Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T01:07:34.207Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Insight and psychosis: the next 30 years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2019

Anthony S. David*
Affiliation:
Director, UCL Institute of Mental Health, University College London, UK
*
Correspondence: Anthony S. David, UCL Institute of Mental Health, University College London, Maple House, Tottenham Court Road, London, WT1 7NF, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Academic interest in the concept of insight in psychosis has increased markedly over the past 30 years, prompting this selective appraisal of the current state of the art. Considerable progress has been made in terms of measurement and confirming a number of clinical associations. More recently, the relationship between insight and involuntary treatment has been scrutinised more closely alongside the link between decision-making capacity and insight. Advances in the clinical and cognitive neurosciences have influenced conceptual development, particularly the field of ‘metacognition’. New therapies, including those that are psychologically and neurophysiologically based, are being tested as ways to enhance insight.

Type
Analysis
Copyright
Copyright © The Author 2019

The study of insight in relation to psychosis began in earnest 30 years ago.Reference David1 An anonymous Lancet editorialist commented at the time that such study was ‘academically nourishing but clinically sterile’.2 Now seems a good time to take stock and look forward to the next 30 years. To date there has been much useful conceptual analysis, the production and widespread use of reliable and valid rating instruments and a set of replicable clinical correlations to add to the psychiatric canon. These include correlations between insight and psychopathology, IQ (poorer insight, worse psychopathology and lower IQ) and mood (lower mood and better insight).Reference Amador and David3 Another obvious and clinically relevant relationship is that between insight and treatment adherence and hence outcome. There is a suggestion that good insight confers a more favourable prognosis over and above adherence, although this would be hard to establish through observational studies alone.Reference Lincoln, Lüllmann and Rief4

Insight and capacity

The relation between insight and adherence, or rather poor insight and coercive treatment, is naturally where critics of the insight concept converge. ‘Insight’, they say, is merely agreeing with the doctor. A person's refusal to accept a doctor's considered diagnosis of, say, cancer would be deemed extraordinary, yet in the case of schizophrenia this is not so. Illness categories in psychiatry remain contested and lack objective criteria, and the psychiatrist's authority is not a given. But where a patient's self-appraisal as not being unwell or needing help is at odds with their peers (including those with lived experience of the condition) and family, might this not be regarded as a lack of insight even without the power imbalance dimension? There is a strong empirical as well as common sense connection between insight and decision-making capacity,Reference Owen, Richardson, David, Szmukler, Hayward and Hotopf5 which requires further ethical and practical scrutiny. However, recent qualitative work in medicolegal contexts such as mental health tribunals, finds that ‘lack of insight’ is often used as a proxy for lack of capacity but without corresponding justification, and may serve to undermine the individual's testimony.Reference Diesfeld6

The interface between insight and capacity to decide upon treatment is seen most vividly in the ability to ‘use and weigh’ information, a key criterion for mental capacity used in the Mental Capacity Act (2005) definition. It is hard to see how the benefits and harms of a proposed treatment can be weighed in the balance if you do not believe you are ill in the first place. But rather than clinicians simply pronouncing that insight is lacking, it would be more informative to trace the arguments the patient proffers (if any). For example, if the patient says they are the victim of a conspiracy to rob them of their freedom and force them to take mind-altering drugs for no reason whatsoever, then the benefits or otherwise of treatment are not being weighed in the balance. Alternatively, if the patient describes realistic plans of how they will survive outside of hospital and that they have previously done so without medication despite their clinician's argument that this has led to relapse, then the statutory authority at least has the basis of a meaningful discussion, which they would not have if they had been told merely that the patient's insight is ‘partial’.

Multidisciplinary enquiry attempting to tackle these and related dilemmas is ongoing thanks to the Wellcome funded Mental Health and Justice programme (mhj.org.uk).

Metacognition

Metacognition is a relatively new area of psychology examining people's ability to reflect upon their own cognition and appears to be related to insight as used in psychiatry. At its most precise it is the degree of confidence a person has on a specific judgement, such as a perceptual decision (did the dots move left or right?) or a mnemonic task (is this word old or new?). The extent to which such confidence is merited is ‘metacognitive efficiency’. This takes into account performance level and seems to have a specific cerebral localisation.Reference Fleming, Weil, Nagy, Dolan and Rees7,Reference Fleming, Ryu, Golfinos and Blackmon8 But is this task-by-task metacognition, whose time course is measured in milliseconds to seconds, related to more day-to-day self-judgements (did I choose the right route home?) carried out over seconds or minutes, or to questions such as did I choose the right career? The work of a lifetime perhaps – quite possibly.

The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition is beginning to make important contributions to psychopathology.Reference Rouault, Seowa, Gillan and Fleming9,Reference David, Bedford, Wiffen and Gilleen10 Lack of metacognitive awareness (not reflecting on whether a decision is correct) on even abstract perceptual tasks may link with impulsivity. Low confidence in decisions globally (‘I'm bound to be wrong whatever I decide’) underpins much thinking in depression, whereas excessive metacognition can inhibit decision making, as in obsessive compulsive disorder. The lack of ability to change one's mind in the light of new evidence is a core feature of delusions. Paradigms that build on advances in metacognitive research and make use of computational modelling also promise much in this regard.Reference van der Plas, David and Fleming11 Models of decision making under conditions of uncertainty are being constructed and tested where personal values are incorporated along with such variables as the strength of current beliefs, contradictory information and likely benefits of any decision (immediate versus delayed).

Insight and metacognition

For insight in psychiatry, the metacognitive challenge posed is to reflect on one's own mental and interpersonal functioning. It involves an attempt to see one's thinking and behaviour ‘objectively’, as if through another person's eyes, and then comparing it to some representation of mental health. There is just one fundamental question asked in relation to clinical insight (after Aubrey Lewis): do I have an illness and is the illness mental? It includes the moment-to-moment evaluation of mental activity (e.g. was someone speaking to me or was it my imagination?) as well as more enduring ‘semantic’ evaluations, such as whether my beliefs are true and shared by others. Note that although that representation of mental health will be the amalgam of received opinion and experience, there is no judging doctor, as it were, in sight.

Cognitive insight is a new construct put forward by Beck et al Reference Beck, Baruch, Balter, Steer and Warman12 and refers to a cognitive style or propensity to question one's ideas, beliefs and behaviour. One advantage it affords research is that it enables insight to be studied in healthy individuals without confounders such as stigma and the effects of treatment, and thus to be linked to normal psychological processes where there is no illness into which one might or might not have insight. An early area of interest is the relationship between cognitive and clinical insight. Thanks to meta-analyses,Reference Van Camp, Sabbe and Oldenburg13 we can say that there is a surprisingly weak correlation between the two. However, cognitive insight may have some predictive validity clinically; for example, better cognitive insight leading to fewer symptoms after 1 and 4 years following a first episode of psychosis.Reference O'Connor, Ellett, Ajnakina, Schoeler, Kollliakou and Trotta14 We still do not know if poor cognitive insight in a vulnerable individual may be a risk factor for later psychosis per se, or whether, in the event of them developing a psychosis, they would have good or poor clinical insight.

A relationship between mood and clinical (and cognitive) insight is now well established. It applies to most conditions in which it has been studied: the lower the mood the better the insight,Reference Murri, Respino, Innamorati, Cervetti, Calcagno and Pompili15 as noted above. Such is the closeness of the association that it is reasonable to suggest that they are two sides of the same coin and spring from the human condition. The notion is that removal of rose-tinted spectacles reveals the world as it truly is: depressive realism. Although this links neatly with metacognition and confidence, it runs counter to received clinical folklore that the gaining of insight, particularly after a psychotic episode, induces depression and at worst, may even lead to suicide. Empirical justification for unidirectional causality is lacking,Reference Lopez-Morinigo, Di Forti and Ajnakina16 perhaps because of the messy complicating factors that often precede suicide in people with psychosis in the real world: longstanding depression, rejection of treatment and disengagement with social and professional support. These factors attest more to the loss of insight than its gain, notwithstanding the pain attached to the latter. Nevertheless, any psychotherapeutic attempt to restore insight (see below) should be in the form of acknowledging difficulties as a first step in gaining mastery over them, encouraging openness to taking up an effective treatment for those symptoms that cause distress at least as a start, and not at all the forced acceptance of some abstract illness model.

Treatment

Metacognitive therapies

Talking therapies designed to improve metacognition (metacognitive therapy and metacognitive training) across a range of mental disorders have been developed and tested in small clinical trials. A systematic reviewReference Philipp, Kriston, Lanio, Kühne, Härter, Moritz and Meistert17 found 19 controlled studies in schizophrenia, of which 15 were randomised. The results approached significance when compared with standard or other psychological treatments, with a pooled standard mean difference in positive symptoms scores estimated to be −0.31 (95% CIs −0.50 to −0.12). Two small but intensive trials of metacognitive reflection and insight therapy versus treatment as usual to improve insight and self-reflection in first-episode psychosisReference Vohs, Leonhardt, James, Francis, Breier and Mehdiyoun18 and schizophreniaReference De Jong, van Donkersgoed, Timmerman, Aan het Rot, Wunderink and Arends19 showed encouraging but modest benefits. A larger (n = 121), recent, multi-centre, group-based psychosocial intervention (‘REFLEX’) with an active control condition showed improvements in insight in both conditions, although marginally greater in the main treatment arm.Reference Pijnenborg, Vos, Timmerman, van der Gaag, Sportel and Arends20 To some extent the success of all these therapies depends on the closeness of the link between metacognition and insight that, as discussed, is itself a topic of ongoing enquiry.

Medication

Given that worse psychopathology goes with worse insight, any effective treatment should improve insight. However there are both state and trait elements to insight.Reference Wiffen, Rabinowitz, Lex and David21 A systematic review found rather sporadic evidence that there were insight-enhancing therapies.Reference Pijnenborg, van Donkersgoed, David and Aleman22 A large, open, randomised controlled trial, the European First-Episode Schizophrenia Trial, compared haloperidol, amisulpride, olanzapine, quetiapine and ziprasidone on insight in first-episode schizophrenia and related disorders. There was a highly significant 56% improvement on the insight and judgement item from the Positive and Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia Scale at 12 months, in line with the level of symptomatic improvement across the board. All the antipsychotic drugs were similar except for quetiapine, which tended to lag behind the others.Reference Pijnenborg, Timmerman, Derks, Fleischhacker, Kahn and Aleman23

Neuroscience

Some early, exploratory applications of neuroscientific methods to study insight showed changes within groups of patients with schizophrenia in the direction of more brain volume loss in those rated as having lower insight scores, but these may have been partially resulting from confounders to do with general illness severity. More refined imaging techniques (e.g. examining cortical thicknessReference Béland, Makowski, Konsztowicz, Buchy, Chakravarty and Lepage24) have not come up with a consistent candidate for an ‘insight centre’ in the brain and nor are they likely to, given the complexity and likely distributed nature of the construct. More hypothesis-driven work for example that insight deficits might be linked to the right cerebral hemisphere analogous to anosognosia continues (see Morgan et al Reference Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Lappin, Hutchinson and Sucking25 for review). New technologies have revealed subtle white matter and connectivity problems.Reference Asmal, du Plessis, Vink, Fouche, Chiliza and Emsley26,Reference Ćurčić-Blake, van der Meer, Pijnenborg, David and Aleman27 However, given the fluctuating nature of insight, the promise of functional imaging to shed light on the process has always been greater than structural, and more so since a normative functional system underlying self-appraisal and involving a cortical midline network has been established.Reference van der Meer, Costafreda, Aleman and David28 This system may be operating suboptimallyReference Holt, Cassidy, Andrews-Hanna, Lee, Coombs and Goff29Reference van der Meer, de Vos, Stiekema, Pijnenborg, van Tol and Nolen31 in patients with psychosis and this could relate to illness appraisal. Similarly, the default mode network (involving an overlapping area of medial frontal structures activated during internally directed thinking) is a region of interest to insight researchers.Reference Gerretsen, Menon, Mamo, Fervaha, Remington and Pollock32

Given the effectiveness of dopamine-blocking drugs to improve psychotic symptoms and insight noted above, it is natural to explore the relationship between D2 receptor blockade and changes in insight. This was indirectly studied in 16 patients with schizophrenia, using a pharmacological estimation of dopamine blockade based on plasma level concentrations.Reference Gerretsen, Takeuchi, Ozzoude, Graff-Guerrero and Uchida33 An association was found at baseline but not after gradual medication dose reduction, perhaps because it was swamped by other illness-related measures. So far, neurochemical imaging techniques have yet to be deployed systematically to study insight.

A genetic contribution has also been explored by analysing insight in participants in the Clinical Antipsychotics Trials of Intervention Effectiveness trial. Using the psychosis risk score derived from genome-wide association studies carried out by the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, the authors found that patients with the highest psychosis risk score had 5.9 times increased risk for poor insight compared with patients with the lowest scores, although this only explained 3.2% of the variance in poor insight.Reference Xavier, Vorderstrasse, Keefe and Dungan34

Neuromodulation

An emerging area of therapeutic research is neuromodulation. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a simple, safe and non-invasive method for selectively modulating cortical excitability. Of interest, anodal tDCS over the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex has been reported to significantly increase conscious awareness of errors on attention tasks in the elderly.Reference Harty, Robertson, Miniussi, Sheehy, Devine and McCreery35 Crucially, a pilot study showed that tDCS to same region increased insight in patients with schizophrenia,Reference Bose, Shivakumar, Narayanaswamy, Nawani, Subramaniam and Agarwal36 although unfortunately the study did not utilise a sham control condition.

In conclusion, the study of insight has proved to be both academically simulating and clinically fertile. As a biopsychosocial construct par excellence, the topic has the capacity to bring in new concepts and knowledge from across the spectrum of research relevant to mental disorders. I am personally looking forward to what new insights the next 30 years will bring.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank my many collaborators, including Kevin Ariyo, Steve Fleming, Javier Lopez-Morinigo, Wayne Martin, Andy McWilliams, Jen O'Connor, Gareth Owen and Elisa van der Plas.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest: None.

References

1David, AS. Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 156: 798808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Anonymous. Real insight (editorial). Lancet 1990; 336: 408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3Amador, XF, David, AS eds. Insight and Psychosis: Awareness of Illness in Schizophrenia and Related Disorders (2nd edn). Oxford University Press, 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4Lincoln, TM, Lüllmann, E, Rief, W. Correlates and long-term consequences of poor insight in patients with schizophrenia. a systematic review. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33: 1324–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Owen, GS, Richardson, G, David, AS, Szmukler, G, Hayward, P, Hotopf, M. Mental capacity, diagnosis, and insight in psychiatric inpatients: a cross sectional study. Psychol Med 2009; 39: 1389–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Diesfeld, K. Insight: unpacking the concept in mental health law. Psychiatry Psychol Law 2004; 10(1): 6370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7Fleming, SM, Weil, RS, Nagy, Z, Dolan, RJ, Rees, G. Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 2010; 329: 1541–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Fleming, SM, Ryu, J, Golfinos, JG, Blackmon, KE. Domain-specific impairment in metacognitive accuracy following anterior prefrontal lesions. Brain 2104; 137: 2811–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9Rouault, M, Seowa, T, Gillan, CM, Fleming, SM. Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance. Biol Psychiatry 2018; 84: 443–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10David, AS, Bedford, N, Wiffen, B, Gilleen, J. Failures of metacognition and lack of insight in neuropsychiatric disorders. Phil Trans R Soc B 2012; 267: 1379–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11van der Plas, E, David, AS, Fleming, SM. Advice-taking as a bridge between decision neuroscience and mental capacity. Int J Law Psychiat, in press.Google Scholar
12Beck, AT, Baruch, E, Balter, JM, Steer, RA, Warman, DM. A new instrument for measuring insight: the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale. Schizophr Res 2004; 68: 319–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Van Camp, LSC, Sabbe, BGC, Oldenburg, JFE. Cognitive insight: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2017; 55: 1224.10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14O'Connor, JA, Ellett, L, Ajnakina, O, Schoeler, T, Kollliakou, A, Trotta, A, et al. Can cognitive insight predict symptom remission in a first episode psychosis cohort? BMC Psychiatry 2017; 17: 54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15Murri, MB, Respino, M, Innamorati, M, Cervetti, A, Calcagno, P, Pompili, M, et al. Is good insight associated with depression among patients with schizophrenia? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2015; 162: 234–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16Lopez-Morinigo, JD, Di Forti, M, Ajnakina, O. Insight and risk of suicidal behaviour in two first-episode psychosis cohorts: effects of previous suicide attempts and depression. Schizophr Res 2019; 204: 80–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Philipp, R, Kriston, L, Lanio, J, Kühne, F, Härter, M, Moritz, S, Meistert, R. Effectiveness of metacognitive interventions for mental disorders in adults—A systematic review and meta-analysis (METACOG). Clin Psychol Psychother 2019; 26: 227–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18Vohs, JL, Leonhardt, BL, James, AV, Francis, MM, Breier, A, Mehdiyoun, N, et al. Metacognitive reflection and insight therapy for early psychosis: a preliminary study of a novel integrative psychotherapy. Schizophr Res 2018; 195: 428–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19De Jong, S, van Donkersgoed, R, Timmerman, M, Aan het Rot, M, Wunderink, L, Arends, J, et al. Metacognitive reflection and insight therapy (MERIT) for patients with schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2019; 49: 303–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Pijnenborg, GHM, Vos, AE, Timmerman, ME, van der Gaag, M, Sportel, BE, Arends, J, et al. Social cognitive group treatment for impaired insight in psychosis: a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Schizophr Res 2019; 206: 362–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Wiffen, BDR, Rabinowitz, J, Lex, A, David, AS. Correlates, change and ‘state or trait’ properties of insight in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2010; 122: 94103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Pijnenborg, GHM, van Donkersgoed, RJM, David, AS, Aleman, A. Changes in insight during treatment for psychotic disorders: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2013; 144: 109–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Pijnenborg, GH, Timmerman, ME, Derks, EM, Fleischhacker, WW, Kahn, RS, Aleman, A. Different effects of antipsychotic drugs on insight in first episode schizophrenia: data from the European First-Episode Schizophrenia Trial (EUFEST). Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2015; 25: 808–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24Béland, S, Makowski, C, Konsztowicz, S, Buchy, L, Chakravarty, MM, Lepage, M. Clarifying associations between cortical thickness, subcortical structures, and a comprehensive assessment of clinical insight in enduring schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2019; 204: 245–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25Morgan, KD, Dazzan, P, Morgan, C, Lappin, J, Hutchinson, G, Sucking, J, et al. Insight, grey matter and cognitive function in first-onset psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 141–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Asmal, L, du Plessis, S, Vink, M, Fouche, J-P, Chiliza, B, Emsley, R. Insight and white matter fractional anisotropy in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2017; 183: 8894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Ćurčić-Blake, B, van der Meer, L, Pijnenborg, GH, David, AS, Aleman, A. Insight and psychosis: functional and anatomical brain connectivity and self-reflection in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36: 4859–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28van der Meer, L, Costafreda, SC, Aleman, A, David, AS. Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010; 34: 935–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29Holt, DJ, Cassidy, BS, Andrews-Hanna, JR, Lee, SM, Coombs, G, Goff, DC, et al. An anterior-to-posterior shift in midline cortical activity in schizophrenia during self-reflection. Biol Psychiat 2011; 69: 415–23.10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Bedford, N, Surguladze, S, Giampietro, V, Brammer, MJ, David, AS. Self-evaluation in schizophrenia: an fMRI study with implications for the understanding of insight. BMC Psychiatry; 21012(12): 106.Google Scholar
31van der Meer, L, de Vos, AE, Stiekema, APM, Pijnenborg, GHM, van Tol, M-J, Nolen, WA, et al. Insight in schizophrenia: involvement of self-reflection networks? Schizophr Bull 2013; 39: 1288–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Gerretsen, P, Menon, M, Mamo, DC, Fervaha, G, Remington, G, Pollock, BG, et al. Impaired insight into illness and cognitive insight in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: resting state functional connectivity. Schizophr Res 2014; 160: 4350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Gerretsen, P, Takeuchi, H, Ozzoude, M, Graff-Guerrero, A, Uchida, H. Insight into illness and its relationship to illness severity, cognition and estimated antipsychotic dopamine receptor occupancy in schizophrenia: an antipsychotic dose reduction study. Psychiatry Res 2017; 251: 20–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34Xavier, RM, Vorderstrasse, A, Keefe, RSE, Dungan, JR. Genetic correlates of insight in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2108; 195: 290–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35Harty, S, Robertson, IH, Miniussi, C, Sheehy, OC, Devine, CA, McCreery, S, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation over right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances error awareness in older age. J Neurosci 2010; 34: 3646–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36Bose, A, Shivakumar, V, Narayanaswamy, JC, Nawani, H, Subramaniam, A, Agarwal, SM, et al. Insight facilitation with add-on tDCS in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2014; 156: 63–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.