Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:21:31.544Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of childhood exposure to a natural disaster on adult mental health: 20-year longitudinal follow-up study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Alexander C. McFarlane
Affiliation:
The Centre for Military and Veterans' Health, University of Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
Miranda Van Hooff*
Affiliation:
The Centre for Military and Veterans' Health, University of Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
*
Miranda Van Hooff, Centre for Military and Veterans' Health, Level 2/122 Frome Street, Adelaide, South Australia 5000, Australia. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Studies examining the impact of childhood disaster exposure on the development of adult psychopathology report increased rates of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other psychopathology.

Aims

To examine the rates of PTSD and other lifetime DSM–IV disorders in adults exposed to an Australian bushfire disaster as children in 1983 using a matched control sample recruited at the time of the original study.

Method

A total of 1011 adults recruited from an original sample of 1531 were assessed 20 years following the fires using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI–2.1).

Results

Results showed only a small direct impact of the fires on adult psychiatric morbidity. A higher proportion of bushfire survivors met criteria for ‘any DSM–IV disorder’ and ‘any anxiety’.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that the long-term impact of a prominent trauma in childhood should be assessed in the context of other lifetime trauma in order to provide a more accurate account of PTSD prevalence rates.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2009 

A number of longitudinal studies examine the psychological outcome of experiencing a disaster in childhood. Reference Goenjian, Pynoos, Steinberg, Najarian, Asarnow and Karayan1Reference Goenjian, Walling, Steinberg, Karayan, Najarian and Pynoos18 To our knowledge only three of these studies, however, have followed the disaster-affected sample into adulthood and have included a matched control sample. Green et al, Reference Green, Grace, Vary, Kramer, Gleser and Leonard7 conducted a 17-year follow-up of 99 survivors of the Buffalo Creek Dam collapse in 1972. Yule et al Reference Yule, Bolton, Udwin, Boyle, O'Ryan and Nurrish8 and Bolton et al Reference Bolton, O'Ryan, Udwin, Boyle and Yule9 conducted a 5- to 8-year follow-up of 216 survivors of the sinking of the cruise ship Jupiter in Greek waters in 1988. Finally, Morgan and colleagues Reference Morgan, Scourfield, Williams, Jasper and Lewis17 conducted a 33-year follow-up of 41 survivors of the 1966 ‘Aberfan’ disaster, in which a coal slagheap collapsed onto a primary school in South Wales killing 116 children. These studies reported significantly higher rates of lifetime post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in disaster survivors (34–51.5%) compared with controls (3.4–20%). Reference Green, Grace, Vary, Kramer, Gleser and Leonard7,Reference Yule, Bolton, Udwin, Boyle, O'Ryan and Nurrish8,Reference Morgan, Scourfield, Williams, Jasper and Lewis17 Current prevalence rates of PTSD in survivors ranged from 4 to 29%. Reference Green, Grace, Vary, Kramer, Gleser and Leonard7,Reference Yule, Bolton, Udwin, Boyle, O'Ryan and Nurrish8,Reference Morgan, Scourfield, Williams, Jasper and Lewis17 Only one study reported significantly higher lifetime rates of anxiety disorder and affective disorder in disaster survivors compared with controls. Reference Bolton, O'Ryan, Udwin, Boyle and Yule9

There are two major methodological limitations to these studies. First, survivors are compared with a matched comparison group recruited at the time of follow-up rather than at the time of the original disaster, raising questions about the adequacy and matching of the comparison samples. Furthermore, not one of these studies incorporated an assessment of the role or impact of other lifetime trauma in the disaster-affected population. Given the significant rates of lifetime trauma exposure found in general population studies, including a PTSD rate of 20% from other traumas among the comparison sample in the Aberfan study, Reference Morgan, Scourfield, Williams, Jasper and Lewis17 the contribution of additional trauma exposure needs to be taken into account in long-term studies of disaster survivors. Reference Creamer, Burgess and McFarlane19,Reference Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes and Nelson20

The aim of the current study is to investigate the adult psychiatric outcomes of childhood exposure to a major Australian bushfire in the context of other lifetime trauma by utilising an unexposed control sample recruited at the time of the initial disaster.

Method

Sample

The bushfire-exposed cohort

The original bushfire-exposed cohort consisted of 806 children attending primary school in an easily demarcated region of South Australia that was vastly devastated by the Ash Wednesday Fires on 16 February 1983. Participants were recruited as children in the 2-year period following the fires, with initial findings being reported elsewhere. Reference McFarlane21Reference McFarlane, Policansky and Irwin23 Of these 806 children, 440 (54.6%) completed the full follow-up protocol and a further 540 (67%) completed at least one part of the protocol. All but one of the schools attended by the children on the day of the fires were physically threatened by the bushfire, with the fire encroaching on to the school grounds. The mean age of the bushfire sample at the time of the disaster was 8.44 years (range 3.23–13.49) and at follow-up was 28.64 years (range 23–34).

The control sample

In 1985, 725 primary school children from a neighbouring region in South Australia that was not directly devastated by the fires were recruited as a comparison sample. The schools attended by these children were both unaffected by the bushfire and socio-demographically matched to the schools in the fire-affected region. Reference McFarlane, Policansky and Irwin23 Of these 725 children, 382 (52.7%) completed the full follow-up study protocol and 471 (65%) completed at least one part of the protocol. The mean age of the control sample at the time of the disaster was 7.39 years (range 1.67–13.11) and at follow-up was 27.66 years (range 22–33). To ensure that socioeconomic advantage/disadvantage had not emerged between the regions in the interim 20-year period that could account for differences, the Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA) obtained from the Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) was compared for the control group council region and the two bush-fire council regions in 2001. 24

A register of all primary school children in 1983–1985 was compiled using data from the original questionnaires and archived school admission records. Current contact information was obtained through the State Department of Births, Deaths and Marriages, the Australian Electoral Commission, and an online telephone directory. A National Death Registry was used to identify the deceased. Initial contact with the participants was by letter, followed by a telephone call. Consenting participants completed a 1-hour telephone interview and a self-report booklet.

Interviewers were trained researchers with a Bachelor of Psychology and extensive experience in telephone recruitment, interviewing and psychiatric assessment. The appropriate ethical clearance and informed consent was obtained.

Instruments

Demographics

Demographic questions were derived from the 1997 Australian National Survey of Health and Wellbeing 25 and are reported using standards employed by the ABS and the Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW). Occupations were categorised using The Australian Standard Classification of Occupations (ASCO). 26 The proportion of participants residing in metropolitan, rural and remote locations in Australia was determined using the Rural, Remote Metropolitan Area Classification (RRMA) that gives an index of residential remoteness based on distance to service centres as well as distance from other people.

Adult psychiatric morbidity

Lifetime and 1-month point prevalence rates of DSM–IV 27 disorder were assessed using a computerised version of the fully structured, standardised and comprehensive Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). 28 Disorders classified by DSM–IV were subgrouped as follows: any depressive disorder (major depression – single episode, major depression – recurrent, depressive disorder not otherwise specified and dysthymia); any anxiety disorder (panic disorder, panic with agoraphobia, agoraphobia, specific phobia – animal, specific phobia – environmental, specific phobia – blood, specific phobia – situational, specific phobia – other, social phobia, obsessive–compulsive disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, anxiety not otherwise specified, and PTSD – based on worst event); and any eating disorder (anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and eating disorder not otherwise specified). Current and lifetime DSM–IV PTSD was examined in all participants in relation to the event rated subjectively by the participant to be their ‘worst lifetime traumatic event’. This event was chosen by the participant from a standard list of 10 Criterion-A events from the CIDI. 28 These were direct combat, life-threatening accident, fire, flood or natural disaster, witnessed someone being badly injured or killed, rape, sexual molestation, serious physical attack or assault, threatened with a weapon/held captive/kidnapped, tortured or victim of terrorists, and any other stressful event. In addition, the researchers included seven other event types (domestic violence, witnessed domestic violence, threatened/harassed without a weapon, finding a dead body, witnessing someone die by suicide or attempt suicide, child physical abuse, child emotional abuse). These events were based on a systematic recoding of the ‘other’ trauma category in a random community sample. Reference Goldney, Wilson, Dal Grande, Fisher and McFarlane29 Participants were asked whether they had experienced any of these events and whether they had ever experienced a great shock as a result of any of these events happening to someone close to them. They were then asked to nominate which of these events was their worst lifetime event. Post-traumatic stress disorder was assessed in reference to this self-nominated worst lifetime event. This methodology is in contrast to previous longitudinal studies on disaster-affected populations, which predominantly focus on the disaster in the exposed population and the worst lifetime traumatic event in the controls. The purpose of assessing PTSD in this way was to determine how the disaster was subjectively rated in relation to other traumatic life events that the person had experienced and whether the experience of other events has an impact on the prevalence of PTSD in relation to the bushfire.

Lifetime and current prevalence of bushfire-related PTSD

In addition to the assessment of PTSD in relation to the participant's worst lifetime event, the CIDI PTSD section was also completed for the second and third worst events to allow a more detailed examination of the prevalence of lifetime and current DSM–IV PTSD. This method also allowed an assessment of the bushfire-related PTSD, particularly how the participants rank the bushfire in the hierarchy of their stressful experiences. If the participants nominated the bushfire as their worst event, they were included in Fire Group 1. If the bushfire-exposed group did not nominate the bushfire as their worst lifetime event, but instead nominated another event as their worst, PTSD symptom questions were first asked in specific relation to this other event (Fire Group 2). This group represents individuals with PTSD symptoms related to some other worst event such as a motor vehicle accident in the group who had been exposed to the fires. Furthermore, Fire Group 3 represents all bushfire-related cases independent of whether this was rated as either their worst, second or third worst lifetime event. Therefore, Group 3 included all those in Fire Group 1 where the disaster had been their worst experience and also those in Fire Group 2 who nominated the bushfire as their second or third worst lifetime event. In total, therefore, 352 (67%) of those originally exposed to the bushfires were questioned as to whether they had experienced PTSD symptoms as a direct result of the bushfire at some point in their lives. Bushfire-exposed participants were subgrouped in this way to maximise sample numbers for analysis of bushfire-related PTSD.

Current self-reported levels of PTSD-related distress

Current distress associated with PTSD symptoms was examined at 20-year follow-up using the self-report Impact of Events Scale–Revised (IES–R). Reference Weiss, Marmar, Wilson and Keane30 Symptom questions were anchored to the bushfire in the survivor group and their most stressful self-nominated life event in the controls. Scores were also added to produce an overall total score and three symptom scores: intrusion, avoidance and hyperarousal.

Alcohol consumption and problem drinking

This was examined using the AUDIT (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test). Reference Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente and Grant31 This instrument consists of 10 questions to examine the quantity and frequency of alcohol consumption, drinking behaviour and the reactions or problems related to alcohol (score range 0–36). The version we used has been slightly modified for the Australian context by the Centre for Drug and Alcohol Studies. Scores of 7–12 indicated harmful or hazardous drinking, and a score of 13 or greater indicated possible alcohol dependence.

Analysis

Log linear analyses (χ2-tests) and independent sample t-tests were performed on selected baseline data to investigate differences between the responders and non-responders. The strength of these associations was calculated using the phi coefficient (for χ2 tests) and effect sizes (for t-tests). Primary unadjusted analysis of the effects of childhood exposure to the bushfire on demographics, alcohol consumption, and lifetime and current DSM–IV disorder was performed using chi-square analyses (for categorical variables) and t-tests or linear regression (for continuous variables). To account for the possible confounding effects of age and gender (known to be strong predictors of adult psychopathology), adjusted P-values were calculated using generalised estimating equations for demographics and lifetime psychopathology. In cases where the dependent variable was binary, generalised estimating equations were logistic regressions. For skewed sampling distributions (IES–R), the differences in medians were calculated using non-parametric tests of significance. To obtain relative risks, a binomial generalised estimating equation was used with log link, adjusting for gender and age. Analyses were performed using Statistical Analysis Systems (SAS) (version 9.1) for Windows.

Results

Response rates

Table 1 shows the response and completion rates for the entire sample. Of the original sample, 714 (88.6%) of the bushfire-exposed group and 624 (86.1%) of the non-exposed group were relocated around the 20-year anniversary of the fires. Of those who were contacted, 440 (61.62%) of the bushfire-exposed group and 382 (61.21%) of the non-exposed group completed the entire protocol, and 529 bushfire-exposed and 464 control participants were interviewed using the CIDI.

Table 1 Response rates for the entire sample

Total Bushfire group Control group
n % n % n %
Initially recruited 1531 100.0 806 100.0 725 100.0
Follow-up data
   Any data 1011 66.0 540 67.0 471 65.0
   All data 822 53.7 440 54.6 382 52.7
   Booklet data 838 54.7 450 55.8 388 53.5
   Interview data (any) 996 65.1 530 65.8 466 64.3
   Composite International Diagnostic Interview data 993 64.9 529 65.6 464 64.0
Contacted but no follow-up
   Deceased 20 1.3 14 1.7 6 0.8
   Withdrawn 111 7.3 60 7.4 51 7.0
   Not interested 148 9.7 77 9.6 71 9.8
   Other 48 3.1 23 2.9 25 3.4
   Not contacted 193 12.6 92 11.4 101 13.9

Responders v. non-responders

A comparison of the 1011 responders and the 520 non-responders was carried out using demographic data collected during the initial phases of the study. Overall, there were few differences between responders and non-responders on a number of variables, with non-responders more likely to be male (χ2(1,1531) = 4.05, P<0.05) and to be younger in age (t(1529) = −2.28, P<0.05). The strength of these relationships however was weak (phi coefficient: gender 0.05; effect sizes: age −0.12).

Demographic characteristics of the follow-up sample

Online Table DS1 reports demographic results adjusted for age at follow-up. Demographic differences between the fire-exposed and control participants suggest that exposure to the bushfire may have made the exposed group less likely to extend their education and their careers, and to be less socially mobile. Overall, a significantly greater proportion of bushfire-exposed individuals compared with controls had children (χ2(1,838) = 4.64, P<0.05), lived in rural areas (χ2(1,838) = 8.38, P<0.01) and were employed as production workers (χ2(1,838) = 6.66, P<0.01). In contrast, a higher proportion of the control participants had completed high-school education (χ2(1,838) = 9.56, P<0.01) and were currently employed in associate professional roles (χ2(1,838) = 5.43, P<0.05). These differences could not be explained by differences in the advantage/disadvantage index (SEIFA) that may have emerged since the disaster between the control and fire-affected regions as measured in 2001 (F = 2.68, d.f. = 2, P = 0.08). In the earlier phase of this study, it was noted that the educational performance of the symptomatic group of exposed children significantly declined in the aftermath of the fire. Reference McFarlane, Policansky and Irwin23 Hence, the psychopathological measures of outcome alone do not entirely represent the disadvantage experienced by this disaster-affected group.

Overall, participants in the bushfire-exposed group were slightly older at follow-up, as a result of the fact that the comparison sample was recruited 1 year later than the bushfire-exposed sample. Hence, this is an artefact of the initial study design and has been explained elsewhere. Reference McFarlane, Policansky and Irwin23

Lifetime and current psychopathology

In total, 36.7% of the bushfire-exposed participants and 31.7% of the controls met criteria for any DSM–IV psychiatric disorder during their lifetime, and 15.2% of the bushfire-exposed group and 11% of controls met criteria for a disorder in the past month (Table 2). Only lifetime rates of ‘any anxiety, excluding PTSD’ (χ2(1,993) = 5.39, P = 0.01) were significantly more prevalent in the bushfire-exposed group than in the controls. A higher proportion of bushfire-exposed participants compared with controls met criteria for ‘any disorder’ (χ2(1,993) = 4.32, P<0.05) and ‘any anxiety’ (χ2(1,993) = 5.66, P<0.05) in the past month. When the individual DSM–IV diagnoses were examined, the only disorder that was more prevalent in the disaster-exposed group was a lifetime history of specific phobia (environmental type) (relative risk (RR) = 1.92, 95% CI 1.11–3.34, P = 0.02), whereas current environmental phobia only approached significance (RR = 2.27, 95% CI 0.97–7.62, P = 0.06).

Table 2 Lifetime and point (1-month) prevalence rates for DSM–IV disorders in bushfire survivors and controls a

Lifetime Point (1-month)
Bushfire group (n = 529) Control group (n = 464) Bushfire group (n = 529) Control group (n = 464)
Adult symptoms n (%) n (%) RR (95% CI) P n (%) n (%) RR (95% CI) P
PTSD (worst event) 41 (7.8) 27 (5.8) 1.36 (0.84–2.19) 0.21 17 (3.2) 11 (2.4) 1.35 (0.63–2.90) 0.44
Any DSM–IV disorder 194 (36.7) 147 (31.7) 1.14 (0.96–1.36) 0.14 80 (15.2) 51 (11.0) 1.42 (1.02–1.98) 0.04
Any depressive disorder 127 (24.0) 99 (21.3) 1.11 (0.88–1.40) 0.38 24 (4.5) 20 (4.3) 1.00 (0.55–1.81) 1.00
Any anxiety disorder, excluding PTSD 114 (21.5) 74 (16.0) 1.37 (1.05–1.78) 0.02 57 (10.8) 32 (6.9) 1.66 (1.09–2.53) 0.02
Any eating disorder 5 (0.9) 6 (1.3) 0.81 (0.24–2.70) 0.73 5 (0.9) 6 (1.3) 0.81 (0.24–2.70) 0.73

RR, relative risk; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder

a. Adjusted for current age and gender

Using the standard CIDI method of nominating the worst lifetime event, there were no significant differences in the rates of lifetime or 1-month prevalence of PTSD between the bushfire-exposed group and the controls. For the bushfire-exposed individuals, the event most commonly nominated as their worst lifetime event was a ‘natural disaster’ (31.6%), suggesting the enduring impact of the bushfire, followed by ‘having a family or friend in a life-threatening accident’ (12.5%) and ‘witnessing someone being badly injured or killed’ (10.2%). The event most frequently nominated as the worst in the control group was ‘being in a life-threatening accident’ (11.2%) followed by ‘witnessing someone being badly injured or killed’ (8.6%) and ‘having a family or friend in a life threatening accident’ (8.2%).

Lifetime and current PTSD in relation to the bushfire

Using the more thorough method of assessing PTSD from the three worst traumatic experiences, 352 (67%) of the 529 bushfire survivors nominated the bushfire as one of their three worst lifetime events: 30.4% (n = 161) as their worst event, 21.4% (n = 113) as their second worst event and 14.7% (n = 78) as their third worst lifetime event. As previously reported, there were no significant differences between the bushfire group and the controls in the prevalence of PTSD resulting from the self-nominated worst lifetime event (Table 3). Interestingly, bushfire-exposed participants who nominated the bushfire as their worst lifetime event were at a lower risk of developing lifetime and current PTSD from the bushfire than the bushfire-exposed individuals who nominated some other event as their worst event (Table 4). In addition, bushfire-exposed participants who nominated the fire as one of their worst three events were less likely to develop PTSD from the fire than were the controls in relation to their self-nominated worst lifetime event. Importantly, of the 161 bushfire-exposed individuals who nominated the bushfire as their worst event, 62.7% (n = 101) had reported the bushfire as their only lifetime event. The percentage in both the bushfire-exposed and the control population who had been exposed to only one event was similar being 21% (n = 113) and 23% (n = 107) respectively. Interestingly, a further 114 (24.5%) participants in the control group reported not being exposed to a single traumatic event during their lifetime. Therefore those in the disaster-exposed group who nominated the fire as their worst event appeared to have a lower total lifetime exposure to traumatic stress, explaining the apparent paradox of their lower rates of PTSD. This contrasts to the bushfire-exposed group who nominated another event as their worst event and who had a greater risk of PTSD (Table 4) than the controls. This group appeared to have been sensitised to subsequent trauma exposure by their experience of the bushfire.

Table 3 Number and proportion of participants satisfying each DSM–IV post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) diagnostic criteria in relation to bushfire and worst lifetime event

Bushfire survivors a (n = 352) Bushfire survivors b (n = 529) Control participants b (n = 464)
DSM–IV PTSD criteria n % n % n %
A2: Fear, helplessness or horror 218 61.9 405 76.6 285 61.4
B: Re-experiencing 118 33.5 303 57.3 236 50.9
C: Avoidance 11 3.1 90 17.0 68 14.7
D: Hyperarousal 64 18.2 203 38.4 161 34.7
E: Duration more than 1 month 56 15.9 205 38.8 161 34.7
F: Significant distress or impairment 13 3.7 77 14.6 49 10.6
PTSD lifetime 6 1.7 41 7.8 27 5.8
PTSD current 0 0 17 3.2 11 2.4

a. PTSD symptoms arising from the 1983 bushfires

Table 4 Relative risk (RR) of lifetime and current post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in bushfire participants (in relation to the bushfire and a self-nominated worst lifetime event) and control participants (in relation to a self-nominated worst lifetime event)

Lifetime PTSD Point prevalence (1-month PTSD)
Contrasts performed RR Adjusted RR 95% CI P RR Adjusted RR 95% CI P
Model 1
   Fire group 1 a v. fire group 2 b 0.248 0.229 0.08–0.63 0.004 0.143 0.133 0.02–0.99 0.049
   Fire group 1 a v. controls c 0.428 0.413 0.15–1.17 0.095 0.263 0.248 0.03–1.92 0.181
   Fire group 2 b v. controls c 1.725 1.804 1.11–2.93 0.017 1.831 1.869 0.86–4.05 0.113
   Fire group 1 a and 2 b v. controls c 0.859 0.863 0.46–1.62 0.648 0.693 0.681 0.21–2.20 0.521
Model 2
   Fire group 3 d v. controls c 0.294 0.297 0.12–0.72 0.008 0.361 0.349 0.10–0.27 0.011

a. Fire group 1: bushfire-exposed participants who nominated the bushfire as their worst lifetime event (n = 161)

b. Fire group 2: bushfire-exposed participants who nominated an event other than the bushfire as their worst lifetime event (n = 368)

c. Controls: control participants who self-nominated a traumatic life event as their worst (n = 464)

d. Fire group 3: bushfire-exposed participants who nominated the bushfire as one of their worst three events (n = 352)

b. PTSD symptoms arising from the participant's self-nominated worst lifetime event (includes bushfire when nominated as worst event n = 161)

Table 3 demonstrates that the C criteria (avoidance) and the F criterion (distress) were the diagnostic criteria that were those least frequently endorsed, and hence were the ‘gate-keepers’ for the diagnosis of PTSD. This effect was most pronounced in the bushfire-exposed group in relation to the nominated trauma of the fire, where only 3.1% satisfied the C (avoidance and estrangement) criteria, in contrast to the worst trauma where this was met by 17% of the sample.

Current bushfire-related distress

Twenty-seven per cent of the bushfire-exposed group reported no current bushfire-related distress (IES–R = 0) at the follow-up assessment. In general, total scores on the IES–R were low, with 80% of the bushfire-exposed participants scoring 10 or below. Intrusion symptoms were most common affecting 66% of the population. Hyperarousal symptoms affected only 28% of the sample. Of those who reported some distress, the two most commonly endorsed symptoms were ‘any reminders brought back feelings about it’ (54.2%) and ‘pictures popped into my mind’ (46.4%).

The total IES–R scores were significantly lower in the bushfire-exposed individuals (in relation to the bushfire) than in the non-disaster-exposed controls (in relation to their self-nominated ‘most stressful and upsetting event life’) (total IES–R score: bushfire group mean = 6.92 (s.d. = 11.0); control group mean = 10.08 (s.d. = 14.0), z = 3.03, P = 0.002) with similarly significant differences existing on the avoidance and hyperarousal subscales but not the intrusion subscales. In the control sample, death of a family member or friend was the event most commonly reported as being most stressful (15.8%). Only 8.3% of the unexposed population reported never having experienced a stressful life event.

Alcohol consumption and problem drinking

There were no significant differences in the total AUDIT score between the bushfire-exposed group and controls, or in the proportion of participants classified as ‘harmful or hazardous drinkers’ (30.8% of bushfire-exposed individuals compared with 31.5% of controls (χ2(1,806) = 0.46, P = 0.831)) or ‘alcohol dependent’ (14.2% of bushfire-exposed group compared with 10.9% of controls (χ2(1,806) = 1.95, P = 0.162)).

Discussion

This study is the first to follow-up a large cohort of primary school children exposed to a natural disaster, using a matched control sample recruited as children at the time of the original study, as well as being the largest long-term longitudinal study to follow a disaster-exposed cohort from childhood. Overall, the increased rates of lifetime and current ‘any anxiety disorder’ and ‘any disorder’ in the bushfire-exposed participants were relatively small, despite the fact that the bushfire-exposed children were rated as more symptomatic than the controls by their parents following the fires. Reference McFarlane, Policansky and Irwin23 The enduring disadvantage of the disaster was also apparent in the educational and occupational disadvantage of the bushfire-exposed group (online Table DS1). However, a caveat of this later finding is that we did not statistically control for the individual socioeconomic status of each child at the time of the disaster as the comparison group was recruited by matching indices of regional social advantage. In summary, the disaster appears to have had only a small long-term effect on anxiety rather than depressive disorders, despite the major losses of this community. Reference McFarlane32

These findings contrast to the higher rates of depression and anxiety following the long-term studies of the Aberfan Reference Morgan, Scourfield, Williams, Jasper and Lewis17 and Jupiter disasters. Reference Bolton, O'Ryan, Udwin, Boyle and Yule9 Interestingly, the current prevalence of anxiety and depressive disorders in the bushfire-exposed participants and controls were similar to the Australian community. Reference Andrews, Henderson and Hall33 The similar prevalence rates in the exposed and control groups are likely to be as a result of the important effect of traumatic events, other than disasters in the general community, as was the case in both groups. The effect of non-disaster-related trauma has not been addressed in these previous follow-up studies.

There were no significant differences in the lifetime or 1-month prevalence of PTSD between bushfire survivors and the unexposed controls in relation to their self-nominated worst event, implying a degree of consistency in both groups with respect to how they respond to trauma over time. The event most frequently nominated by bushfire-exposed individuals as the worst event was a ‘natural disaster’ (31.6%), but the remainder nominated another traumatic event, many relating to motor vehicle accidents, which had generally occurred at an older age. The association between multiple exposure to traumatic events and increased prevalence and severity of PTSD, suggesting a cumulative risk model arising from multiple trauma exposures, has been well validated in the literature. Reference Green, Goodman, Krupnick, Corcoran, Petty and Stockton34,Reference Breslau, Peterson and Schultz35 This effect was apparent in the bushfire-exposed participants who nominated an event other than the bushfire as their worst lifetime event, with this group having a greater risk of PTSD than the controls. This finding highlights the importance of assessing other traumatic exposures in the long-term follow-up of disaster-affected populations to get true prevalence estimates of trauma-related psychopathology.

Seventy-five per cent of the bushfire-exposed group still reported some degree of distress in relation to the bushfires 20 years on. These distressing intrusions, measured by the IES–R, which have been found to drive the hyperarousal and avoidance PTSD symptoms in a group of emergency firefighters exposed to the same disaster, Reference McFarlane36 may be indicative of an ongoing risk of dysregulated affect and arousal. However, despite this distress, low rates of PTSD in relation to the bushfire were observed in the bushfire-exposed participants, although the rates of intrusive and hyperarousal symptoms were substantial (Table 3), indicative of subsyndromal PTSD. Only six of the bushfire survivors (1.7% of those questioned in relation to the bushfire, 1.1% of the entire sample) met full diagnostic criteria for lifetime PTSD in relation to the fires. This is in stark contrast to other longitudinal studies, which have reported lifetime rates of PTSD in disaster survivors that have ranged from 34.0 to 51.5%. Reference Green, Grace, Vary, Kramer, Gleser and Leonard7,Reference Yule, Bolton, Udwin, Boyle, O'Ryan and Nurrish8,Reference Morgan, Scourfield, Williams, Jasper and Lewis17 The severity of the loss of life in the Aberfan and the Buffalo Creek disaster may explain these differences but this was not the case in the Jupiter sinking where four lives were lost.

A methodological strength in this study is the use of a matched comparison group collected at the time of initial recruitment because the significant differences emerging in other studies may be as a result of the bias related to the retrospective nature of the recruitment of the control populations. Furthermore, the failure of previous studies to adequately assess non-disaster trauma exposure in the exposed and control groups may have led to underestimation of PTSD. Therefore future studies should explicitly assess PTSD in the disaster-exposed group in relation to both a self-nominated worst lifetime event and the disaster itself, in order to gain a more accurate representation of the true effects of the disaster in the context of other lifetime trauma.

A series of other methodological issues require consideration in the interpretation of these results. First, clinician administered measures, such as those used in the previous studies of disaster victims, have more potential for interviewer bias than the CIDI that was used in this study. Reference Grayson, O'Toole, Marshall, Schureck, Dobson and Ffrench37 This potential arises as a consequence of the flexibility for clinician interpretation, which can lead to bias towards the hypothesised effect, similar to the substantial bias found in unmasked clinician-rated assessments in treatment trials. Reference Schulz, Chalmers and Altman38,Reference Erdman, Klein and Greist39 As a consequence, the previous studies may have had more potential for bias in rating PTSD symptoms leading to finding the presumed association with the disaster. Second, the higher rates of PTSD in the Aberfan study may have been partly as a result of the use of ICD–10 40 criteria, whereas the current study used DSM–IV criteria. The 12-month PTSD prevalence rates using the ICD–10 criteria have been found to be double those obtained with DSM–IV criteria. Reference Peters, Issakidis, Slade and Andrews41,Reference Peters, Slade and Andrews42

Third, in contrast to the Aberfan and Buffalo Creek follow-up studies, the response bias issues in recruiting the sample were significantly less in our study due to the interview of 65% of the original sample. A limitation of the current paper arises from dealing with disaster exposure as being present or absent rather that looking at the gradient of exposure, an issue that will be addressed in further papers. Also, although the finding of social and economic disadvantage in the disaster-affected group was consistent with the progressive educational underachievement that emerged in the 2 years following the disaster, future research should control for parental education and occupation as well as the child's IQ at the time of the disaster. Finally, compensation did not play a significant role Reference Peters, Issakidis, Slade and Andrews41 in the response of our sample. In contrast, the Buffalo Creek disaster and the Jupiter samples were recruited in the course of their legal action against the defendants. Reference Sayer, Spoont and Nelson43

In summary, this study provides some support for long-term effects of childhood disaster on those exposed. Importantly, however, the effects are much more subtle than has previously been reported and should be viewed through the lens of the significant impact of other lifetime traumatic events. The lack of differences between the disaster-exposed group and the comparison group do not appear to be solely a result of the disaster having little effect on the disaster-exposed group, but because of the overriding impact of other trauma in both the control group and the survivors, and the unique methodology employed in the current study to document this.

Acknowledgements

The authors also wish to acknowledge the following organisations and personnel for their assistance and support with the project: Nancy Briggs from the University of Adelaide Data Management and Analysis Centre, the Births Deaths and Marriages Registration Office of South Australia, the Australian Institute of Health and Welfare, the Australian Electoral Commission and the teachers, principals and staff from the 8 schools that participated in the study. A special thanks to all participants who generously gave their time, support and cooperation with the project.

Footnotes

This research was supported by a project grant from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC Project Grant ID 201813 and program grant 300403).

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Goenjian, AK, Pynoos, RS, Steinberg, A, Najarian, LM, Asarnow, J, Karayan, I, et al. Psychiatric comorbidity in children after the 1988 earthquake in Armenia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995; 34: 1174–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Hsu, C-C, Choong, M-Y, Yang, P, Yen, C-F. Posttraumatic stress disorder among adolescent earthquake victims in Taiwan. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002; 4: 875–81.Google Scholar
3 Goenjian, AK, Molina, L, Steinberg, AM, Fairbanks, LA, Alvarez, ML, Goenjian, HA, et al. Posttraumatic stress and depressive reactions among Nicaraguan adolescents after hurricane Mitch. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158: 788–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 La Greca, AM, Silverman, WK, Wasserstein, SB. Children's predisaster functioning as a predictor of posttraumatic stress following hurricane Andrew. J Consult Clin Psychol 1998; 66: 883–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Shaw, JA, Applegate, B, Schorr, C. Twenty-one month follow-up study of school-age children exposed to hurricane Andrew. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35: 359–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Dollinger, SJ. Lightning-strike disaster among children. Br J Med Psychol 1985; 58: 375–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Green, BL, Grace, M, Vary, MG, Kramer, TL, Gleser, GC, Leonard, AC. Children of disaster in the second decade: a 17-year follow-up of Buffalo Creek survivors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1994; 33: 71–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Yule, W, Bolton, D, Udwin, O, Boyle, S, O'Ryan, D, Nurrish, J. The long-term psychological effects of a disaster experienced in adolescence: I: the incidence and course of PTSD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41: 503–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Bolton, D, O'Ryan, D, Udwin, O, Boyle, S, Yule, W. The long-term psychological effects of a disaster experienced in adolescence: II: general psychopathology. J Child Psychiat 2000; 41: 513–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Udwin, O, Boyle, S, Yule, W, Bolton, D, O'Ryan, D. Risk factors for long-term psychological effects of a disaster experienced in adolescence: predictors of post traumatic stress disorder. J Child Psychiat 2000; 41: 969–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Terr, LC. Chowchilla revisited: the effects of psychic trauma four years after a school-bus kidnapping. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140: 1543–50.Google ScholarPubMed
12 Pfefferbaum, B, Nixon, SJ, Tucker, P, Tivis, RD, Moore, VL, Gurwitch, RH, et al. Posttraumatic stress responses in bereaved children after the Oklahoma city bombing. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38: 1372–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Nader, K, Pynoos, RS, Fairbanks, L, Frederick, C. Children's PTSD reactions one year after a sniper attack at their school. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147: 1526–30.Google ScholarPubMed
14 Pynoos, RS, Frederick, C, Nader, K, Arroyo, W, Steinberg, A, Eth, S, et al. Life threat and posttraumatic stress in school-age children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44: 1057–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Tyano, S, Iancu, I, Solomon, Z, Sever, J, Goldstein, I, Touviana, Y, et al. Seven-year follow-up of child survivors of a bus-train collision. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35: 365–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Winje, D, Ukik, A. Long-term outcome of trauma in children: the psychological consequences of a bus accident. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998; 39: 635–42.Google ScholarPubMed
17 Morgan, L, Scourfield, J, Williams, D, Jasper, A, Lewis, G. The Aberfan disaster: 33-year follow-up of survivors. Br J Psychol 2003; 182: 532–6.Google ScholarPubMed
18 Goenjian, AK, Walling, D, Steinberg, AM, Karayan, I, Najarian, LM, Pynoos, RS. A prospective study of posttraumatic stress and depressive reactions among treated and untreated adolescents 5 years after a catastrophic disaster. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 2302–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Creamer, M, Burgess, P, McFarlane, AC. Post-traumatic stress disorder: findings from the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Well-being. Psychol Med 2001; 31: 1237–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Kessler, A, Sonnega, A, Bromet, E, Hughes, M, Nelson, C. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52: 1048–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21 McFarlane, AC. Family functioning and overprotection following a natural disaster: the longitudinal effects of post-traumatic morbidity. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1987; 21: 210–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 McFarlane, AC. Posttraumatic phenomena in a longitudinal study of children following a natural disaster. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1987; 26: 764–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23 McFarlane, AC, Policansky, SK, Irwin, C. A longitudinal study of the psychological morbidity in children due to a natural disaster. Psychol Med 1987; 17: 727–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 Australian Bureau of Statistics. Census of Population and Housing: Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), Australia – Data Cube only. ABS, 2001.Google Scholar
25 Australian Bureau of Statistics. National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing of Adults: Users' Guide, 1997. Report No. 4327.0. ABS, 1999.Google Scholar
26 Australian Bureau of Statistics. Australian Standard Classification of Occupations (ASCO) (2nd edn). Report No. 1220.0. ABS, 1997.Google Scholar
27 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). APA, 1994.Google Scholar
28 World Health Organization. Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI–AUTO). WHO, 1997.Google Scholar
29 Goldney, RD, Wilson, D, Dal Grande, E, Fisher, LJ, McFarlane, AC. Suicidal ideation in a random community sample: attributable risk due to depression and psychosocial and traumatic events. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2000; 34: 98106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30 Weiss, D, Marmar, C. The impact of event scale-revised. In Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD (eds Wilson, J, Keane, T). Guilford Press, 1997.Google Scholar
31 Saunders, JB, Aasland, OG, Babor, TF, de la Fuente, JR, Grant, M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption – II. Addiction 1993; 88: 791804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32 McFarlane, A. The effects of stressful life events and disasters: research and theoretical issues. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1985; 19: 409–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Andrews, G, Henderson, S, Hall, W. Prevalence, comorbidity, disability and service utilisation. Overview of the Australian National Mental Health Survey. Br J Psychiatry 2001; 178: 145–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34 Green, BL, Goodman, LA, Krupnick, JL, Corcoran, CB, Petty, RM, Stockton, P, et al. Outcomes of single versus multiple trauma exposure in a screening sample. J Trauma Stress 2000; 13: 271–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35 Breslau, N, Peterson, EL, Schultz, LR. A second look at prior trauma and the posttraumatic stress disorder effects of subsequent trauma: a prospective epidemiological study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65: 431–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36 McFarlane, A. Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 1992; 180: 439–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Grayson, DA, O'Toole, BI, Marshall, RP, Schureck, RJ, Dobson, M, Ffrench, M, et al. Interviewer effects on epidemiologic diagnoses of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Epidemiol 1996; 144: 589–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38 Schulz, KF, Chalmers, I, Altman, DG. The landscape and lexicon of blinding in randomized trials. Ann Intern Med 2002; 136: 254–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39 Erdman, HP, Klein, MH, Greist, JH. Direct patient computer interviewing. J Consult Clin Psychol 1985; 53: 760–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40 World Health Organization. The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
41 Peters, L, Issakidis, C, Slade, T, Andrews, G. Gender differences in the prevalence of DSM–IV and ICD–10 PTSD. Psychol Med 2005; 35: 19.Google Scholar
42 Peters, L, Slade, T, Andrews, G. A comparison of ICD–10 and DSM–IV criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 1999; 12: 335–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43 Sayer, NA, Spoont, M, Nelson, D. Veterans seeking disability benefits for posttraumatic stress disorder: who applies and the self-reported meaning of disability compensation. Soc Sci Med 2004; 58: 2133–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Response rates for the entire sample

Figure 1

Table 2 Lifetime and point (1-month) prevalence rates for DSM–IV disorders in bushfire survivors and controlsa

Figure 2

Table 3 Number and proportion of participants satisfying each DSM–IV post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) diagnostic criteria in relation to bushfire and worst lifetime eventRelative risk (RR) of lifetime and current post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in bushfire participants (in relation to the bushfire and a self-nominated worst lifetime event) and control participants (in relation to a self-nominated worst lifetime event)

Figure 3

Table 4 Relative risk (RR) of lifetime and current post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in bushfire participants (in relation to the bushfire and a self-nominated worst lifetime event) and control participants (in relation to a self-nominated worst lifetime event)

Supplementary material: PDF

McFarlane and Van Hooff supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1

Download McFarlane and Van Hooff supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 43.6 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.