Stressful life events have been associated with both unipolar depression and bipolar disorder. Reference Hosang, Korszun, Jones, Jones, Gray and Gunasinghe1–Reference Harris4 Stressful events that have been linked to both disorders include divorce/separation, personal illness, work-related and interpersonal problems. Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott5–Reference Honkalampi, Hintikka, Haatainen, Koivumaa-Honkanen, Tanskanen and Viinamaki9 Examining the role of specific events (for example bereavement v. divorce) in unipolar depression and bipolar disorder may elucidate disorder-specific associations. There are several benefits to identifying whether certain events are strongly linked to unipolar depression and bipolar disorder. First, this would be clinically valuable, the occurrence of a life event known to be depressogenic will be a warning sign that an individual with a mood disorder may be at risk of relapse. Therefore clinicians, the individual and family members will know that additional monitoring, support and/or treatment is required to avoid the development of an illness episode. Second, identification of events associated with episodes of unipolar depression and bipolar disorder will provide an insight into possible mechanisms that explain the relationship between stress and mood disorders. Understanding such mechanisms will inform intervention and treatment strategies. For instance, research shows that humiliating events (rendering a person devalued to themselves or others, such as infidelity of a partner) trigger depressive episodes. Reference Farmer and McGuffin10 In the face of such an event, psychological interventions can focus on processing the humiliating element of the event in an adaptive and positive way.
Comparing the relationship between specific events in unipolar depression and bipolar disorder has not been a focus of research. The available literature indicates that work-related difficulties and failures characterise bipolar disorder, whereas somatic illnesses are more salient for unipolar depression. Reference Bidzińska8 Moreover, there is a difference in the types of stressful events associated with mania and depression in bipolar disorder; with personal illness being linked to bipolar depression, Reference Hall, Dunner, Zeller and Fieve11 whereas bereavement characterises mania. Reference Kessing, Agerbo and Mortensen12 The majority of studies addressing these issues are limited by their small samples, which makes generalising the results problematic. Replication and extension of this work in larger samples is required. Although establishing an association between life stress and mood disorders is important, it is helpful to determine the direction of this association. To do this researchers have distinguished between events that are independent (beyond an individual's control, such as bereavement) and dependent (related to a person's psychopathology and actions, such as being arrested). This distinction provides some insight as to whether the occurrence of a life event is a cause (independent event) or consequence (dependent event) of mood disorders, or other illnesses. Reference Hunt, Bruce-Jones and Silverstone13 Independent events have been consistently associated with the onset of unipolar depression Reference Harris4,Reference Hammen14 and subsequent episodes. Research shows that having one severe independent event is sufficient to trigger an episode of unipolar depression. Reference Hammen14 In terms of bipolar disorder, the evidence is more mixed, with some studies reporting a significant relationship between independent events, bipolar depression Reference Johnson, Cuellar, Ruggero, Winett-Perlman, Goodnick and White15,Reference Beyer, Kuchibhatla, Cassidy and Krishnan16 and mania; Reference Ambelas17 whereas others have failed to detect such a relationship. Reference Horesh and Iancu18–Reference Malkoff-Schwartz, Frank, Anderson, Sherrill, Siegel and Patterson20 It is unclear whether experiencing a stressful event may trigger the illness or episode onset in bipolar disorder and this warrants further investigation. The objectives of this study were fourfold. First, the role of specific events in bipolar disorder and unipolar depression were examined. Second, the pertinence of different events on unipolar depression compared with bipolar disorder was explored. Third, the role of independent and dependent events in unipolar depression and bipolar disorder were investigated. Finally, the salience of independent and dependent life events on bipolar disorder compared with unipolar depression was examined.
Method
Participants
The sample were recruited for two case–control genetic association studies, one focused on unipolar depression Reference Cohen-Woods, Gaysina, Craddock, Farmer, Gray and Gunasinghe21 and the other focused on bipolar disorder. Reference Schosser, Schloegelhofer, Fuchs, Stojanovic, Mossaheb and Kindler22 There were a total of 512 people with bipolar disorder (bipolar group) and 1448 people with unipolar depression (unipolar group). A total of 1346 controls were recruited for both studies but were selected in the present investigation to match the mean age (plus or minus 1 standard deviation) of the bipolar group (26–49 years, n = 612) and unipolar group (24–49 years, n = 679) at the time of their self-defined worst affective episodes. This was to prevent the confounding effect of age at interview, since we previously reported a negative association between age at index period and number of life events in this sample. Reference Hosang, Korszun, Jones, Jones, Gray and Gunasinghe1 The demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1.
The majority of participants in the unipolar group were recruited from out-patient psychiatric clinics in the UK, a smaller proportion were enlisted via self-help groups, general practice and media advertisement, during the years 2001–2004 from London, Birmingham and Cardiff. Individuals were only included if they had experienced two or more major depressive episodes of at least moderate severity that fulfilled operational criteria for recurrent unipolar depression according to the DSM-IV. 23 The exclusion criteria were: intravenous drug dependence, depression occurring only in relation to substance misuse or medical illness, and/or having a personal or family history of mania or schizophrenia (the latter exclusion criterion was so that participants in the unipolar group had a low risk of switching diagnosis, research shows that having a family history of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia are significant risk factors for such diagnostic conversions Reference Holma, Melartin, Holma and Isometsa24 ).
Participants in the bipolar group fulfilled DSM-IV criteria for bipolar I or II disorder, the majority of which were recruited from out-patient psychiatric clinics and the rest were enlisted via UK-based self-help groups and media advertisement between 2004 and 2007. Participants were excluded if their bipolar episodes only occurred in relation to substance misuse or a physical disorder or if they had a personal or family history of schizophrenia (research shows that approximately 20% of individuals initially diagnosed with bipolar disorder have a diagnostic switch to schizophrenia Reference Chen, Swann and Johnson25 and a family history of schizophrenia is a risk factor for this diagnostic switch Reference Jarbin and von Knorring26 ).
Controls drawn from the unipolar depression study were selected from among 34 371 participants originally recruited through general practices in the UK, to the Genetic and Environmental Nature of Emotional States in Siblings study and who fell into the bottom 20% of the distribution on the Sham Composite Index of liability to depression and anxiety (‘G’). Reference Sham, Sterne, Purcell, Cherny, Webster and Rijsdijk27 Those who responded positively to a postal invitation to participate were then contacted by telephone. Controls taken from the bipolar disorder study were recruited via newspaper advertisement and from staff working at King's College London but were not screened using the Sham Composite Index. The exclusion criteria for all controls from both the unipolar depression and bipolar disorder studies was a personal or family (first-degree relatives) history of any psychiatric disorder.
Bipolar group |
Unipolar group |
Controls for comparison with unipolar group |
Controls for comparison with bipolar group |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
n | 512 | 1448 | 679 | 612 |
Women, n (%) | 339 (66) | 1007 (70) | 399 (59) | 357 (58) |
Age at interview, years: mean (s.d.) | 47.95 (11.39) | 47.32 (12.36) | 37.36 (7.93) | 38.72 (7.14) |
Age at worst episode(s), years: mean (s.d.) | ||||
Depression | 37.42 (11.78) | 36.56 (12.27) | – | – |
Hypomania or mania | 37.28 (11.35) | – | – | – |
Age at illness onset, years: mean (s.d.) | 21.80 (18.55) | 23.25 (11.59) | – | – |
Number of lifetime illness episodes, years: mean (s.d.) | ||||
Depression | 12.35 (19.76) | 4.11 (5.00) | – | – |
Hypomania or mania | 11.06 (19.40) | – | – | – |
Diagnosis of bipolar I disorder, n (%) | 460 (90) | – | – | – |
All participants were aged at least 18 years and provided written informed consent. The broad research aims of the studies were explained to the participants (increase knowledge of the causes and factors that make people vulnerable to bipolar disorder/unipolar depression), it was required that all participants agreed for their data to be analysed to address the team's research questions. All participants were informed that they could withdraw at any time, and that all of the information that they provided would be kept confidential and stored in an anonymised format. Ethical approval was obtained from the Joint South London and Maudsley, and Institute of Psychiatry Research Ethics Committee for both studies and from ethics committees in Birmingham and Cardiff for the unipolar depression study.
Measures
Clinical assessment
All participants in the unipolar and bipolar groups were interviewed face to face using the Schedule for Clinical Assessments in Neuropsychiatry, Version 2.1 (SCAN) Reference Wing, Babor, Brugha, Burke, Cooper and Giel28 to establish a formal lifetime diagnosis of recurrent unipolar depression or bipolar disorder. The presence and severity of the psychopathology items were retrospectively rated by trained research assistants for the worst depressive episode in participants with unipolar depression and for participants with bipolar disorder for their worst depressive and manic episodes.
The 4- to 6-week peak intensity of symptoms within each worst episode was then used to rate the presence and severity of each SCAN item. The computerised version of the SCAN 2.1 is built on top of the iSHELL system (version 1.0.3.5 for Windows), which is a computer-aided personal interviewing tool produced by the World Health Organization and which provides DSM-IV operationally defined diagnoses.
Screening questionnaire
All controls received the Past History Schedule Reference McGuffin, Katz and Aldrich29 to assess lifetime psychopathology. This instrument has six items requiring a ‘yes’ or ‘no’ response. If the participants responded with a ‘yes’ for any item, further information was obtained to assess whether the participant was describing psychopathological symptoms.
Present symptoms
Participants completed the second edition of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) Reference Beck, Steer and Brown30 reporting on their mood over the previous 2 weeks. This scale consists of 21 groups of 4 statements; each statement represents a different degree of severity. The participants endorsed one of the four statements in each group that reflected their mood over the past 2 weeks. Although, the BDI-II is a self-report measure, the items were read out to the participants by the researcher in the current investigation. This was to reduce the possibility for missing data and ensure the participant's comprehension of the statements. The total BDI-II score was used as a measure of severity of current depressive symptoms and no cut-offs were applied.
Stressful life events
The experience of 11 life events were recorded using the List of Threatening Experiences Questionnaire. Reference Brugha, Bebbington, Tennant and Hurry31 This instrument is designed to detect events carrying significant long-term threat and particular salience for mood disorders. Reference Kim, Stewart, Kim, Yang, Shin and Kim32 Previous analyses have shown that the List of Threatening Experiences Questionnaire successfully captures 82.5% of the life events covered in the more extensive life event interviews. Reference Brugha, Bebbington, Tennant and Hurry31
Participants in the unipolar group reported the events that occurred 6 months before their worst depressive episode, and participants in the bipolar group recorded the events they experienced 6 months prior to their worst depressive and worst manic episodes. For controls the events occurring in the 6 months prior to interview were rated. The List of Threatening Experiences Questionnaire was administered by trained research assistants, this involved asking whether the participant experienced an event, confirming the event occurred during the index period and obtaining some contextual information to establish that the reference event fulfilled the classification of the items. Whereas the original instrument consists of 12 events, 2 of these were combined for the present study (these were: ‘… did you have a separation due to marital difficulties’ and ‘… did you break off a steady relationship’ combined to ‘… did you have a separation due to marital difficulties or break off a steady relationship’).
Drawing on previous research, Reference Brugha, Bebbington, Tennant and Hurry31 events were divided into two broad categories: dependent (items 5–11, Table 2) and independent (items 1–4, Table 2). With one modification, that is the item ‘valued item lost or stolen’ has previously been classified as independent, however there is a high possibility that if the item were lost, negligence from the individual would have contributed to the occurrence of this event, therefore in this study it was categorised as a dependent event.
Participants reporting each event, n (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Event | Unipolar group (n = 1448) | Controls (n = 679) | OR (95% CI)Footnote a |
1. Serious illness, injury or an assault | 282 (19.5) | 39 (5.7) | 3.61 (2.48–5.24)Footnote *** |
2. Serious illness, injury or assault of close relative | 280 (19.3) | 110 (16.2) | 0.97 (0.73–1.29) |
3. Death of spouse or first-degree relative | 175 (12.1) | 23 (3.4) | 4.11 (2.54–6.64)Footnote *** |
4. Death of a close family friend or other relative | 183 (12.6) | 103 (15.2) | 0.60Footnote b (0.43–0.82)Footnote *** |
5. Separation due to marital difficulties or break up of a steady relationship | 321 (22.2) | 36 (5.3) | 4.46 (3.05–6.51)Footnote *** |
6. Serious problem with a close friend, neighbour or relatives | 357 (24.7) | 58 (8.5) | 3.04 (2.21–4.18)Footnote *** |
7. Job loss | 104 (7.2) | 12 (1.8) | 3.20 (1.62–6.30)Footnote *** |
8. Seeking work without success for more than 1 month | 103 (7.1) | 25 (3.7) | 0.98 (0.57–1.66) |
9. Major financial crisis | 199 (13.7) | 27 (4.0) | 2.38 (1.50–3.80)Footnote *** |
10. Problems with the police involving a court appearance | 58 (4.0) | 10 (1.5) | 1.63 (0.77–3.45) |
11. Valued item lost or stolen | 100 (6.9) | 33 (4.9) | 0.94 (0.58–1.52) |
a. Reported OR for the prevalence of each life event among the unipolar group compared with controls. Adjusted for age at index period.
b. This prevalence is higher among the controls relative to the unipolar group.
*** P⩽0.001.
Data analyses
Logistic regression models were undertaken to test whether there were any differences in the proportions of participants in the unipolar and bipolar groups compared with controls reporting each event prior to their index periods. Caseness (bipolar disorder/unipolar depression or control) was used in these models as the dependent variable, and all 11 life events were entered into a logistic regression model simultaneously and used as predictor variables. Age at index period was included as a covariate. Given that our independent and dependent variables were categorical and we were keen to include covariates in our analyses, logistic regression models were the most appropriate statistical tests to use.
Logistic regression models were also used to compare the proportion of participants in the unipolar and bipolar groups reporting each event prior to their index periods and test for any differences. Caseness (bipolar disorder or unipolar depression) was used as the dependent variable. All 11 events were entered simultaneously into one logistic regression model and used as predictor variables. The BDI-II score was included as a covariate to account for the confounding effect of current mood; we previously reported a positive correlation between depressive symptoms and number of events recorded. Reference Johnson3
Results
Unipolar group v. controls
Six events were found to be more commonly reported by participants with unipolar depression prior to their worst depressive episode relative to controls prior to their interview (Table 2). Interestingly, significantly more controls reported the death of a relative or family friend compared with those in the unipolar group.
Independent and dependent events
A significantly greater proportion of participants in the unipolar group reported experiencing at least one dependent and independent life event 6 months before their worst depressive episode compared with controls for the 6 months prior to their assessment (Tables 3 and 4).
Bipolar group v. controls
A significantly larger proportion of participants in the bipolar group reported six events before their worst depressive episode relative to controls for the period prior to their interview (Table 5). Similar results were found for the worst manic episode (Table 6). Conversely, a significantly greater percentage of controls reported serious illness of a first-degree relative and death of family friend or other relative compared with those in the bipolar group for their worst depressive and manic episodes.
Participants reporting at least one event 6 months leading up to their index periods, n (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Group and index period | n | Dependent events | Independent events |
Controls for comparison with the unipolar group | 679 | 162 (23.9) | 228 (33.6) |
Controls for comparison with the bipolar group | 612 | 139 (22.7) | 210 (34.3) |
Unipolar group | |||
Worst depressive episode | 1448 | 754 (52.1) | 655 (45.2) |
Bipolar group | |||
Worst depressive episode | 512 | 249 (48.6) | 188 (36.7) |
Worst manic episode | 512 | 258 (50.4) | 174 (34.0) |
a. For controls the events occurring in the 6 months prior to interview were rated.
Dependent events | Independent events | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Unadjusted | Adjusted for age at index period | Unadjusted | Adjusted for age at index period | |||||
Index period | β | OR (95% CI) | β | OR (95% CI) | β | OR (95% CI) | β | OR (95% CI) |
Unipolar group | ||||||||
Worst depressive episode | 1.24 | 3.47 (2.83–4.25)Footnote *** | 1.29 | 3.68 (2.93–4.48)Footnote *** | 0.49 | 1.63 (1.35–1.98)Footnote *** | 0.54 | 1.71 (1.40–2.09)Footnote *** |
Bipolar group | ||||||||
Worst depressive episode | 1.17 | 3.22 (2.49–4.16)Footnote *** | 1.15 | 3.17 (2.43–4.12)Footnote *** | 0.11 | 1.11 (0.87–1.42) | 0.10 | 1.11 (0.86–1.43) |
Worst manic episode | 1.24 | 3.46 (2.68–4.47)Footnote *** | 1.26 | 3.51 (2.69–4.57)Footnote *** | –0.02 | 0.99 (0.77–1.26) | –0.04 | 0.96 (0.74–1.24) |
*** P⩽0.001.
Dependent and independent events
Dependent events were found to be significantly more common before the worst manic and depressive episodes for participants in the bipolar group compared with controls for the period prior to their assessment (Tables 3 and 4). The proportion of those in the bipolar group and the controls reporting an independent event prior to their index periods was not significantly different.
Bipolar disorder: worst manic episode v. worst depressive episode
When comparing the percentage of participants with bipolar disorder reporting each of the 11 events prior to the worst depressive and worst manic episodes, only one significant difference was observed. That is, experiencing a serious problem with a friend, neighbour or relative was reported by significantly more participants in this group prior to their worst manic episode relative to their worst depressive episode (odds ratio (OR) = 1.36, 95% CI 1.01–1.84, Ples;0.05). No significant differences were found between the proportion in this group reporting an independent (OR = 0.89, 95% CI 0.69–1.15, P = not significant) or dependent (OR = 1.07, 95% CI 0.84–1.37, P = not significant) event before their worst depressive episode compared with their worst manic episode.
Unipolar v. bipolar group
Eight events were recorded by a similar proportion of participants in the unipolar and bipolar groups for the 6 months prior to their worst affective episodes (Tables 7 and 8). However, the death of a spouse or first-degree relative was reported by significantly more participants with unipolar depression compared with bipolar disorder. The illness of a first-degree relative was reported by significantly more participants in the unipolar group prior to their worst depressive episode compared with those in the bipolar group for their worst depressive but not manic episodes.
Experiencing a financial crisis on the other hand was experienced by a significantly larger proportion of individuals in the bipolar group compared with the unipolar group prior to their worst affective episodes.
Dependent and independent events
A significantly greater percentage of participants in the unipolar group reported experiencing an independent event prior to their worst depressive episode relative to the bipolar group prior to the worst affective episodes (bipolar disorder worst depressive episode: OR = 1.41, 95% CI 1.11–1.77, Ples;0.01; worst manic episode: OR = 1.59, 95% CI 1.26–2.02, Ples;0.001). Events classified as dependent were reported by a similar proportion in both groups prior to their worst affective episodes (bipolar disorder worst depressive episode: OR = 1.11, 95% CI 0.89–1.40; worst manic episode: OR = 1.02, 95% CI 0.82–1.28, P was not significant for either).
It should be noted that all analyses concerning the bipolar group were repeated with the exclusion of those diagnosed with bipolar II disorder and the results remained the same.
Discussion
Specific events and unipolar depression
Six events were associated with unipolar depressive episodes, including personal illness, job loss and financial crisis, consistent with the literature. Reference Honkalampi, Hintikka, Haatainen, Koivumaa-Honkanen, Tanskanen and Viinamaki9 The death of a friend or family member, other than a spouse or first-degree relative was reported by a higher proportion of controls compared with those in the unipolar or bipolar groups. Those in the unipolar group did report significantly more deaths of spouses or first-degree relatives, which substantiate findings from the literature. Reference Maciejewski, Prigerson and Mazure33 The impairments of mood disorders are far reaching and have an impact on employment and relationships. Reference Miklowitz and Johnson34,Reference McGuffin, Murray, Kendler, McGuffin, Wessely and Castle35 Such factors could limit the social network of people with mood disorders meaning they are less likely than unaffected individuals to experience certain events, such as illness or death of a family friend or relative, explaining the findings observed in this investigation.
Participants reporting each event, n (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Event | Bipolar group (n = 512) | Controls (n = 612) | OR (95% CI)Footnote a |
1. Serious illness, injury or an assault | 100 (19.5) | 33 (5.4) | 3.61 (2.27–5.73)Footnote *** |
2. Serious illness, injury or assault of close relative | 63 (12.3) | 106 (17.3) | 0.51Footnote b (0.34–0.77)Footnote *** |
3. Death of spouse or first-degree relative | 30 (5.9) | 22 (3.6) | 1.98 (1.03–3.78)Footnote * |
4. Death of a close family friend or other relative | 60 (11.7) | 93 (15.2) | 0.54Footnote b (0.36–0.83)Footnote ** |
5. Separation due to marital difficulties or break up of a steady relationship | 112 (21.9) | 28 (4.6) | 4.40 (2.75–7.04)Footnote *** |
6. Serious problem with a close friend, neighbour or relatives | 114 (22.3) | 52 (8.5) | 2.30 (1.54–3.44)Footnote *** |
7. Job loss | 42 (8.2) | 10 (1.6) | 3.69 (1.64–8.29)Footnote ** |
8. Seeking work without success for more than 1 month | 36 (7.0) | 20 (3.3) | 0.93 (0.46–1.88) |
9. Major financial crisis | 87 (17.0) | 25 (4.1) | 2.85 (1.67–4.87)Footnote *** |
10. Problems with the police involving a court appearance | 24 (4.7) | 10 (1.6) | 1.32 (0.53–3.27) |
11. Valued item lost or stolen | 37 (7.2) | 28 (4.6) | 0.78 (0.40–1.49) |
a. Reported OR for the prevalence of each life event among the bipolar group compared with controls. Adjusted for age at index period.
b. The prevalence is higher among the controls relative to the bipolar group
* P⩽0.05,
** P⩽0.01,
*** P⩽0.001.
Participants reporting each event, n (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Event | Bipolar group (n = 512) | Controls (n = 612) | OR (95% CI)Footnote a |
1. Serious illness, injury or an assault | 77 (15.0) | 33 (5.4) | 2.37 (1.47–3.83)Footnote *** |
2. Serious illness, injury or assault of close relative | 66 (12.9) | 106 (17.3) | 0.60Footnote b (0.40–0.88)Footnote ** |
3. Death of spouse or first-degree relative | 30 (5.9) | 22 (3.6) | 1.19 (0.61–2.36) |
4. Death of a close family friend or other relative | 54 (10.5) | 93 (15.2) | 0.47Footnote b (0.31–0.73)Footnote *** |
5. Separation due to marital difficulties or break up of a steady relationship | 102 (19.9) | 28 (4.6) | 4.05 (2.51–6.54)Footnote *** |
6. Serious problem with a close friend, neighbour or relatives | 135 (26.4) | 52 (8.5) | 3.47 (2.37–5.09)Footnote *** |
7. Job loss | 35 (6.8) | 10 (1.6) | 3.24 (1.41–7.43)Footnote ** |
8. Seeking work without success for more than 1 month | 36 (7.0) | 20 (3.3) | 0.97 (0.49–1.93) |
9. Major financial crisis | 80 (15.6) | 25 (4.1) | 2.91 (1.69–5.01)Footnote *** |
10. Problems with the police involving a court appearance | 19 (3.7) | 10 (1.6) | 1.38 (0.55–3.47) |
11. Valued item lost or stolen | 31 (6.1) | 28 (4.6) | 0.77 (0.39–1.50) |
a. Reported OR for the prevalence of each life event among the bipolar group compared with controls. Adjusted for age at index period.
b. The prevalence is higher among the controls relative to the bipolar group.
* P⩽0.05,
** P⩽0.01,
*** P⩽0.001.
The results of this study showed that dependent and independent events were associated with unipolar depressive episodes comparable with previous research. Reference Harris4,Reference Hammen14 These findings suggest that the occurrence of stressful events may trigger an episode of unipolar depression. But also those individuals susceptible to depression may also generate their life stress (dependent events), as outlined in the stress generation theory. Reference Hammen36 Symptoms such as irritability (characteristic of unipolar depression) may contribute to the occurrence of some events (such as interpersonal conflicts) possibly exacerbating depressive symptoms, potentially spiralling into a depressive episode. According to the stress generation theory, the relationship between life stress and unipolar depression is transactional, such that individuals with unipolar depression may contribute to the occurrence of stressful events, as well as reacting to them. Reference Hammen36
Specific events and bipolar disorder
Findings from the present investigation also showed that six events were recorded by significantly more participants with bipolar disorder before their worst depressive episode compared with controls for the period before their assessment; a similar pattern was observed for the worst manic episode. The events included interpersonal problems, financial difficulties and job loss, which are consistent with existing research. Reference Dunner, Patrick and Fieve7,Reference Kessing, Agerbo and Mortensen12
Dependent events were associated with depressive and manic episodes in the bipolar group, consistent with studies applying the stress generation theory to bipolar disorder. Reference Bender, Alloy, Sylvia, Urosevic and Abramson37 Independent events were unrelated to bipolar episodes, supporting the results of previous studies, Reference Horesh and Iancu18 suggesting that the link between life stress and bipolar disorder maybe a consequence of the illness. Using a self-report checklist is not the most sensitive way of measuring independence and some events classified as dependent here should be reclassified as independent due to the context and biography of the person. It is important to employ more sensitive life stress measures (for example the Life Events and Difficulties Schedule Reference Brown and Harris38 ) where independence can be established more readily.
Participants reporting each event, n (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Event | Bipolar group (n = 512) | Unipolar group (n = 1448) | OR (95% CI)Footnote a |
1. Serious illness, injury or an assault | 100 (19.5) | 282 (19.5) | 0.90 (0.67–1.21) |
2. Serious illness, injury or assault of close relative | 63 (12.3) | 280 (19.3) | 1.61 (1.15–2.27)Footnote ** |
3. Death of spouse or first-degree relative | 30 (5.9) | 175 (12.1) | 1.86 (1.18–2.93)Footnote ** |
4. Death of a close family friend or other relative | 60 (11.7) | 183 (12.6) | 0.94 (0.66–1.35) |
5. Separation due to marital difficulties or break up of a steady relationship | 112 (21.9) | 321 (22.2) | 1.07 (0.81–1.41) |
6. Serious problem with a close friend, neighbour or relatives | 114 (22.3) | 357 (24.7) | 1.11 (0.84–1.46) |
7. Job loss | 42 (8.2) | 104 (7.2) | 0.83 (0.53–1.31) |
8. Seeking work without success for more than 1 month | 36 (7.0) | 103 (7.1) | 1.12 (0.70–1.80) |
9. Major financial crisis | 87 (17.0) | 199 (13.7) | 1.48Footnote b (1.04–2.10)Footnote * |
10. Problems with the police involving a court appearance | 24 (4.7) | 58 (4.0) | 0.96 (0.54–1.71) |
11. Valued item lost or stolen | 37 (7.2) | 100 (6.9) | 1.08 (0.68–1.73) |
a. Reported OR for the prevalence of each life event among the unipolar group compared with the bipolar group. Adjusted for age at index period and depression symptoms.
b. This prevalence is higher among the bipolar group relative to the unipolar group.
* P⩽0.05,
** P⩽0.01.
Participants reporting each event, n (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Event | Bipolar group (n = 512) | Unipolar group (n = 1448) | OR (95% CI)Footnote a |
1. Serious illness, injury or an assault | 77 (15.0) | 282 (19.5) | 1.27 (0.93–1.73) |
2. Serious illness, injury or assault of close relative | 66 (12.9) | 280 (19.3) | 1.36 (0.97–1.90) |
3. Death of spouse or first-degree relative | 30 (5.9) | 175 (12.1) | 2.25 (1.39–3.64)Footnote *** |
4. Death of a close family friend or other relative | 54 (10.5) | 183 (12.6) | 1.15 (0.79–1.67) |
5. Separation due to marital difficulties or break up of a steady relationship | 102 (19.9) | 321 (22.2) | 1.24 (0.93–1.65) |
6. Serious problem with a close friend, neighbour or relatives | 135 (26.4) | 357 (24.7) | 0.80 (0.62–1.05) |
7. Job loss | 35 (6.8) | 104 (7.2) | 1.16 (0.71–1.88) |
8. Seeking work without success for more than 1 month | 36 (7.0) | 103 (7.1) | 1.18 (0.73–1.92) |
9. Major financial crisis | 80 (15.6) | 199 (13.7) | 1.57Footnote b (1.10–2.23)Footnote * |
10. Problems with the police involving a court appearance | 19 (3.7) | 58 (4.0) | 0.99 (0.54–1.80) |
11. Valued item lost or stolen | 31 (6.1) | 100 (6.9) | 1.17 (0.72–1.90) |
a. Reported OR for the prevalence of each life event among the unipolar group compared with the bipolar group. Adjusted for age at index period and depression symptoms.
b. This prevalence is higher among the bipolar group relative to the unipolar group.
* P⩽0.05,
*** P⩽0.01.
Bipolar disorder: depression v. mania
Experiencing interpersonal problems was found to be more pertinent to mania than bipolar depression, a finding also reported by other studies. Reference Dunner, Patrick and Fieve7,Reference Hall, Dunner, Zeller and Fieve11 This contradicts a previous study that found that illness within the family and work-related difficulties were more strongly associated with mania than bipolar depression. Reference Dunner, Patrick and Fieve7 However, this study consisted of 79 participants compared with 512 with bipolar disorder in the present investigation, suggesting that our failure to replicate Dunner et al's Reference Dunner, Patrick and Fieve7 results is unlikely to be as a result of lack of power.
Moreover, similar proportions of those in the bipolar group reported dependent and independent events prior to their worst depressive and manic episodes, supporting prior research. Reference Malkoff-Schwartz, Frank, Anderson, Hlastala, Luther and Sherrill39,Reference Malkoff-Schwartz, Frank, Anderson, Sherrill, Siegel and Patterson40 But other studies have found that independent events are more common prior to a manic rather than a bipolar depressive episode. Reference Hall, Dunner, Zeller and Fieve11,Reference McPherson, Herbison and Romans41 McPherson et al Reference McPherson, Herbison and Romans41 and Hall et al Reference Hall, Dunner, Zeller and Fieve11 enquired about life events that occurred 3 months prior to relapse, whereas the present study focused on events experienced 6 months prior to the worst affective episodes. This 3-month difference increases the likelihood that an event may occur that may explain the discrepant findings.
Unipolar depression v. bipolar disorder
Our findings suggest that the majority of life events associated with unipolar depression are equally important for bipolar disorder. However, death of a spouse or relative was found to be more pertinent to unipolar than bipolar episodes. Illness of a close relative was more salient to an unipolar rather than a bipolar depressive episode. We also found experiencing a financial crisis was more characteristic of bipolar rather than unipolar depressive episodes.
Other studies have reported different patterns of event specificity for unipolar depression and bipolar disorder. Bidzinska Reference Bidzińska8 recruited 50 participants with bipolar disorder and 47 with unipolar depression and found that work-related difficulties and failures characterised bipolar disorder, whereas somatic illnesses indexed unipolar depression. Differences in sample size and time frames are possible explanations of the differences found in this earlier study. Reference Bidzińska8
In our study, 22% (n = 114) of the bipolar group had a temporal overlap between their worst depressive and manic episodes; excluding these participants from all sets of analyses made little difference to the results. There was one exception: the association between financial crises and bipolar rather than unipolar depressive episodes was no longer significant.
Methodological limitations
The present study possesses a number of strengths (such as a large sample), but three methodological limitations must be considered when interpreting the results. First, all the participants in the unipolar and the bipolar groups recalled the events they experienced 6 months leading up to their worst affective episodes, which occurred on average 10 years prior to their participation. This may have resulted in retrospective reporting biases (for example normal forgetting). Reference Johnson3 In the present study there were no significant differences in the recall periods between the unipolar and bipolar groups thus possible retrospective reporting biases are likely to apply equally to both groups. This suggests that retrospective biases have limited influence on the comparisons between unipolar and bipolar groups and any differences between the groups are likely to be the result of differential diagnoses.
Second, the controls used in the present study may be described as super healthy or resilient given that they have no personal or family history of any psychiatric disorder. By comparing such individuals to both our affected case groups, the differences are likely to be inflated. However, this issue does not invalidate our findings given that they are supported by previous studies using controls that were not screened for a personal or family history of any psychiatric disorder. Reference Ambelas42
Finally, using a life-event questionnaire has some limitations, including the participants’ subjective interpretation as to what counts as an instance of a certain item and the misdating of events. Although we acknowledge that interviews are comprehensive and likely to be the optimum way of measuring life events, we also recognise that they are time and labour intensive resulting in high data collection costs, especially when applied to large samples similar to our own. Questionnaires in contrast are quick and easy to complete making them more affordable and suitable for large studies. Some information is lost when utilising questionnaires that create various problems (for example establishing independence). In the current study we attempted to compromise by using a researcher-administered questionnaire that attempts to surmount dating and contextual issues related to using a self-report questionnaire with the added benefit of reducing the time and expense of more lengthy interviews.
Implications
Similar types of stressful events were associated with unipolar depression and bipolar disorder highlighting their overlap. However, some event specificity was detected suggesting some distinction between these illnesses. There are various benefits to understanding the relationship between specific events and mood episodes. Clinically, experiencing a particular event will provide warning signs of possible relapse and thus a time for the individual's support network to offer additional monitoring and care. Our findings also have implications for gene–environment interaction research. Many of the studies exploring interactions between stressful events and genetic variants in mood disorders have focused on the number of life events Reference Hosang, Uher, Keers, Cohen-Woods, Craig and Korszun43 or the reporting of at least one. Reference Mandelli, Serretti, Marino, Pirovano, Calati and Colombo44 But it is possible that certain events, such as those identified in this investigation, are driving this relationship. Future studies should therefore focus on exploring interactions between particular events and genetic factors in mood disorders.
Funding
G.M.H. is supported by a Medical Research Council (MRC) and Economic Social Research Council (ESRC) UK interdisciplinary postdoctoral fellowship (PTA-037-27-0165). The Depression Case Control study was supported by a MRC grant and the Bipolar affective disorder Case Control study data collection was funded by an unrestricted grant from GlaxoSmithKline Research and Development.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Audrey Morgan, Cerisse Gunasinghe, Katharine Mead, Joanna Gray, Ylva Dahlin, Amanda Elkin, Joanna O’Leary, Nathan O’Neill, Nicola Reynolds, Zainab Samaan, Abraham Stern, Linda Southwick, Kopal Tandon, Alison Wheatley, Richard Williamson, Julia Woods, Sarah Ball, Ophelia Beer, Julian Childs, Sam Keating, Rachel Marsh, Penny Machin and Lucy Maddox who were involved with the data collection and management of the studies.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.