Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T17:19:58.002Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mental health of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities in Britain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Eric Emerson*
Affiliation:
Institute for Health Research, Lancaster University, Lancaster
Chris Hatton
Affiliation:
Institute for Health Research, Lancaster University, Lancaster
*
Professor Eric Emerson, Institute for Health Research, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Few studies have employed formal diagnostic criteria to determine the prevalence of psychiatric disorders in contemporaneous samples of children with and without intellectual disabilities.

Aims

To establish the prevalence of psychiatric disorders against ICD—10 criteria among children with and without intellectual disabilities, the association with social/environmental risk factors, and risk attributable to intellectual disability.

Method

Secondary analysis of the 1999 and 2004 Office for National Statistics surveys of the mental health of British children and adolescents with (n=641) and without (n = 17774) intellectual disability.

Results

Prevalence of psychiatric disorders was 36% among children with intellectual disability and 8% among children without (OR=6.5). Children with intellectual disabilities accounted for 14% of all British children with a diagnosable psychiatric disorder Increased prevalence was particularly marked for autistic-spectrum disorder (OR=33.4), hyperkinesis (OR=8.4) and conduct disorders (OR=5.7). Cumulative risk of exposure to social disadvantage was associated with increased prevalence.

Conclusions

A significant proportion of the elevated risk for psychopathology among children with intellectual disability may be due to their increased rate of exposure to psychosocial disadvantage.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2007 

Several well-constructed community-based population studies suggest that 35-40% of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities Footnote 1 are likely to have a diagnosable psychiatric disorder (Reference Rutter, Tizard and YuleRutter et al, 1976; Reference Einfeld and TongeEinfeld & Tonge, 1996; Reference Linna, Piha and KumpulainenLinna et al, 1999; Reference Stromme and DisethStromme & Diseth, 2000; Reference Dekker, Koot and van-der-EndeDekker et al, 2002; Reference Dekker and KootDekker & Koot, 2003; Reference EmersonEmerson, 2003). However, few studies have either employed formal diagnostic criteria to examine prevalence rates of specific psychiatric disorders in contemporaneous samples of children with and without intellectual disability, or have investigated the direction and strength of association between personal/social/environmental factors and risk of psychiatric disorders for children with and without intellectual disabilities.

The aims of this study were: (a) to establish the prevalence of diagnosable psychiatric disorders against ICD–10 criteria (World Health Organization, 1993) among British children with and without intellectual disabilities; (b) to assess the association between exposure to psychosocial disadvantage and presence of psychiatric disorders in children with and without intellectual disabilities; (c) to estimate the extent to which elevated risk for psychiatric disorders among children with intellectual disabilities may be accounted for by elevated rates of exposure to psychosocial disadvantage.

METHOD

Sample

The present study involved secondary analysis of data collected in the 1999 and 2004 Office for National Statistics (ONS) surveys of the mental health of British children and adolescents (Reference Meltzer, Gatward and GoodmanMeltzer et al, 2000; Reference Green, McGinnity and MeltzerGreen et al, 2005). Data files were obtained from the UK Data Archive (http://www.data-archive.ac.uk). The 1999 survey collected information on 10 438 children between 5 and 15 years of age, which represented 83% of the target sample (Reference Meltzer, Gatward and GoodmanMeltzer et al, 2000). The 2004 survey collected information on 7977 children between 5 and 16 years of age, which represented 76% of the target sample (Reference Green, McGinnity and MeltzerGreen et al, 2005). Both samples were stratified by age and gender within postcode sectors in England, Scotland and Wales. Comparison of the results from the 1999 and 2004 surveys has failed to reveal any meaningful changes in the prevalence of psychiatric disorders among the total sample of young people between these two time points (Reference Green, McGinnity and MeltzerGreen et al, 2005). In addition, our preliminary analyses indicated no significant variation across samples with regard to the hypotheses being examined. As a result, analyses were undertaken on the combined sample of 18 415 children.

Procedure

The surveys used identical procedures for the collection of information, the identification of psychiatric disorders and the collection of information on child and family demographics and functioning. Information was collected by computer-assisted face-to-face personal interview with the child's primary carer (in 94% of cases the child's mother) and, wherever possible, with children aged 11 years or over. If consent was obtained from the child's primary carer, information was also collected by postal questionnaire from the child's teacher. Teacher information was available for 72% of the achieved sample. Children for whom teachers did not provide information were more likely to be supported by a lone parent (27 v. 22%; χ2=64.0, d.f.=1, P<0.001; OR=1.35), more likely to be living in income poverty (36 v. 28%; χ2=87.0, d.f.=1, P<0.001; OR=1.41) and more likely to be living in families with poorer family functioning (20 v. 18%; χ2=16.9, d.f.=1, P<0.001; OR=1.19).

Measures

The presence of psychiatric disorders among the children and adolescents was identified through the use of the Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA; Reference Goodman, Ford and RichardsGoodman et al, 2000). This consists of two structured interviews (one undertaken with the child's primary carer and the other, for children aged 11 years or more, with the child), a questionnaire used with the child's teacher and a computer-assisted diagnostic rating system that provides diagnoses against DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and ICD–10 criteria. The time frame (period prevalence) for DAWBA questions is the previous month unless ICD–10 diagnostic criteria specify a minimum period for the duration for symptoms (e.g. 6 months for generalised anxiety disorder). The DAWBA has been shown to discriminate well between samples of children drawn from population-based child benefit registers and from those attending child and adolescent mental health services, have good convergent validity with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Reference GoodmanGoodman, 1999), predict contact with health services and prognosis, and possess acceptable levels of agreement with diagnoses derived from case-note review (Reference Goodman, Ford and RichardsGoodman et al, 2000). It has not, however, been validated on children with intellectual disabilities.

In addition, information was also collected in both 1999 and 2004 on indicators of family socio-economic position (occupation, income, education), life events, parental mental health using the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ–12; Reference Goldberg and WilliamsGoldberg & Williams, 1988), family functioning using the General Functioning Scale of the MacMaster Family Activity Device (Reference Miller, Epstein and BishopMiller et al, 1985) and teacher ratings of child academic attainment. Income data were equivalised using the modified Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) scale (Department of Work and Pensions, 2007). Income poverty was defined as living in a household whose equivalised income was less that 60% of the national median for the sampled year.

Identifying children with intellectual disabilities

Following preliminary analysis we identified children and adolescents as having intellectual disabilities if one of the following conditions was met.

  1. (a) The child's primary carer reported that the child had ‘learning difficulties’ and the child's teacher reported that either they had marked difficulty in all three areas of scholastic attainment assessed (reading, maths, spelling) or their estimated developmental quotient (DQ) fell two or more standard deviations below the sample average. Child DQ was calculated by dividing the child's mental age (as estimated by their teacher) by chronological age.

  2. (b) The child's primary carer did not report that the child had ‘learning difficulties’ but the child's teacher reported that they had marked difficulty in all three areas of scholastic attainment assessed and their DQ fell two or more standard deviations below the average DQ.

  3. (c) No information was available from the child's teacher but the child's primary carer reported that the child had ‘learning difficulties’ and that they had been concerned about the child's speech development in the first 3 years of life.

This approach identified 641 children (3.5% of the total sample) as having intellectual disabilities and 17 774 children as not having intellectual disabilities. Of the children with intellectual disabilities, 395 (62%) were identified by combined parental and teacher report, 71 (11%) by teacher report and 175 (27%) by parental report. Children with intellectual disabilities were significantly more likely to be male (66 v. 50%, χ2=61.9, d.f.=1, P<0.001; OR=1.93). There were no differences between the two groups with regard to age (mean age 10.1 years) or ethnicity (90% White).

RESULTS

Prevalence of psychiatric disorders

The point prevalence of psychiatric disorders for children and adolescents with and without intellectual disabilities is shown in Table 1 for all disorders with a prevalence of approximately 1% or greater for either group. Prevalence rates were higher among children with intellectual disabilities for 27 of the 28 comparisons and statistically significantly elevated (P<0.01) for 20 of the 28 comparisons. Children with intellectual disabilities accounted for 14% of all British children with a diagnosable psychiatric disorder.

Table 1 Point prevalence of psychiatric disorders among children and adolescents with and without intellectual disabilities 1

Point prevalence, % Odds ratio (95% CI)
With intellectual disability Without intellectual disability
Any psychiatric disorder 36.0 8.0 6.5 (5.4–7.7) ***
Any emotional disorder 12.0 3.7 3.6 (2.8–4.6) ***
Any anxiety disorder 11.4 3.2 3.9 (3.0–5.0) ***
   Separation anxiety 2.7 0.6 4.9 (2.9–8.3) ***
   Specific phobia 2.0 0.8 2.4 (1.4–4.3) **
   Social phobia 0.9 0.3 3.3 (1.4–7.7) **
   Panic disorder 0.2 0.2 1.0 (0.1–7.3)
   Agoraphobia 0.2 0.1 1.7 (0.2–13.1)
   Post-traumatic stress disorder 0.5 0.2 3.1 (0.9–10.2)
   Obsessive–compulsive disorder 0.2 0.2 0.7 (0.1–5.1)
   Generalised anxiety disorder 1.6 0.6 2.5 (1.3–4.9) **
   Other anxiety disorder 4.4 0.9 4.8 (3.2–7.2) ***
Any depressive disorder 1.4 0.9 1.7 (0.8–3.3)
   Depressive episode 0.9 0.6 1.5 (0.7–3.4)
   Other depressive episode 0.5 0.2 2.1 (0.7–7.0)
Hyperkinesis (ADHD) 8.3 0.9 8.4 (6.1–11.5) ***
Any conduct disorder 20.5 4.3 5.7 (4.6–7.0) ***
   Oppositional defiant disorder 11.1 2.3 5.3 (4.1–6.9) ***
   Unsocialised conduct disorder 1.9 0.4 4.9 (2.8–8.5) ***
   Socialised conduct disorder 1.3 0.9 2.1 (1.2–3.8) **
   Other conduct disorder 5.2 0.5 10.5 (7.0–15.7) ***
Autistic-spectrum disorder 8.0 0.3 33.4 (22.3–50.2) ***
Tic disorder 0.8 0.2 5.2 (2.0–13.5) **
Eating disorder 0.2 0.1 1.3 (0.2–9.4)
Emotional disorder + conduct disorder 4.4 0.8 5.8 (3.8–8.8) ***
Conduct disorder + ADHD 5.8 0.6 9.4 (6.5–13.8) ***
Emotional disorder + ADHD 1.3 0.1 9.8 (4.4–21.9) ***
Emotional disorder + conduct disorder + ADHD 0.8 0.1 8.7 (3.2–23.9) ***

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

* P<0.05

** P<0.01

*** P<0.001

1. Those with missing data were excluded from analyses

Associated social and environmental factors

Associations between gender, age and eight social/environmental variables and risk of having the three most common categories of psychiatric disorder (conduct disorder, emotional disorder including anxiety disorder, and hyperkinesis) are presented in Table 2 for children with and without intellectual disabilities. For emotional disorders the direction of effect is identical across the two groups for all potential risk factors. In addition, there is close correspondence in the strength of effect for eight of the ten variables. For conduct disorders the direction of effect is identical across the two groups for all potential risk factors. There is close correspondence in the strength of effect for four of the ten variables. For hyperkinesis, the direction of effect is identical across the two groups for eight of the ten variables, with close correspondence in the strength of effect for one variable.

Table 2 Association between personal, social and environmental variables and risk of emotional disorder, conduct disorder and hyperkinesis among children with and without intellectual disabilities 1

With intellectual disability Without intellectual disability
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Emotional disorder
   Male gender 0.7 0.4–1.1 0.8 0.7–0.9 **
   Age 11–16 years 1.8 1.1–3.0 * 1.9 1.6–2.2 ***
   Lone parent family 2.4 1.5–3.9 *** 2.2 1.9–2.6 ***
   Income poverty 2.2 1.3–3.9 ** 2.2 1.9–2.6 ***
   Exposure to two or more negative life events 3.2 1.9–5.2 *** 3.3 2.8–3.8 ***
   Poor family functioning 2 2.5 1.5–4.1 ** 2.2 1.9–2.6 ***
   Primary carer has no educational qualifications 1.8 1.1–3.0 * 2.0 1.7–2.4 ***
   Household with no paid employment 2.3 1.4–3.7 ** 2.5 2.1–3.0 ***
   Mother with potential mental health disorder 3 2.5 1.5–4.1 ** 3.6 3.1–4.3 ***
   Maternal self-rated physical health less than ‘good’ 1.5 0.8–2.6 5.3 4.4–6.5 ***
Conduct disorder
   Male gender 2.3 1.5–3.7 *** 2.1 1.8–2.4 ***
   Age 11–16 years 1.2 0.8–1.8 1.4 1.2–1.7 ***
   Lone-parent family 2.3 1.5–3.4 *** 2.4 2.1–2.8 ***
   Income poverty 1.7 1.1–2.5 * 2.9 2.5–3.4 ***
   Exposure to two or more negative life events 2.1 1.4–3.1 *** 3.4 2.9–3.9 ***
   Poor family functioning 2 1.7 1.1–2.5 * 3.8 3.2–4.4 ***
   Primary carer has no educational qualifications 2.2 1.5–3.3 *** 2.4 2.0–2.8 ***
   Household with no paid employment 2.0 1.3–2.9 ** 3.2 2.7–3.7 ***
   Mother with potential mental health disorder 3 2.0 1.3–3.0 ** 3.4 3.0–4.0 ***
   Maternal self-rated physical health less than ‘good’ 1.8 1.1–2.8 * 3.0 2.4–3.7 ***
Hyperkinesis
   Male gender 3.2 1.5–6.9 ** 6.5 4.3–9.8 ***
   Age 5–10 years 2.0 1.1–3.6 * 0.9 0.7–1.2
   Lone parent family 1.3 0.7–2.4 2.1 1.6–2.8 ***
   Income poverty 0.9 0.5–1.6 2.1 1.6–2.9 ***
   Exposure to two or more negative life events 1.6 0.9–2.9 2.9 2.2–3.9 ***
   Poor family functioning 2 1.4 0.8–2.5 2.7 2.0–3.6 ***
   Primary carer has no educational qualifications 1.6 0.9–2.8 1.9 1.4–2.6 ***
   Household with no paid employment 1.1 0.6–2.0 2.7 2.0–3.7 ***
   Mother with potential mental health disorder 3 1.2 0.7–2.2 2.7 2.0–3.6 ***
   Maternal self-rated physical health less than ‘good’ 1.4 0.7–2.6 3.2 2.2–4.8 ***

1. Those with missing data were excluded from analyses

2. Family scores above cut-off (> 2) on the General Functioning Scale of the MacMaster Family Assessment Device

3. Mother scores above cut-off (> 2) on the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ–12)

* P < 0.05

** P < 0.01

*** P < 0.001

A cumulative social risk index was derived from the eight potential social/environmental risk factors by counting the number of potential risk factors to which each child was exposed. The association between the cumulative social risk index and prevalence of emotional disorders, conduct disorders and hyperkinesis is shown in Fig. 1. Rank order correlations between cumulative social risk and prevalence were 1.0 (P<0.001) for emotional disorders and for conduct disorders for children with and without intellectual disability, 0.93 (P=0.008) for hyperkinesis among children with intellectual disability and 0.97 (P<0.001) for hyperkinesis among children without intellectual disability. Although visual inspection of the data suggested a stronger association between cumulative social risk and prevalence among children with intellectual disabilities, post hoc tests for interaction effects (using a logistic regression model) were not significant.

Fig. 1 Association between cumulative social risk and prevalence of emotional disorder, conduct disorder and hyperkinesis among children with () and without () intellectual disabilities.

Rates of exposure to potential social and environmental risk factors

Given the evidence that risk of emotional disorders, conduct disorders and hyperkinesis was associated with potential social/environmental risk factors for both groups of children, we explored between-group rates of exposure to these potential risk factors (Table 3). Exposure to all eight indicators of potential social/environmental risk was significantly higher among children with intellectual disabilities (P<0.001).

Table 3 Exposure of children with and without intellectual disabilities to social and environmental risk factors 1

% OR (95% CI)
With intellectual disability Without intellectual disability
Lone parent family 30 23 1.4 (1.2–1.7) ***
Income poverty 47 30 2.1 (1.8–2.5) ***
Exposure to two or more negative life events 37 24 1.9 (1.6–2.2) ***
Poor family functioning 2 27 18 1.7 (1.4–2.0) ***
Primary carer has no educational qualifications 38 20 2.5 (2.1–3.0) ***
Household with no paid employment 30 14 2.6 (2.2–3.1) ***
Mother with potential mental health disorder 3 33 24 1.6 (1.3–1.9) ***
Maternal self-rated physical health less than ‘good’ 20 6 3.8 (3.1–4.7) ***
Exposure to three or more potential risk factors 46 24 2.6 (2.2–3.1) ***

1. Those with missing data were excluded from analyses

2. Family scores above cut-off on the General Functioning Scale of the MacMaster Family Assessment Device

3. Mother scores above cut-off on the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ–12)

* P<0.05

** P<0.01

*** P<0.001

Estimating risk after controlling for between-group differences in social/environmental risk factors

Finally we estimated the extent to which intellectual disability represented a risk factor for psychiatric disorder after controlling for the marked between-group differences in exposure to potential social/environmental risk factors. We used binary logistic regression to estimate the corrected odds ratio for associated psychiatric disorder after controlling for between-group differences in age, gender and the eight potential social/environmental risk factors (Table 4). Variables were entered in two blocks (block 1 comprising the variables related to the child's intellectual disability, gender and age and block 2 the eight potential social/environmental risk factors in a forward conditional stepwise model; P variable entry <0.05, P variable exit >0.1).

Table 4 Association between intellectual disability and psychiatric disorder before and after controlling for between-group differences in exposure to potential social/environmental risks (n=15 900) 1

Variable OR 95% CI P
Any emotional disorder: Block 1 2
   Intellectual disability 3.59 (2.68–4.80) <0.001
   Male gender 0.78 (0.67–0.92) 0.003
   Age 0.91 (0.89–0.93) <0.001
Any emotional disorder: Block 2 3
   Intellectual disability 2.28 (1.67–3.12) <0.001
   Male gender 0.77 (0.65–0.92) 0.003
   Age 0.93 (0.90–0.95) <0.001
   Poor maternal physical health 3.15 (2.54–3.90) <0.001
   Poor maternal mental health 2.43 (2.04–2.89) <0.001
   Exposure to two or more adverse life events 2.12 (1.78–2.52) <0.001
   Poor family functioning 1.47 (1.22–1.77) <0.001
   Mother has no educational qualifications 1.42 (1.17–1.72) <0.001
   Household with no paid employment 1.40 (1.14–1.72) 0.002
Any conduct disorder: Block 1 4
   Intellectual disability 5.68 (4.51–7.15) <0.001
   Male gender 2.05 (1.75–2.40) <0.001
   Age 0.93 (0.91–0.95) <0.001
Any conduct disorder: Block 2 5
   Intellectual disability 3.88 (3.03–5.00) <0.001
   Male gender 2.14 (1.82–2.52) <0.001
   Age 0.95 (0.93–0.98) <0.001
   Poor family functioning 2.25 (1.92–2.65) <0.001
   Poor maternal mental health 2.07 (1.76–2.43) <0.001
   Exposure to two or more adverse life events 2.04 (1.74–2.40) <0.001
   Poor maternal physical health 1.83 (1.47–2.29) <0.001
   Poverty 1.49 (1.24–1.80) <0.001
   Mother has no educational qualifications 1.49 (1.25–1.78) <0.001
   Household without paid employment 1.44 (1.17–1.77) <0.001
Hyperkinesis: Block 1 6
   Intellectual disability 8.20 (5.90–11.46) <0.001
   Male gender 5.73 (3.89–8.44) <0.001
   Age 1.00 (0.96–1.04) NS
Hyperkinesis: Block 2 7
   Intellectual disability 5.79 (4.08–8.21) <0.001
   Male gender 5.77 (3.81–8.52) <0.001
   Age 1.02 (0.97–1.06) NS
   Exposure to two or more adverse life events 2.02 (1.51–2.69) <0.001
   Poor maternal physical health 1.81 (1.23–2.66) 0.002
   Poor family functioning 1.62 (1.19–2.20) 0.002
   Poor maternal mental health 1.58 (1.18–2.13) 0.002
   Household without paid employment 1.43 (1.02–2.00) 0.038
   Mother has no educational qualifications 1.39 (1.01–1.90) 0.043

NS, not significant

1. Those with missing data were excluded from analyses

2. χ2=116.6, d.f.=3, P<0.001, r 2=0.026

3. χ2=620.9, d.f.=9, P<0.001, r 2=0.137

4. χ2=295.7, d.f.=3, P<0.001, r 2=0.058

5. χ2=928.2, d.f.=10, P<0.001, r 2=0.178

6. χ2=232.8, d.f.=3, P<0.001, r 2=0.108

7. χ2=330.8, d.f.=9, P<0.001, r 2=0.153

Comparing the corrected odds ratio for intellectual disability at blocks 1 and 2 indicates that controlling for between-group differences in exposure to potential social/environmental risk involves a 51% reduction in attributable risk for emotional disorder, a 38% reduction for conduct disorder and a 33% reduction for hyperkinesis.

DISCUSSION

Principal findings

The results of the study indicated that: (a) the prevalence of a wide range of psychiatric disorders was significantly higher among children with intellectual disabilities than among children without: children with intellectual disabilities accounted for 14% of all British children with a diagnosable psychiatric disorder; (b) increased prevalence rates were particularly marked for autistic-spectrum disorder, hyperkinesis and any conduct disorders (the latter accounting for approximately two-thirds of all diagnoses among children with intellectual disabilities); (c) cumulative risk of exposure to social disadvantage was associated with increased prevalence rates for any emotional disorder, any conduct disorder and hyperkinesis among children with and without intellectual disabilities; (d) children with intellectual disabilities were at significantly greater risk of exposure to all forms of social disadvantage examined; (e) controlling for these between-group differences in rates of exposure to social disadvantage significantly reduced the increased risk of psychiatric disorders among children with intellectual disabilities.

Strengths and limitations

The main strengths of the present study are that it investigated the prevalence of diagnosable psychiatric disorders against ICD–10 criteria in a large nationally representative sample of British children with and without intellectual disabilities. The main weaknesses of the study lie in: (a) the identification of children with intellectual disability; (b) the use of a measure of psychiatric disorder that has not been validated for use with children with intellectual disabilities; (c) the use of a cross-sectional design.

With regard to the identification of children with intellectual disability, we attempted (wherever possible) to combine parent and teacher report. The overall prevalence rate of intellectual disabilities within the sample (3.5%) is within the bounds reported in population-based epidemiological studies that have included children with mild intellectual disabilities (Reference Leonard and WenLeonard & Wen, 2002). However, the ascertained prevalence rates are slightly higher than the commonly assumed prevalence of intellectual disability (2–3%). It is therefore possible that our operational definition might have led to the inclusion of a small proportion of children with ‘borderline’ intellectual disabilities. It is not possible to predict the impact of this on our results. Confidence in our operational definition is somewhat strengthened by the (expected) association between prevalence and gender and poverty (Reference Leonard and WenLeonard & Wen, 2002). Nevertheless, accuracy in the identification of children would have been significantly strengthened if information had been available with regard to general intellectual functioning and adaptive behaviour. Unfortunately, although British Picture Vocabulary Scale scores were obtained (although not released through the UK Data Archive) for the 1999 cohort, these data were not collected in 2004.

The use of a measure of psychiatric disorder that has not been validated for use with children with intellectual disabilities does represent a threat to the internal validity of the results. There are two main grounds for concern regarding the generalisation of test validity to populations with intellectual disabilities. First, it has been argued that psychiatric disorders may manifest themselves differently among people with intellectual disabilities, and in particular people with more severe intellectual disabilities (Reference DykensDykens, 2000; Reference Wallander, Dekker, Koot and GliddenWallander et al, 2003). For example, recent research has reported overall prevalence rates for psychiatric disorders in an adult population with primarily severe intellectual disabilities of 17% when using ICD–10 criteria and 35% when using criteria specifically developed for use with people with intellectual disabilities (Reference Cooper, Smiley and MorrisonCooper et al, 2007). Notably, however, this discrepancy was primarily attributable to differences in rates of ‘problem behaviours’ identified by the two approaches (0.1 and 19% respectively). Given that the most commonly diagnosed disorder in the present study was conduct disorder (a group of diagnoses that are likely to capture ‘problem behaviours’), such discrepancies may be less likely in studies applying ICD–10 criteria to children. Unfortunately, no data are available at present on the actual correspondence of diagnoses of conduct disorders and the classification of problem or challenging behaviour in children with intellectual disabilities. Second, the identification of psychiatric disorders whose diagnostic criteria require self-report (e.g. obsessive–compulsive disorders) will obviously be problematic among groups who have difficulty in either accessing or reporting on internal states. The consequences of both of these issues for the present study would be to lead to an underestimation of prevalence rates for psychiatric disorders among the subsample of children with intellectual disabilities.

Finally, it must be kept in mind that the results of cross-sectional studies cannot provide evidence of causality. This is particularly relevant to the analyses undertaken of the association between social/environmental factors and the prevalence of psychiatric disorders. These associations might reflect the causal influence of social adversity on psychopathology and, as such, would be consistent with the rapidly growing literature on the social determinants of physical and mental health (Reference Marmot and WilkinsonMarmot & Wilkinson, 2006). They might also reflect the causal influence of child mental health on social adversity (Reference Baker, McIntyre and BlacherBaker et al, 2003), the influence of unmeasured third variables (e.g. genetic factors) on risk of exposure to both social adversity and risk of child psychopathology or possible confounding arising from the operational definition of intellectual disabilities (e.g. low academic attainment or developmental progression being more likely among children with psychiatric disorders).

Implications

The high prevalence rates of psychopathology observed in the present study among children with intellectual disabilities are highly consistent with the results of previous research (Reference Rutter, Tizard and YuleRutter et al, 1976; Reference Einfeld and TongeEinfeld & Tonge, 1996; Reference Linna, Piha and KumpulainenLinna et al, 1999; Reference DykensDykens, 2000; Reference Stromme and DisethStromme & Diseth, 2000; Reference Dekker, Koot and van-der-EndeDekker et al, 2002; Reference Dekker and KootDekker & Koot, 2003; Reference EmersonEmerson, 2003; Reference Wallander, Dekker, Koot and GliddenWallander et al, 2003). These results must be of concern given the evidence that mental health problems have a major negative impact on the well-being, social inclusion and life opportunities of children (Reference Quilgars, Searle, Keung, Bradshaw and MayhewQuilgars et al, 2005). With regard to children with intellectual disabilities, for example, evidence suggests that mental health problems have a negative impact on the well-being of their families, and especially their mothers (Reference Baker, McIntyre and BlacherBaker et al, 2003; Reference Hatton and EmersonHatton & Emerson, 2003), and are likely to lead to out-of-home placements, including the use of high-cost residential educational placements (Reference Llewellyn, McConnell and ThompsonLlewellyn et al, 2005).

Three main factors have been proposed to account for the high rates of psychopathology observed among children with intellectual disabilities (Reference DykensDykens, 2000; Reference Einfeld, Emerson, Rutter, Bishop and PineEinfeld & Emerson, 2007). First, studies undertaken on children in general have provided evidence of an association between lower IQ and psychiatric disorder (Reference GoodmanGoodman, 1995), an association possibly mediated by the role of IQ in determining a child's vulnerability or resilience when faced with adversity (Reference LutharLuthar, 2003). As a result, higher rates of psychopathology would be expected among children with intellectual disabilities given that intellectual impairment is a definitional characteristic of the group. Second, studies undertaken on children in general have also provided evidence of an association between exposure to social disadvantage and increased risk for psychopathology (Reference Green, McGinnity and MeltzerGreen et al, 2005; BMA Board of Science, 2006). Increased rates of psychiatric disorders among children with intellectual disabilities would be predicted, therefore, given that such children are at significantly greater risk of exposure to social disadvantage (Reference Emerson, Graham, Hatton and GliddenEmerson et al, 2006; Reference Emerson and HattonEmerson & Hatton, 2007). Third, the biological bases or sequelae of some syndromes associated with intellectual disability appear to be associated with increased susceptibility to some particular forms of psychopathology (Reference DykensDykens, 2000; Reference Dykens and HodappDykens & Hodapp, 2001; Reference Hodapp, Dykens, Emerson, Hatton and ThompsonHodapp & Dykens, 2004; Reference Einfeld, Emerson, Rutter, Bishop and PineEinfeld & Emerson, 2007).

The results of the present study are consistent with the notion that a potentially socially important proportion of the elevated risk for psychopathology among children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities may be a result of their increased rate of exposure to adverse social conditions (e.g. poverty, less than optimal parenting). Such an interpretation would suggest that approaches to reducing the personal, social and economic costs associated with psychiatric disorders among children with intellectual disabilities should focus on: (a) reducing their exposure to adverse social conditions (BMA Board of Science, 2006); (b) building the resilience of children with intellectual disabilities (and their families) when prevention of exposure to adversity cannot be guaranteed (Reference EmersonEmerson, 2004).

Future research

It is now reasonably well established that intellectual disability is associated with an increased risk for psychopathology (Reference DykensDykens, 2000; Reference Wallander, Dekker, Koot and GliddenWallander et al, 2003; Reference Einfeld, Emerson, Rutter, Bishop and PineEinfeld & Emerson, 2007). Future research needs to identify the relative contribution of (and interplay between) intellectual impairment, social/environmental factors, psychological factors and biological factors to these elevated rates of psychiatric disorders. Addressing this demanding research agenda will require the use of more sophisticated longitudinal and experimental research designs, the validation of existing measures or the development of new measures of psychopathology applicable to children with intellectual disabilities, and the development and use of robust measures of social/environmental risk (Reference Emerson, Graham, Hatton and GliddenEmerson et al, 2006). Exploring the interplay between biological and social factors will also require an increased emphasis on transdisciplinary research that bridges the gap between social epidemiology and behavioural genetics.

Acknowledgements

The analyses were funded by the Foundation for People with Learning Disabilities.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

1 The term ‘intellectual disability’ will be used synonymously with the terms ‘learning disability’ (as used in the UK) and ‘mental retardation’ (as used in the USA and ICD–10).

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). APA.Google Scholar
Baker, B. L., McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., et al (2003) Pre-school children with and without developmental delay: behaviour problems and parenting stress over time. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 217230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
BMA Board of Science (2006) Child and Adolescent Mental Health: A Guide for Healthcare Professionals. British Medical Association.Google Scholar
Cooper, S.-A., Smiley, E., Morrison, J., et al (2007) Mental ill-health in adults with intellectual disabilities: prevalence and associated factors. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 2735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dekker, M. C. & Koot, H. M. (2003) DSM–IV disorders in children with borderline to moderate intellectual disability. I. Prevalence and impact. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 915922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dekker, M. C., Koot, H. M., van-der-Ende, J., et al (2002) Emotional and behavioral problems in children and adolescents with and without intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 10871098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Work and Pensions (2007) Households Below Average Income (HBAI) 1994/5-2005/6 (Revised). Department of Work and Pensions.Google Scholar
Dykens, E. M. (2000) Psychopathology in children with intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 407417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykens, E. M. & Hodapp, R. M. (2001) Research in mental retardation: toward an etiological approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 4971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einfeld, S. & Emerson, E. (2007) Intellectual disability. In Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (eds Rutter, M., Bishop, D., Pine, D., et al, in press). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Einfeld, S. & Tonge, B. J. (1996) Population prevalence of psychopathology in children and adolescents with intellectual disability. I. Epidemiological findings. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 99109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emerson, E. (2003) Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents with and without intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 5158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emerson, E. (2004) Poverty and children with intellectual disabilities in the world's richer countries. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 29, 319337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emerson, E. & Hatton, C. (2007) Contribution of socioeconomic position to health inequalities of British children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 140150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emerson, E., Graham, H. & Hatton, C. (2006) The measurement of poverty and socio-economic position in research involving people with intellectual disability. In International Review of Research in Mental Retardation (ed. Glidden, L. M.), pp. 77108. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, D. & Williams, P. (1988) A Users Guide to the General Health Questionnaire. nfer Nelson.Google Scholar
Goodman, R. (1995) The relationship between normal variation in IQ and common childhood psychopathology: a clinical study. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 4, 187196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1999) The extended version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a guide to child psychiatric caseness and consequent burden. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 791801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R., Ford, T. & Richards, H. (2000) The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 645656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, H., McGinnity, A., Meltzer, H., et al (2005) Mental Health of Children and Young People in Britain, 2004. Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Hatton, C. & Emerson, E. (2003) Families with a person with intellectual disabilities: stress and impact. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 16, 497501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. & Dykens, E. M. (2004) Studying behavioural phenotypes: issues, benefits, challenges. In International Handbook of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities (eds Emerson, E., Hatton, C., Thompson, T., et al), pp. 203220. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, H. & Wen, X. (2002) The epidemiology of mental retardation: challenges and opportunities in the new millennium. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8, 117134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linna, S.-L., Piha, J., Kumpulainen, K., et al (1999) Psychiatric symptoms in children with intellectual disability. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 7782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llewellyn, G., McConnell, D., Thompson, K., et al (2005) Out-of-home placement of school age children with disabilities. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disability, 18, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luthar, S. S. (ed.) (2003) Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marmot, M. & Wilkinson, R. G. (eds) (2006) Social Determinants of Health (2nd edn). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzer, H., Gatward, R., Goodman, R., et al (2000) Mental Health of Children and Adolescents in Great Britain. TSO (The Stationery Office).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, I. W., Epstein, N. B., Bishop, D. S., et al (1985) The MacMaster Family Assessment Device: reliability and validity. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 11, 345356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quilgars, D., Searle, B. & Keung, A. (2005) Mental health and well-being. In The Well-Being of Children in the UK (eds Bradshaw, J. & Mayhew, E.), pp. 134160. Save the Children.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Tizard, J., Yule, W., et al (1976) Isle of Wight studies 1964–1974. Psychological Medicine, 6, 313332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stromme, P. & Diseth, T. H. (2000) Prevalence of psychiatric diagnoses in children with mental retardation: data from a population-based study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42, 266270.Google ScholarPubMed
Wallander, J. L., Dekker, M. C. & Koot, H. M. (2003) Psychopathology in children and adolescents with intellectual disability: measurement, prevalence, course, and risk. In International Review of Research in Mental Retardation (ed. Glidden, L.), pp. 93134. Elsevier.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1993) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Point prevalence of psychiatric disorders among children and adolescents with and without intellectual disabilities1

Figure 1

Table 2 Association between personal, social and environmental variables and risk of emotional disorder, conduct disorder and hyperkinesis among children with and without intellectual disabilities1

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Association between cumulative social risk and prevalence of emotional disorder, conduct disorder and hyperkinesis among children with () and without () intellectual disabilities.

Figure 3

Table 3 Exposure of children with and without intellectual disabilities to social and environmental risk factors1

Figure 4

Table 4 Association between intellectual disability and psychiatric disorder before and after controlling for between-group differences in exposure to potential social/environmental risks (n=15 900)1

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.