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 657666.Google Scholar
Jablensky, A., McGrath, J., Herrman, H., et al (1999) People Living with Psychotic Illness: An Australian Study. National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing, Report 4. Canberra: Department of Health and Aged Care.Google Scholar
Jeffreys, S. E., Harvey, C. A., McNaught, A. S., et al (1997) The Hampstead Schizophrenia Survey 1991. I: Prevalence and service use comparisons in an inner London health authority, 1986–1991. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 301306.Google Scholar
Jenkins, R., Bebbington, P., Brugha, T. S., et al (1998) British psychiatric morbidity survey. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 47.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Gallagher, T. J., Abelson, J. M., et al (1996) Lifetime prevalence, demographic risk factors, and diagnostic validity of non-affective psychosis as assessed in a US community sample. The National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 10221031.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C. (1999) The World Health Organization International Consortium on Psychiatric Epidemiology (ICPE): initial work and future directions. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 99, 29.Google Scholar
Levav, I., Kohn, R., Dohrenwend, B. P., et al (1993) An epidemiological study of mental disorders in a 10-year cohort of young adults in Israel. Psychological Medicine, 23, 691707.Google Scholar
Lewis, G. & Williams, P. (1989) Clinical judgement and the standardized interview in psychiatry. Psychological Medicine, 19, 971979.Google Scholar
Lewis, G., Pelosi, A. J., Araya, R., et al (1992) Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychological Medicine, 22, 465486.Google Scholar
National Advisory Mental Health Council (1993) Health care reform for Americans with severe mental illnesses. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 14471465.Google Scholar
Parker, G. (1987) Are lifetime prevalence estimates in the ECA study accurate? Psychological Medicine, 17, 275282.Google Scholar
Phelan, M., Slade, M., Thornicroft, G., et al (1995) The Camberwell Assessment of Need: the validity and reliability of an instrument to assess the needs of people with severe mental illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 589595.Google Scholar
Regier, D. A., Narrow, W. E. & Rae, D. S. (1993) The de facto US mental and addictive disorders service system: Epidemiologic Catchment Area prospective one-year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 8594.Google Scholar
Regier, D. A., Kaelber, C. I., Rae, D. S., et al (1998) Limitations of diagnostic criteria and assessment instruments for mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 55, 109115.Google Scholar
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Croughlan, J., et al (1981) The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 381389.Google Scholar
Robins, L. N., Wing, J. K., Wittchen, H. U., et al (1988) The Composite International Diagnostic Interview. An epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 10691077.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodgers, B. & Mann, S. A. (1986) The reliability and validity of PSE assessment by lay interviewers: a national population survey. Psychological Medicine, 16, 689700.Google Scholar
Shepherd, M., Cooper, B., Brown, A. C., et al (1966) Psychiatric Illness in General Practice. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shrout, P. E. & Newman, S. C. (1989) Design of two phase prevalence surveys of rare disorders. Biometrics, 45, 549555.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Fleiss, J. L. & Burdock, E. L., et al (1964) The Mental Status Schedule: rationale, reliability and validity. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 5, 384395.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., Fleiss, J. L., et al (1970) The Psychiatric Status Schedule: a technique for evaluating psychopathology and impairment in role functioning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 23, 4155.Google Scholar
Tiemens, B. G., Ormel, J. & Simon, G. E. (1996) Occurrence, recognition and outcome of psychological disorders in primary care. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 636644.Google Scholar
Üstün, T. B. & Sartorius, N. (eds) (1995) Mental Illness in General Health Care. An International Study. Chichester: J. Wiley.Google Scholar
Von Korff, M., Nestadt, G., Romanski, A., et al (1985) Prevalence of treated and untreated DSM–III schizophrenia. Results of a two-stage community survey. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 577581.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K. (1976) A technique for studying psychiatric morbidity in in-patient and out-patient series and in general population samples. Psychological Medicine, 6, 665671.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974) The Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Mann, S. A., Leff, J. P., et al (1978) The concept of a ‘case’ in psychiatric population surveys. Psychological Medicine, 8, 203217.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Bebbington, P. E. & Robins, L. N. (1981) What is a Case? The Problem of Definition in Community Surveys. London: McIntyre.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Babor, T., Brugha, T., et al (1990) SCAN: Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 589593.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1993) The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.