A strong theoretical and empirical basis supports the adoption of a life-course framework to explain health and disease in later life. Reference Ben-Shlomo and Kuh1 Evidence implicates early life and childhood exposures in metabolic disorders, cardiovascular disease, psychopathology and premature death in adulthood. Reference Barker2–Reference Kelly-Irving, Lepage, Dedieu, Bartley, Blane and Grosclaude7 The long-term effects of early life exposures, such as poor diet during gestation, on later life cardiovascular and metabolic outcomes are well established. Reference Barker2 Disease susceptibility that stems from experiences of abuse, neglect and material deprivation during childhood and consequent mortality have also been studied. Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman4–Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz and Edwards9 Further, poor parenting has been associated with adverse outcomes in childhood and adolescence including dysregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, Reference Luecken and Lemery10–Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim12 obesity, Reference Sleddens, Gerards, Thijs, de Vries and Kremers13 poor academic achievement, maladaptation, problem behaviour and aggression Reference Chan and Koo14,Reference Kawabata, Alink, Tseng, van IJzendoorn and Crick15 as well as adult psychopathology. Reference Weich, Patterson, Shaw and Stewart-Brown16 Despite the potential importance of parenting as a life-course determinant of health and disease, the link between experiences of parenting in childhood and survival in later life, where most of deaths occur, has yet to be investigated. We studied whether experiences of parenting style in childhood are associated with survival in a national sample of older people. Since parenting style influences the entire period from birth to adolescence, which spans the most sensitive periods of cognitive, behavioural and socioemotional development, we hypothesised that poor parenting style characterised by lack of affection and care and overprotection and lack of autonomy Reference Parker, Tupling and Brown17 would be associated with increased risk of all-cause and cause-specific mortality.
Method
Study population
The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is a prospective observational study of community-dwelling people aged 50 years and over in England. At baseline, in 2002–2003, the sample comprised 11 391 core members and was nationally representative. Follow-up interviews took place biennially after the baseline. A one-off life-history survey that gathered retrospective information about the experiences and life circumstances of the participants, from birth to age 50 years, took place in 2007 as an add-on to the second follow-up interview. The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is approved by the National Research Ethics Service (MREC/01/2/91) and all participants have provided informed consent. A detailed description of the study can be found at: http://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/.
For the needs of our study, we used data from both the main study and the life-history survey. Because of the potential importance of generational differences in experiences of parenting style and the inability to distinguish between age and cohort effects in our data, we confined our analysis to participants aged 65 to 79 years in 2007. To eliminate the possibility of experiences of individuals who were reared by a single parent or non-natural parents confounding our results, we excluded participants who did not report on the parenting style of both natural parents. Of the 7535 core members who had participated in the second follow-up interview, 6199 participated in the life-history survey of which 2744 were aged 65 to 79 years. The analytic sample comprised 1964 participants, after the exclusion of those who did not complete the childhood experiences questionnaire (n = 312); were not reared by both natural parents or did not report on the parenting style of both natural parents (n = 253); or had missing values in any of the variables used in the analysis (n = 215). Non-response to the life-history survey was associated with lower socioeconomic position, poorer health, not living alone and non-White ethnicity.
Assessment of perceived parenting style
Parenting style was measured using the seven-item Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) during the life-history survey in 2007. Reference Parker, Tupling and Brown17,Reference Todd, Boyce, Heath and Martin18 The PBI is one of the most widely used measures of parenting style. Conceptually, it draws on the works of Bowlby and Rutter on the importance of mothering and the bond between a parent and a child and was created to measure the parental contribution to a parent–child bond. Reference Parker, Tupling and Brown17 The PBI is designed to retrospectively assess adults' perceptions of their parents' parenting style and examine two fundamental dimensions of parenting, care and overprotection. Reference Parker, Tupling and Brown17 In our study, the PBI referred to the period when the participants were aged 15 years or younger. The seven-item PBI includes three care items (i.e. understood my problems and worries, emotionally cold mother/father, and made me feel not wanted) and four overprotection items (i.e. let me do things I liked, liked me to make decisions, made me feel dependent on them and were overprotective). The four-point response scale ranges from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. According to the PBI scoring system, optimal parenting is characterised by high care and low overprotection scores. The full 25-item PBI has good psychometric properties Reference Parker19 and demonstrates good stability over extended time periods in non-clinical samples. Reference Murphy, Wickramaratne and Weissman20 The psychometric properties of the seven-item PBI are comparable with those of the full 25-item PBI. Reference Todd, Boyce, Heath and Martin18 The PBI has been used to predict psychopathology; parental coldness and low levels of parental care have been shown to be consistently associated with depression and other mental health conditions in a non-specific manner. Reference Enns, Cox and Clara21–Reference Yap, Pilkington, Ryan and Jorm23 Parental overprotection is also associated with mental health problems, Reference Enns, Cox and Clara21,Reference Yap, Pilkington, Ryan and Jorm23 but evidence on this association is less consistent. Reference Long, Aggen, Gardner and Kendler22
Parenting style scores were separately derived for the mother and father. They ranged from 0 (poorest parenting style: lowest levels of care and highest levels of overprotection) to 21 (optimal parenting style: highest levels of care and lowest levels of overprotection). The overall parenting style score, which was derived by aggregating the maternal and paternal parenting style scores, ranged from 0 (poorest parenting style) to 42 (optimal parenting style). Data on the distributions of the overall parenting style score and subscale scores are presented in online Table DS1. To avoid unnecessarily excluding individuals with few missing values, we substitute up to one missing value per subscale per parent with the respective mean score. Thus, the maternal and paternal parenting style scores included participants with up to two imputed values and the overall parenting style score included participants with up to four imputed values. Because the imputed and observed parenting style data produced directly comparable results, we used the former in the main analysis and present the analysis that is based on the latter in online Table DS2). To explore whether the association between parenting style and mortality was dose–response we categorised the scores into quartiles.
Mortality
Mortality data were obtained from the Office for National Statistics central registry for all consenting participants (approximately 95.5% of the sample, http://www.ons.gov.uk/). Information on causes of death was available up to February 2013. The ICD-10 was used to classify deaths. 24 Deaths with ICD-10 codes from C00 to C97 were classified as cancer deaths and deaths with ICD-10 codes from I00 to I99 as cardiovascular deaths. All remaining deaths were classified as ‘other’.
Covariates
Age, gender and measures of self-reported childhood socioeconomic position were used as confounders. Marital status, parenthood status, measures of adult socioeconomic position, and negative (i.e. elevated depressive symptoms that were defined as ⩾4 symptoms on the eight-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale Reference Radloff25 ) and positive affect (i.e. tertiles of the five-item pleasure subscale score of the 19-item CASP (control, autonomy, self-realisation, pleasure) scale, Reference Hyde, Wiggins, Higgs and Blane26 which included items like ‘On balance, I look back on my life with a sense of happiness’ and ‘I enjoy the things that I do’) were also used as confounders. Categories of body mass index (BMI, weight (kg)/height2 (m2)) and waist circumference (cm), smoking (never a smoker, ex-smoker, current smoker), physical activity on a weekly basis (not at all, mild, moderate, vigorous), memory (tertiles of the summary score of immediate and delayed 10-word recall), social support (low v. high) from partner, children, relatives and friends, and number of problems with social relationships were used as potential mediators. All covariates were measured in 2006–2007, except for BMI and waist circumference, which were measured in 2004–2005.
Statistical analysis
We calculated mortality rates according to quartiles of parenting style scores. We estimated Cox proportional hazards regression models of the associations between quartiles of the parenting style score and all-cause and cause-specific mortality. Survival time (in months) was the time that elapsed from the date of life-history interview, in 2007, to the first of either the date of death or censoring i.e. February 2013. We used the pooled sample to estimate the models as there was no significant interaction by gender. The models were initially adjusted for age and gender, then for childhood socioeconomic position, and then in addition for marital and parenthood statuses, adult socioeconomic position, positive and negative affect, obesity, unhealthy behaviours, cognitive function and social factors. We used the same methodology and sample to estimate models of the associations between maternal and paternal parenting style scores and all-cause mortality. In supplementary analyses (Table DS3), we also estimated models of the associations between parental care and overprotection scores and all-cause mortality. We ascertained that the proportionality assumption was met using survival plots and the Schoenfeld residuals test. To ascertain that our results are not driven by adverse childhood experiences and childhood health problems, in supplementary analyses, we additionally adjusted our models for self-reported adverse childhood experiences including parental mental health and addiction problems (Table DS4) and self-reported childhood health problems that are known to affect parenting (i.e. epilepsy, asthma, diabetes, hearing problems, limiting disability and emotional, nervous or psychiatric problems) (Table DS5). In additional supplementary analyses, we explored baseline self-reported comorbidities (i.e. heart disease, stroke, cancer, psychiatric problems and chronic lung disease) as potential confounders (Table DS6) and inflammatory markers (i.e. fibrinogen and high sensitivity C-reactive protein) that were measured in 2004–2005 as potential mediators (Table DS7). Further, to minimise the chances of reverse causality (i.e. proximity to the death influencing parenting style reports), we estimated models that excluded all deaths that occurred within the first 24 months after the baseline in 2007 (Table DS8).
Results
Participants who reported being raised with a poor parenting style (i.e. those who were in the poorest parenting style score quartile) were more likely to be female and slightly younger, to live in a household with fewer books at age 10 years, to be less happy and report more depressive symptoms and problems with social relationships and less social support compared with those who reported being raised with a good parenting style (i.e. those who were in the optimal parenting style score quartile) (Table 1).
Parenting style score quartiles a | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Optimal (n = 498) |
Second optimal (n = 449) |
Second poorest (n = 485) |
Poorest (n = 532) |
P b | |
Age, mean (s.d.) | 71.6 (4.1) | 71.8 (4.1) | 71.4 (4.2) | 71.0 (4.1) | 0.010 |
Male, n (%) | 246 (49.4) | 205 (45.7) | 247 (50.9) | 219 (41.2) | 0.009 |
Non-married, n (%) | 136 (27.3) | 133 (29.6) | 162 (33.4) | 183 (34.4) | 0.054 |
Childless (without any children), n (%) | 48 (9.6) | 42 (9.4) | 55 (11.3) | 55 (10.3) | 0.75 |
No educational qualifications, n (%) | 156 (31.3) | 137 (30.5) | 180 (37.1) | 178 (33.5) | 0.65 |
Lowest total net household wealth tertile (<£143,000), n (%) | 131 (26.3) | 124 (27.6) | 127 (26.2) | 162 (30.5) | 0.21 |
First ever residence was rented, c n (%) | 362 (74.0) | 297 (70.1) | 351 (74.4) | 365 (71.0) | 0.36 |
⩽10 books at the household at age 10 years, c n (%) | 114 (24.1) | 112 (26.4) | 149 (31.8) | 189 (37.1) | <0.001 |
Father/main carer in manual occupation or unemployed at age 14 years, n (%) |
158 (31.7) | 156 (34.7) | 157 (32.4) | 173 (32.5) | 0.42 |
Current smoker, n (%) | 57 (11.5) | 46 (10.2) | 43 (8.9) | 60 (11.3) | 0.21 |
Physically inactive, n (%) | 31 (6.2) | 30 (6.7) | 34 (7.0) | 40 (7.5) | 0.13 |
Body mass index ⩾30kg/m2, c n (%) | 114 (25.9) | 107 (27.0) | 113 (26.6) | 150 (32.1) | 0.51 |
Waist circumference ⩾102cm (in men)/⩾88cm (in women), c n (%) | 233 (52.5) | 218 (54.0) | 220 (50.8) | 253 (52.9) | 0.74 |
Lowest tertile of cognitive function (⩽8 recalled words), n (%) | 153 (30.7) | 143 (31.9) | 150 (30.9) | 166 (31.2) | 0.67 |
Low social support, c n (%) | 103 (21.5) | 152 (35.2) | 159 (34.2) | 211 (42.9) | <0.001 |
Problems with more than one type of social relationships, c n (%) | 30 (6.3) | 46 (10.7) | 48 (10.4) | 98 (20.0) | <0.001 |
Elevated depressive symptoms (⩾4 symptoms), n (%) | 39 (7.8) | 49 (10.9) | 61 (12.6) | 91 (17.1) | <0.001 |
Lowest positive affect score tertile (positive affect score ⩽12), c n (%) | 74 (16.0) | 92 (21.9) | 125 (27.2) | 162 (33.8) | <0.001 |
a. Parenting style score ranged from 0 (poorest style) to 42 (optimal style). Optimal quartile: score >34; second optimal quartile: score from >31 to ⩽34; second poorest quartile: score from >27 to ⩽31; and poorest quartile: score ⩽27.
b. P-values are generated using chi-square, Kruskal-Wallis and analysis of variance tests for categorical, ordinal and continuous covariates, respectively.
c. The calculation of estimates and P-values was based only on participants with valid values and did not include the category of missing values.
A total of 243 deaths were observed over a mean follow-up of 5.4 years (median 5.7 years). In the unadjusted Cox model, people in the poorest parenting style score quartile had a 50% higher risk of death compared with those in the optimal parenting style score quartile (Table 2). Age exerted a negative confounding effect and adjustment for it increased the strength of the association. Adjustment for childhood socioeconomic position explained a part of the association. In the fully adjusted model, participants in the poorest parenting style score quartiles had a 49% higher risk of death compared with those in the optimal parenting style score quartile. The associations between parenting style and cancer and other mortality were also strong and significant (Table 2). Full adjustment for covariates explained the association between parenting style and cancer mortality, but not the association between parenting style and other mortality. Parenting style was not associated with cardiovascular mortality. Maternal and paternal parenting style scores were also inversely associated with all-cause mortality (Table 3). These associations were explained after adjustment for all covariates.
Parenting style score quartiles b | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Optimal | Second optimal | Second poorest | Poorest | |
All-cause mortality | ||||
Deaths, n | 48 | 51 | 69 | 75 |
Deaths/1000 person-years (95% CI) | 17.7 (13.3–23.4) | 20.9 (15.9–27.5) | 26.5 (20.9–33.6) | 26.5 (21.1–33.2) |
Model 1, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.18 (0.80–1.75) | 1.50 (1.04–2.17) | 1.50 (1.05–2.16) |
Model 2, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.20 (0.81–1.77) | 1.54 (1.07–2.23) | 1.72 (1.20–2.48) |
Model 3, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.13 (0.76–1.68) | 1.45 (1.00–2.10) | 1.59 (1.10–2.30) |
Model 4, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.16 (0.78–1.74) | 1.43 (0.98–2.09) | 1.49 (1.02–2.18) |
Cardiovascular mortality | ||||
Deaths, n | 17 | 13 | 18 | 18 |
Deaths/1000 person-years (95% CI) | 6.3 (3.9–10.1) | 5.3 (3.1–9.2) | 6.9 (4.4–11.0) | 6.4 (4.0–10.1) |
Model 1, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 0.85 (0.41–1.75) | 1.11 (0.57–2.15) | 1.02 (0.53–1.98) |
Model 2, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 0.85 (0.41–1.74) | 1.16 (0.60–2.25) | 1.21 (0.62–2.36) |
Model 3, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 0.78 (0.38–1.62) | 1.05 (0.54–2.06) | 1.04 (0.53–2.04) |
Model 4, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 0.82 (0.38–1.75) | 1.16 (0.58–2.33) | 1.03 (0.51–2.10) |
Cancer mortality | ||||
Deaths, n | 21 | 24 | 28 | 35 |
Deaths/1000 person-years (95% CI) | 7.7 (5.0–11.9) | 9.8 (6.6–14.7) | 10.8 (7.4–15.6) | 12.4 (8.9–17.2) |
Model 1, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.27 (0.71–2.28) | 1.39 (0.79–2.46) | 1.60 (0.93–2.75) |
Model 2, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.29 (0.72–2.32) | 1.41 (0.81–2.49) | 1.77 (1.03–3.05) |
Model 3, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.21 (0.67–2.19) | 1.39 (0.79–2.45) | 1.73 (1.00–2.99) |
Model 4, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.15 (0.64–2.10) | 1.25 (0.70–2.22) | 1.47 (0.84–2.59) |
Other mortality | ||||
Deaths, n | 10 | 14 | 23 | 22 |
Deaths/1000 person-years (95% CI) | 3.7 (2.0–6.8) | 5.7 (3.4–9.7) | 8.8 (5.9–13.3) | 7.8 (5.1–11.8) |
Model 1, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.56 (0.69–3.51) | 2.41 (1.15–5.06) | 2.12 (1.00–4.47) |
Model 2, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.59 (0.70–3.54) | 2.47 (1.19–5.23) | 2.48 (1.17–5.25) |
Model 3, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.56 (0.69–3.53) | 2.27 (1.08–4.78) | 2.26 (1.06–4.81) |
Model 4, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.69 (0.73–3.94) | 2.36 (1.08–5.18) | 2.46 (1.10–5.45) |
Sample sizes | ||||
Participants, n | 498 | 449 | 485 | 532 |
Person-years of follow-up | 2719 | 2443 | 2602 | 2838 |
a. Model 1: this is the unadjusted association; model 2: adjusted for age and gender; model 3 as model 2, plus adjustment for childhood socioeconomic position (i.e. ownership of the first-ever permanent residence, number of books in the household at age 10 years, and father's or main carer's occupational class at age 14 years); model 4: as model 3, plus adjustment for adult socioeconomic position (i.e. education and total net household wealth), marital status, parenthood status, obesity (i.e. body mass index and waist circumference), cognitive function, unhealthy behaviours (i.e. smoking and physical activity), social factors (i.e. social support and number of problems with social relationships), elevated depressive symptoms and positive affect.
b. Parenting style score ranged from 0 (poorest style) to 42 (optimal style). Optimal quartile: score >34; second optimal quartile: score from >31 to ⩽34; second poorest quartile: score from >27 to ⩽31; and poorest quartile: score ⩽27.
Parenting style score quartiles b | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Optimal | Second optimal | Second poorest | Poorest | |
Maternal parenting style score | ||||
All-cause mortality | ||||
Deaths, n | 58 | 64 | 54 | 67 |
Deaths/1000 person-years (95% CI) | 19.6 (15.2–25.4) | 21.0 (16.5–26.9) | 25.1 (19.2–32.8) | 27.4 (21.6–34.9) |
Model 1, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.07 (0.75–1.53) | 1.28 (0.88–1.85) | 1.40 (0.99–1.99) |
Model 2, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.01 (0.71–1.44) | 1.23 (0.85–1.79) | 1.56 (1.10–2.23) |
Model 3, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 0.98 (0.68–1.39) | 1.18 (0.81–1.71) | 1.48 (1.04–2.11) |
Model 4, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.04 (0.73–1.50) | 1.16 (0.79–1.70) | 1.41 (0.97–2.04) |
Sample sizes | ||||
Participants, n | 544 | 561 | 398 | 461 |
Person years of follow-up | 2956 | 3045 | 2153 | 2441 |
Paternal parenting style score | ||||
All-cause mortality | ||||
Deaths, n | 51 | 58 | 60 | 74 |
Deaths/1000 person-years (95% CI) | 19.6 (14.9–25.8) | 21.6 (16.7–27.9) | 23.8 (18.5–30.6) | 26.6 (21.2–33.4) |
Model 1, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.10 (0.76–1.61) | 1.22 (0.84–1.76) | 1.36 (0.95–1.94) |
Model 2, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.19 (0.81–1.73) | 1.24 (0.85–1.80) | 1.57 (1.10–2.25) |
Model 3, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.14 (0.78–1.67) | 1.15 (0.79–1.68) | 1.46 (1.02–2.10) |
Model 4, HR (95% CI) | 1.00 (reference) | 1.21 (0.82–1.78) | 1.08 (0.74–1.59) | 1.38 (0.95–2.00) |
Sample sizes | ||||
Participants, n | 477 | 499 | 464 | 524 |
Person-years of follow-up | 2602 | 2686 | 2523 | 2784 |
a. Model 1: this is the unadjusted association; Model 2: adjusted for age and gender; Model 3 as model 2, plus adjustment for childhood socioeconomic position (i.e. ownership of the first-ever permanent residence, number of books in the household at age 10 years, and father's or main carer's occupational class at age 14 years); model 4: as model 3, plus adjustment for adult socioeconomic position (i.e. education and total net household wealth), marital status, parenthood status, obesity (i.e. body mass index and waist circumference), cognitive function, unhealthy behaviours (i.e. smoking and physical activity), social factors (i.e. social support and number of problems with social relationships), elevated depressive symptoms and positive affect.
b. Both maternal and paternal parenting style scores ranged from 0 (poorest style) to 21 (optimal style). Optimal quartile: score ⩾18; second optimal quartile: score from ⩾16 to <18; second poorest quartile: score from ⩾14 to <16; and poorest quartile: score <14.
Supplementary analyses indicated that the associations between parenting style and all-cause and cause-specific mortality were not affected by additional adjustment for adverse childhood experiences, childhood health problems and baseline comorbidities, except for a modest effect of parental psychopathology on the association between parenting style and other mortality. They also indicated that fibrinogen, but not C-reactive protein, explained a small part of the association between parenting style and all-cause mortality. Sensitivity analyses that excluded deaths that occurred within the first 24 months after the baseline produced comparable but stronger results to those of the main analysis.
Discussion
In a national sample of people aged 65 to 79 years we found that experiences of parenting style in childhood were associated with all-cause mortality. This association was inverse and graded. People who reported being raised with a poor parenting style had increased risk of death compared with those who reported being raised with an optimal parenting style. The risk of death was also increased for people who reported being raised with an intermediate parenting style, which was not the poorest, but also fell short of being optimal. Parenting style was also inversely associated with increased risk of cancer and other mortality, but not cardiovascular mortality. Full adjustment for childhood and adult socioeconomic position, and adult demographic, psychosocial, cognitive and behavioural factors partially explained the association between parenting style and all-cause and other mortality and fully the association between parenting style and cancer mortality. Poor maternal and paternal parenting styles, low levels of parental care and high levels of parental overprotection were individually associated with increased risk of death. Notwithstanding any overlap, their effect on the risk of death in later life is likely to be additive as they all appeared to contribute to the association between parenting style and death.
Findings from other studies
To our knowledge, our study is the first to examine the association between parenting style and risk of death in a national sample of older people. Studies have examined parental abuse and neglect, but not parenting style, and mortality mostly in adults younger than 60 years. Reference Brown, Anda, Tiemeier, Felitti, Edwards and Croft6,Reference Kelly-Irving, Lepage, Dedieu, Bartley, Blane and Grosclaude7,Reference Brown, Anda, Felitti, Edwards, Malarcher and Croft27,Reference Lee and White28 Most Reference Kelly-Irving, Lepage, Dedieu, Bartley, Blane and Grosclaude7,Reference Brown, Anda, Felitti, Edwards, Malarcher and Croft27,Reference Lee and White28 but not all Reference Brown, Anda, Tiemeier, Felitti, Edwards and Croft6 of these studies reported a positive association between adverse childhood experiences and the risk of death. This evidence largely concurs with our findings both in terms of direction of causality and magnitude of the associations. A study of a cohort of US male medical students has also generated findings that concur with ours; they found a significant inverse association between the quality of the father–son relationship and the risk of incident cancer. Reference Shaffer, Duszynski and Thomas29
Strengths and limitations
The use of rich longitudinal data and a national sample are obvious strengths of our study. A weakness of our study is its observational design, which makes it vulnerable to residual confounding. Although we were able to adjust our models for several key confounders, it is still possible that unaccounted for confounders influenced to an extent our findings. Supplementary analyses that excluded deaths that occurred up to 24 months after the baseline and adjusted for baseline comorbidities indicated that our findings are unlikely to be an artefact of reverse causality (i.e. scoring lower on the parenting style scale because of a serious health condition at baseline or proximity to death). Further supplementary analyses indicated that poor parenting style is a childhood risk factor that is associated with the risk of death independent of other known childhood risk factors such as childhood diseases and adverse childhood experiences. The observed negative confounding effect of age on the examined associations is not surprising given that older people tend to remember their past more positively than younger people (i.e. positivity effect). Reference Carstensen, Turan, Scheibe, Ram, Ersner-Hershfield and Samanez-Larkin30 To ascertain that our data are not driven by this tendency we adjusted our models for positive affect that likely accounts for it and restricted our analysis to a narrower age range so that our sample represented only one generation.
Our work is vulnerable to measurement bias. The adjustment for positive and negative affect expectedly reduced mood-congruent memory bias (i.e. the influence of baseline mood on parenting style reports). Nevertheless, the retrospective measurement of parenting style, the long lag between the exposure and its measurement, the use of a short seven-item instrument that focused on only two dimensions of parenting and the potentially sensitive nature of perceived parenting style may have influenced our results. Reference Hardt and Rutter31,Reference Susser and Widom32 Our childhood socioeconomic position measures have been proven to be good predictors of health outcomes such as mortality and morbidity, but they were also retrospectively measured and thus potentially subject to bias. Non-response bias could have also influenced our results. Because non-response to the life-history survey and, in particular, to the childhood experiences section of it is likely to be related to poorer childhood experiences, we hypothesise that our findings are a conservative account of the true association between parenting style and mortality. To contribute to a better interpretation of our findings in relation to non-response, we present, in online Table DS9, an analysis of key characteristics of our study according to various non-response categories. Further, the relatively small number of deaths and the short follow-up period reduced the statistical power of our study and did not allow investigation of the long-term effect of parenting style on survival. Finally, the use of a sample of English community-dwellers aged 65 to 79 years reduces the generalisability of our results; their relevance to other cohorts and populations is unknown.
Interpretation of our findings
The inverse association between parenting style and all-cause, cancer and other mortality lends support to the idea that parenting style is associated with disease and survival at older ages in a systematic way. Parenting style can affect general susceptibility to disease, but our focus on mortality and not the initiation of pathological processes and incidence of disease does not allow for more definite conclusions on this issue. Our findings suggest that poor parenting style qualifies as a risk factor for cancer and thus could inform the debate about the relationship between psychosocial factors and cancer mortality. Reference Chida, Hamer, Wardle and Steptoe33 Further, our findings indicate that deaths from respiratory and external causes might be related to experiences of suboptimal parenting, but the lack of appropriate data precluded a more detailed analysis of this. The lack of an association between poor parenting style and cardiovascular death is unexpected given the importance of stress for cardiovascular disease and the formative role of negative childhood experiences for HPA-axis dysregulation and chronic stress. Reference Luecken and Lemery10–Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim12 Notwithstanding the chance of a statistical artefact, a possible explanation for this finding is that the damaging effect of poor parenting on the circulatory system might be alleviated by the action of protective factors operating at later stages of the life course, for example the use of medication for high cholesterol or blood pressure. This was illustrated in a cohort of older Finnish adults, where parental separation in early childhood was associated with the use of cardiovascular medication, but not cardiovascular mortality. Reference Alastalo, Räikkönen, Pesonen, Osmond, Barker and Heinonen34
Our data indicate that suboptimal parenting style in childhood might be associated with mortality in old age via multiple mechanisms and pathways. Participants' affective and social problems and unhealthy behaviours appeared to be relevant. Parental and family characteristics are also expected to be pertinent to the examined associations. Childhood socioeconomic position partially explained the associations, but our analyses suggest that poor parenting style does not necessarily cluster with socioeconomic adversity. Based on the importance of parental psychopathology and personality for parenting, Reference Kendler, Sham and MacLean35 it is reasonable to assume the existence of mechanisms that link parental psychopathology and personality with offspring's increased risk of mortality at older ages via parenting. Although we lacked detailed data on parental health and personality, in supplementary analyses, we were able to adjust for retrospectively measured self-reported parental mental health and addiction problems up to age 15 years. This additional adjustment did not appear to affect the observed associations, except for the association between parenting style and other mortality, which was modestly attenuated. These findings seem to suggest that parental psychopathology is relevant to the association between parenting style and other mortality, which includes deaths from respiratory and external causes including accidents and suicides.
Our findings presuppose the existence of biological mediators, Reference Rutter36 which induce modifications in the physiology, that, via multiple pathways, lead to increased risk of death at older ages. Epigenetic modifications caused by poor parenting in childhood are a candidate mediator. Reference Champagne and Curley37 A recent study of leukocyte DNA in healthy adults found that a lower score on the PBI care subscale was associated with increased cytosine methylation in the promoter region of the glucocorticoid receptor gene NR3C1. Reference Tyrka, Price, Marsit, Walters and Carpenter38 Telomere shortening, a marker of cellular ageing, may also be a mediator given its association with early life adversity and parenting. Reference Asok, Bernard, Roth, Rosen and Dozier39 Poor parenting is associated with stress in childhood and the dysregulation of the HPA axis, Reference Luecken and Lemery10–Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim12 which can affect the thyroid, growth and gonadal axes function, Reference Charmandari, Kino, Souvatzoglou and Chrousos40 the development and function of the central nervous system, and cognitive and emotional functioning Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim12,Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli41 and thus have long-term implications for adult health. Reference Miller, Chen and Parker3,Reference Carroll, Gruenewald, Taylor, Janicki-Deverts, Matthews and Seeman5 Inflammation processes and the dysregulation of the immune system are also associated with early life stress and could also mediate the observed associations. Reference Miller, Chen and Parker3,Reference Charmandari, Kino, Souvatzoglou and Chrousos40 In our supplementary analyses, baseline fibrinogen, but not C-reactive protein, explained a small part of the association between parenting style and all-cause mortality. This limited effect is an indication that inflammation might be relevant to the examined associations. Finally, earlier age at menarche and risky sexual behaviours in daughters, which are risk factors for reproductive cancer in women, are also associated with an unstable childhood environment and poor parenting Reference Belsky42 and could mediate the observed associations. Reference Milne, Judge, Preen and Weinstein43
Directions for future research
Our findings are novel and need to be replicated in other older cohorts. Future research should examine the association between parenting style and mortality risk using a longer follow-up and a greater number of events. This would result in a more precise measurement of hazard ratios and confidence intervals. The use of parenting style data that have been collected in childhood would be a crucial addition to this research as such data are less susceptible to recall bias. To our knowledge this is unlikely to happen in the near future in the UK because of the lack of suitable data in UK cohorts older than 60 years.
Future research should also examine the associations between parenting style and incidence of disease to gain a better understanding of poor parenting as a factor affecting general susceptibility to disease. Further, emphasis should be placed on the identification of the biological mediators and life-course mechanisms involved in this association. Finally, it is important to identify potential generational differences in this association. The only way to achieve that is to examine how generalisable are our findings to younger generations once they reach the age of our sample.
Implications
Our study indicates that childhood experiences of poor parenting style have long-term implications for survival in later life. They extend current understanding about how childhood experiences might influence health and survival at older ages because they refer to the entire population of children and their everyday experiences and not just children with severe adverse experiences. The potentially modifiable nature of suboptimal parenting style as a risk factor enhances the importance and relevance of our findings for prevention and early life-intervention strategies.
Funding
The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is supported by the National Institute on Aging (Grants and ) and a consortium of the UK government departments coordinated by the Office for National Statistics. A.S. is a British Heart Foundation professor. The funding bodies had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.