Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T11:16:43.807Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Head Circumference as a Measure of Cognitive Reserve

Association with Severity of Impairment in Alzheimer's Disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

A. B. Graves*
Affiliation:
Battelle Memorial Institute, Seattle, WA 98105, currently at the Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of South Florida
J. A. Mortimer
Affiliation:
Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Geriatric Research Education and Clinical Center, Minneapolis, MN 55417, and University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, currently at the Institute on Aging, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33612
E. B. Larson
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
A. Wenzlow
Affiliation:
Battelle Centers for Public Health Research and Evaluation, 4000 N.E. 41st Street, Seattle, WA 98105
J. D. Bowen
Affiliation:
Division of Neurology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
W. C. McCormick
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine and Geriatrics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
*
Amy B. Graves, Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, College of Public Health, MDC-56, University of South Florida, 13201 Bruce B. Downs Blvd., Tampa, FL 33612-3805, USA

Abstract

Background

Recent studies suggest that larger brain size may offer some protection against the clinical manifestations of Alzheimer's disease. However, this association has not been investigated in population-based studies.

Method

The relationship between head circumference, a measure of premorbid brain size, and score on the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI) was studied in a population of 1985 Japanese–Americans aged 65+ living in King County, Washington, USA.

Results

After adjusting for age, sex and education, head circumference was positively associated with CASI score (b=3.8, 95% CI: 2.2, 5.4; P=0.0000), but not with diagnosis of probable AD (odds ratio=0.87, 95% CI: 0.33, 1.87). When the data were stratified by AD status, no association was seen among controls (b=1.6, 95% CI: – 1.7, 5.1; P=0.4), whereas a strong effect was present among cases (b=35.3, 95% CI: 12.2, 58.4: P=0.006).

Conclusions

These results suggest that persons with AD with smaller head circumference either had the disease longer or progressed more rapidly than those with larger head circumference. Improvement in environmental factors in prenatal and early life that partially determine completed brain/head size may have consequences for the late-life expression of Alzheimer's disease in vulnerable individuals.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 1996 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) DSM–III–R. Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., Swayze, V. II., et al (1993) Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 130134.Google Scholar
Brandt, I. (1978) Growth dynamics of low-birth weight infants with emphasis on the perinatal period. In Human Growth. Volume 2 (eds Faulkner, F. & Tanner, J. M.), pp. 469518. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, N. (1978) The methods of auxological anthropometry. In: Human Growth, Volume 2: Postnatal Growth (eds Falkner, F. & Tanner, J. M.), pp. 3590. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobbing, J. & Sands, J. (1973) Quantitative growth and development of human brain. Archives of Diseases in Childhood, 49, 757767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elo, I. T. & Preston, S. H. (1992) Effects of early-life conditions on adult mortality: a review. Population Index, 58, 186212.Google Scholar
Emanuel, I. (1993) Intergenerational factors in pregnancy outcome: implications for teratology? In Issues and Reviews in Teratology, Volume 6 (ed. Kaltner, H.), pp. 4784. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, H. T. (1979) Correlated brain and intelligence development in humans. In Development and Evolution of Brain Size (eds Hahn, M. E., Jensen, C. & Dudek, B. C.), pp. 111131. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, D. A., Smith, L. A., Albert, M. S., et al (1993) Level of education and change in cognitive function in a community population of older persons. Annals of Epidemiology, 3, 7177.Google Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-Mental State”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.Google Scholar
Förstl, H., Burns, A., Jacoby, R., et al (1991) Quantitative CT scan analysis in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: I. Computerized planimetry of cerebrospinal fluid areas. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 6, 709713.Google Scholar
Graves, A. B., Larson, E. B., Edland, S., et al (1995) Prevalence of dementia and its subtypes in a total population of older Japanese Americans in King County, WA. Abstract Neurology, 45 (suppl 4), A216.Google Scholar
Hack, M., Breslau, N., Weissman, B., et al (1991) Effect of very low birth weight and subnormal head size on cognitive abilities at school age. New England Journal of Medicine, 325, 231237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasegawa, K. (1983) The clinical assessment of dementia in the aged: a dementia screening scale for psychogeriatric patients. In: Aging in the Eighties and Beyond (eds Bergener, M., Lehr, U., Lang, E., et al), pp. 207218. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Hosmer, D. W. & Lemeshow, S. (1989) Applied Logistic Regression, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., et al (1982) A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 566572.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. & Sinha, S. N. (1993) Physical correlates of human intelligence. In Biological Approaches to the Study of Human Intelligence (ed. Vernon, P. A.), pp. 139242. Norwood, New Jersey: Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Katzman, R., Terry, R., DeTeresa, R., et al (1988) Clinical, pathological and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Annals of Neurology, 23, 138144.Google Scholar
Larson, E. B., Kukull, W. A., Teri, L., et al (1990) University of Washington Alzheimer's Disease Patient Registry: 1987–1988. Aging, 2, 404408.Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (1989) A nutrition theory of the secular increases in intelligence, positive correlations between height, head size and IQ. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 372377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martorell, R. & Habicht, J. P. (1986) Growth in early childhood in developing countries. In Human Growth: A Comprehensive Treatise, 2nd edition. Volume 3: Methodology, Ecological, Genetic and Nutritional Effects on Growth (eds Falkner, F. & Tanner, J. M.), pp. 241262. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
McKhann, G., Drachmann, D., Folstein, M. F., et al (1984) Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS–ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.Google Scholar
Miller, A. K. H. & Corsellis, J. A. N. (1977) Evidence for a secular increase in human brain weight during the past century. Annals of Human Biology, 4, 253257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortimer, J. A. & Graves, A. B. (1993) Education and other socioeconomic determinants of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 43 (Suppl. 4), S39S44.Google Scholar
Norusis, M. J. (1986) SPSS/PC+ for the IBM PC/XT/AT. Chicago, Ill: SPSS Inc.Google Scholar
SAS/STAT User's Guide (1990) Version 6, Fourth Edition. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc.Google Scholar
Schofield, P. W., Mosesson, R. E., Stern, Y., et al (1995) The age at onset of Alzheimer's disease and an intracranial area measurement Archives of Neurology, 52, 9598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sells, C. J. (1977) Microcephaly in a normal school population. Pediatrics, 59, 262265.Google Scholar
Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T., et al (1994) Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 271, 10041010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teng, B. L. & Chui, H. C. (1987) The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 314318.Google Scholar
Teng, B. L., Hasegawa, K., Homma, A., et al (1994) The Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI): a practical test for cross-cultural epidemiologic studies of dementia. International Psychogeriatrics, 6, 4556.Google Scholar
Teri, L., McCurry, S. M., Edland, S. D., et al (1995) Cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal investigation of risk factors for accelerated decline. Journal of Gerontology, 50A, M49M55.Google ScholarPubMed
Tomlinson, B. E., Blessed, G. & Roth, M. (1968) Observations on the brains of nondemented old people. Journal of Neurological Science, 7, 331356.Google Scholar
Van Valen, L. (1974) Brain size and intelligence in man. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 40, 417424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zar, J. H. (1984) Biostatistical Analysis, 2nd edition. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Zhang, M. Y., Katzman, R., Salmon, D., et al (1990) The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in Shanghai, China: impact of age, gender and education. Annals of Neurology, 27, 428437.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.