Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:16:26.805Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring the quantitative nature of empathy, systemising and autistic traits using factor mixture modelling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rachel Grove*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Andrew Baillie
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Carrie Allison
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, Cambridge University, Cambridge, UK
Simon Baron-Cohen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, Cambridge University, Cambridge, UK and CLASS Clinic, Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Rosa A. Hoekstra
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, Cambridge University, Cambridge, UK and Faculty of Science, The Open University, Milton Keynes, UK
*
Rachel Grove, Department of Psychology, Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Autism research has previously focused on either identifying a latent dimension or searching for subgroups. Research assessing the concurrently categorical and dimensional nature of autism is needed.

Aims

To investigate the latent structure of autism and identify meaningful subgroups in a sample spanning the full spectrum of genetic vulnerability.

Method

Factor mixture models were applied to data on empathy, systemising and autistic traits from individuals on the autism spectrum, parents and general population controls.

Results

A two-factor three-class model was identified, with two factors measuring empathy and systemising. Class one had high systemising and low empathy scores and primarily consisted of individuals with autism. Mainly comprising controls and parents, class three displayed high empathy scores and lower systemising scores, and class two showed balanced scores on both measures of systemising and empathy.

Conclusions

Autism is best understood as a dimensional construct, but meaningful subgroups can be identified based on empathy, systemising and autistic traits.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2015 

A central debate in the development of DSM-5 1 has been whether psychopathology is best conceptualised as a continuum of severity or as discrete categories of disorder, including the DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Understanding the latent structure of autism, also referred to as autism spectrum conditions (ASC), is important for guiding future conceptualisations of diagnostic criteria, as well as for informing the development of instruments assessing characteristics of ASC. A number of studies have assessed the latent structure of autism using either dimensional or discrete statistical techniques. Reference Constantino, Gruber, Davis, Hayes, Passanante and Przybeck2Reference Frazier, Youngstrom, Sinclair, Kubu, Law and Rezai6 In contrast to these methods, factor mixture modelling allows for the presence of a concurrently dimensional and categorical latent structure. Reference Lubke and Muthén7 Three papers have previously applied mixture modelling to assess the latent structure within children with a clinical ASC diagnosis and non-affected siblings. Reference Frazier, Youngstrom, Speer, Embacher, Law and Constantino3,Reference Georgiades, Szatmari, Boyle, Hanna, Duku and Zwaigenbaum8,Reference Georgiades, Boyle, Szatmari, Hanna, Duku and Zwaigenbaum9 However, to date none of this work has focused on adult samples. Moreover, there is a large evidence base for the quantitative nature of autistic traits in the general population, Reference Constantino10 with undiagnosed first-degree relatives of individuals with autism displaying intermediate (or subthreshold) levels of autistic traits, also termed the broader autism phenotype (BAP). Reference Sucksmith, Roth and Hoekstra11,Reference Piven, Palmer, Jacobi, Childress and Arndt12 It is therefore important to assess the latent structure of autism across the full range of genetic vulnerability, from low-risk general population samples to first-degree relatives (medium risk) and individuals with a clinical ASC diagnosis. The Empathising–Systemising (E–S) theory of autism argues that the persistent deficits in communication and social interaction in autism can be accounted for by an impairment in empathy, particularly cognitive empathy (also referred to as ‘theory of mind’), whereas the restricted or repetitive behaviours and unusually narrow interests can be explained by a strong drive to systemise. Reference Baron-Cohen13 Previous research in the same sample as reported here suggested that empathy and systemising are discrete constructs that can reliably be measured in control samples, parents and individuals with ASC. Reference Grove, Baillie, Allison, Baron-Cohen and Hoekstra14 The current study elaborates on these findings using mixture modelling methods. The study aims to assess the dimensional latent structure of empathy, systemising and autistic traits among individuals on the spectrum, first-degree relatives and the general population, while simultaneously examining whether meaningful subgroups can be identified.

Method

Participants

Individuals with autism, parents of a child with autism and general population controls were recruited via two volunteer webpages at the University of Cambridge (www.autismresearchcentre.com; www.cambridgepsychology.com). The total sample consisted of 1034 individuals (controls 232, parents 439, ASC group 363). Controls were restricted to individuals with no psychiatric history and consisted of 110 females and 122 males (mean age 33 years, s.d. = 10). The parent group contained 298 females and 141 males (mean age 42 years, s.d. = 8). Parents were included in the study if they had a child with a formal ASC diagnosis, but did not report having a diagnosis of autism themselves. The ASC group comprised 170 females and 193 males (mean age 36 years, s.d. = 11). These individuals had received a formal clinical diagnosis of autism. The individuals in the parent and ASC groups were not part of the same family. IQ was assessed via an online adapted version of the Ravens Progressive Matrices. Reference Raven, Raven and Court15 The control group scored significantly higher on the Ravens than both the parent group (P<0.01) and individuals with ASC (P<0.01). There were no differences in IQ between parents and individuals with autism (P = 0.11). As this study relied on self-report, the ASC group was restricted to high-functioning individuals, who were able to effectively complete these online questionnaires.

Measures

Autistic traits

The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin and Clubley16 is designed to assess quantitative autistic traits including those related to communication, social skills, attention to detail, imagination and attention switching. A total of 50 items are assessed with four response options: ‘definitely agree’, ‘definitely disagree’, ‘slightly agree’ and ‘slightly disagree’. A raw scoring method was used, Reference Hoekstra, Bartels, Cath and Boomsma17 eliciting scores ranging from 50 to 200, higher scores indicating more autistic traits. Previous research suggested the AQ can be split into two reliable subscales relating to social and non-social traits. Reference Hoekstra, Bartels, Cath and Boomsma17 A broad social interaction factor (comprising 40 items assessing communication, social skills, imagination and attention switching) and an attention to detail factor (consisting of the remaining 10 items) were included in all analyses.

Systemising

The Systemising Quotient Revised (SQ) Reference Wheelwright, Baron-Cohen, Goldenfeld, Delaney, Fine and Smith18 is a measure designed to assess an individual's propensity to systemise; to construct and understand rule-based systems for categorisation. This measure includes 75 items scored on a Likert response scale with four response options: ‘strongly agree’, ‘strongly disagree’, ‘slightly agree’ and ‘slightly disagree’. Strong responses score one point, with slightly agree/disagree responses receiving one point. Scores range from 0 to 150, with higher scores indicative of a heightened drive to systemise.

Empathy

The Empathy Quotient (EQ) Reference Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright19 is a self-report measure of empathy. This 40-item measure includes equivalent response options and scoring methods to the SQ. Full endorsement of all items gives a score of 80, with higher scores indicative of a better capacity to empathise.

Apart from the AQ, SQ and EQ self-report questionnaires, which were included in the mixture analyses, data were also collected on two performance-based measures of empathy. Due to a large proportion of missing data in the sample of fathers these measures were not included in the factor analyses. The ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test’ revised (Eyes) Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb20 assesses how accurately an individual can read the emotion in another by viewing only the eye region of the face. A total of 36 items are presented with four descriptions of mental states. Relatively subtle and complex mental states are used, for example, joking, insisting, amused and relaxed, making the task an advanced test of empathy. The total number of correct items is recorded, with higher scores reflecting better ability.

The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF) Reference Lundqvist, Flykt and Öhman21 test is a test of more basic emotion recognition, including the basic emotions happy, sad, angry, afraid, disappointed, surprised and neutral, and giving participants the opportunity to view the whole face. This measure consists of 140 items in which accuracy and response time information is recorded. Response times were weighted for accuracy. Reference Sutherland and Crewther22 For ease of interpretation, the KDEF was rescored so that higher values indicate higher ability rather than a slower response time.

Analytic strategy

Confirmatory factor analysis

Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were conducted to assess the dimensional structure of empathy, systemising and autistic traits using the EQ, SQ and the two subscales of the AQ. A one-factor model was implemented to assess whether these traits lie on a continuum of severity (Model 1). Next, a two-factor model representing the distinction between empathy and systemising was fit to the data (Model 2). The EQ and the social interaction factor of the AQ were predicted to load onto a factor representing empathy, with the SQ and the attention to detail factor of the AQ loading onto a second-factor representing systemising.

Latent class analysis

Latent class analysis (LCA) is a technique designed to evaluate whether a number of observed variables (in this case empathy, systemising and autistic traits) can help to define an underlying categorical variable or class. Reference Hagenaars and McCutcheon23 A series of models ranging from one to five classes were implemented (Models 3–7).

Factor mixture models

Factor mixture models (FMM) combining both CFA and LCA models were also assessed. Measurement invariance is given when a measurement model relating observed variables to underlying latent variables does not vary across latent classes. Reference Meredith24 There is considerable debate over the invariance restrictions placed on classes in FMM and their varying effects on the results obtained. Reference Lubke and Muthén25 Lubke & Muthén Reference Lubke and Muthén7 show that correct class assignment is maximised by constraining factor loadings to be equal across classes while allowing thresholds to vary. This method was therefore applied to all models in the current study. Models were estimated up to and including the number of factors from the best fitting CFA model and the number of classes from the best fitting LCA model (Models 8–17).

Analyses were estimated using Mplus version 7 Reference Muthén, Hancock and Samuelsen26 with the robust maximum likelihood ratio estimator. A number of fit indices were estimated including the Akaike information criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criterion (BIC), sample size adjusted BIC (SSABIC), entropy and the Lo–Mendell–Rubin likelihood ratio test (LMR). Reference Nylund, Asparouhov and Muthen27 Lower AIC, BIC and SSABIC values are indicative of a better fit to the data. These fit statistics were used to evaluate the fit of the CFA models. In LCA and FMM, the BIC has been shown to be more reliable in obtaining the best fitting models than AIC and SSABIC. Reference Nylund, Asparouhov and Muthen27,Reference Gebregziabher, Shotwell, Charles and Nicholas28 Therefore, the BIC was used to reach a final decision on the best fitting model in LCA and FFM analyses. In addition to these measures, the LMR test and entropy statistic are useful to determine the optimal number of latent classes. An LMR value that is not statistically significant indicates that a model with one less class provides a better fit to the data. Reference Lo, Mendell and Rubin29 Similarly, the entropy statistic is a measure of the accuracy with which each individual can be categorised into a latent class, with higher entropy values indicating better categorisation. Reference Ramaswany, Desarbo, Reibstein and Robinson30 In addition to AIC, BIC and SSABIC, the LMR and entropy statistic were also consulted when evaluating the LCA and FMM models.

As well as taking into account the fit indices mentioned above, in evaluating the model fit the conceptual appropriateness of the latent class profiles was also considered. Once the best fitting model was identified, further analyses assessing mean differences and demographic information for the classes identified was conducted using SPSS Version 21.0. 31

Results

As reported previously, Reference Wheelwright, Baron-Cohen, Goldenfeld, Delaney, Fine and Smith18,Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb20,Reference Sucksmith, Allison, Baron-Cohen, Chakrabarti and Hoekstra32 there were a number of differences in the mean scores on the questionnaires for controls, parents and individuals with ASC (Table 1). Individuals with ASC scored significantly lower on the social interaction items of the AQ and higher on the attention to detail items compared with controls and parents (P<0.01). Individuals with ASC also obtained significantly higher SQ scores and lower EQ scores compared with all other participants. Parents scored significantly lower on the social interaction factor of the AQ than controls (P<0.05). Control females displayed higher scores on the EQ and on the AQ social interaction factor as well as lower scores on the SQ than control males (P<0.01). This pattern was also observed among parents (P<0.01). Males with ASC displayed higher scores on the social interaction subscale of the AQ than females with ASC (P<0.01), whereas females displayed higher scores on the attention to detail items of the AQ than males with autism (P<0.05). However, there were no gender differences within the ASC group on the other two measures.

TABLE 1 Mean scores across groups on questionnaire data evaluating empathy, systemising and autistic traits

AQ_soc
Mean (s.d.)
AQ_att
Mean (s.d.)
SQ
Mean (s.d.)
EQ
Mean (s.d.)
Controls 117.2 (15.9) 25.3 (5.1) 63.7 (23.9) 43.1 (14.2)
    Male 113.4 (15.3) 25.1 (5.0) 67.0 (23.0) 38.5 (12.9)
    Female 121.5 (15.5) 25.4 (5.3) 60.0 (24.5) 48.1 (13.9)
Parents 113.6 (22.4) 24.3 (5.6) 57.8 (25.3) 42.4 (18.2)
    Male 107.0 (23.2) 24.7 (5.4) 71.0 (26.9) 33.2 (17.1)
    Female 116.7 (21.3) 24.1 (5.7) 51.6 (22.0) 46.8 (17.1)
ASC 75.2 (16.5) 29.9 (5.1) 77.4 (25.2) 18.4 (10.0)
    Male 77.7 (17.1) 29.1 (4.9) 76.4 (25.3) 18.1 (10.6)
    Female 72.3 (15.2) 30.8 (5.2) 78.5 (25.2) 18.7 (9.4)

AQ_att, Attention to detail factor of the Autism Spectrum Quotient; AQ_soc, Social interaction factor of the Autism Spectrum Quotient; ASC, autism spectrum condition; EQ, Empathy Quotient; SQ, Systemising Quotient Revised.

Model fit indices for the CFA, LCA and FMM analyses are given in Table 2. Results from the CFA indicated that a two-factor model (Model 2) consisting of empathy and systemising provided the best fit to the data, with lower AIC, BIC and SSABIC values than the one-factor model. There was a moderate negative correlation between the two factors, suggesting that better systemising is associated with lower empathy abilities. LCA models with up to five latent classes were then estimated. The LMR value for the five-class model did not reach significance, indicating that a model with one less class provided a better classification of individuals. The four-class LCA also had the smallest AIC, BIC and SSABIC values of the remaining four models (Model 6) as well as an entropy statistic of 0.81. This model was therefore selected as providing the best fit to the data.

TABLE 2 CFA, LCA, and FMM results for empathy, systemising and autistic traits

Fit statistics
Model Analysis/model
description
AIC BIC SSABIC Entropy LMR
CFA
1 1f 33 001.576 33 060.870 33 022.757
2 2f
Factor correlation (−0.65)
32 728.316 32 787.610 32 749.497
LCA
3 1c 35 057.523 35 097.053 35 071.644
4 2c 33 498.692 33 562.927 33 521.638 0.872 0.0000
5 3c 33 012.493 33 101.434 33 044.264 0.852 0.0002
6 4c 32 814.899 32 928.546 32 855.495 0.812 0.0172
7 5c 32 717.575 32 855.928 32 766.997 0.800 0.2492
FMM
8 1f1c 33 001.576 33 060.870 33 022.757
9 1f2c 32 753.226 32 842.168 32 784.997 0.626 0.0000
10 1f3c 32 592.453 32 711.041 32 634.814 0.811 0.0563
11 1f4c 32 505.882 32 654.118 32 558.834 0.801 0.0822
12 1f5c 32 455.023 32 632.905 32 518.565 0.784 0.0003
13 2f1c 32 728.316 32 787.610 32 749.497
14 2f2c 32 578.082 32 676.906 32 613.383 0.769 0.0035
15 2f3c 32 491.851 32 625.264 32 539.508 0.760 0.0232
16 2f4c 32 462.540 32 625.599 32 520.787 0.707 0.0249
17 2f5c 32 443.404 32 641.052 32 514.007 0.726 0.0982

AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; c, class; CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; f, factor; FMM, factor mixture modelling; LCA, latent class analysis; LMR, Lo–Mendell–Rubin likelihood ratio test; SSABIC, sample size adjusted BIC.

Mixture models consisting of one and two factors and up to five latent classes were then implemented. The smallest BIC values were identified in the two-factor three-class and two-factor four-class models. The three-class model had the smallest BIC value as well as the largest entropy value and provided the most parsimonious explanation for the data. This model, designating three classes with varying levels of empathy, systemising and autistic traits, was therefore selected as the best fitting model (Model 15). Model fit statistics were compared across Models 2 (CFA), 6 (LCA) and 15 (FMM) to establish whether empathy, systemising and autistic traits are best conceptualised as dimensional, categorical, or a combination of both dimensional and categorical constructs. The mixture model (Model 15) provided the best fit across all three analyses and was therefore selected as providing the soundest explanation for the data.

Model 15 contained two factors representing empathy and systemising. This model identified three latent classes of individuals. Class one comprised 45% of the sample (n = 461, mean age 38 years, s.d. = 11), class two approximately 30% (n = 310, mean age 37 years, s.d. = 11) and class three the remaining 25% (n = 263, mean age 40 years, s.d. = 9). Class three was significantly older than both class two (P<0.01) and class one (P<0.05). Class three also had a lower IQ score than class one (P<0.05). The systemising and empathy factors were moderately correlated (r = −0.49). There were no significant differences in the factor correlations across the three classes.

Mean differences on measures of empathy, systemising and autistic traits for each class are given in Fig. 1. Scores on all measures were converted to the same scale via Z score transformation. These Z scores were then increased by two to remove any negative values before being plotted to ease interpretation. The first class identified (Class S) scored significantly higher on self-reported systemising (including the SQ and the attention to detail factor of the AQ) and lower on self-reported empathy (including the EQ and the social interaction factor of the AQ) than the other latent classes (P<0.01). Class three (Class E) scored significantly higher on measures of empathy and lower on systemising than classes one and two (P<0.01). Class two displayed scores on measures of systemising, empathy and autistic traits intermediate to that of the other two classes, showing a balance between empathy and systemising (Class B).

Fig. 1 Mean scores on self-report and performance tasks across classes.

AQatt, Attention to detail factor of the Autism Spectrum Quotient; AQsoc, Social interaction factor of the Autism Spectrum Quotient; EQ, Empathy quotient; Eyes, Reading the Mind in the Eyes task; KDEF, Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces task; SQ, Systemising Quotient Revised.

Subsequently, mean differences on performance-based measures of empathy were assessed. Class S scored significantly lower on the Eyes task than both Class B and Class E (P<0.01). Class B also performed more poorly than Class E on this task (mean difference −0.26, P<0.01). Class S scored significantly lower than both other classes on the KDEF task (P<0.01), with no differences in mean scores between Class B and Class E.

The proportion of males, females, controls, parents and individuals with ASC falling into each class is given in Fig. 2. Class E primarily comprised females, whereas the gender division was similar in the other two classes. Individuals with ASC made up the majority of Class S (71%), along with 23% of parents and a very small proportion of controls (6%). Class B consisted of approximately 39% controls, 50% parents and 11% of individuals with ASC. Class E consisted predominantly of parents (67%) and controls (32%) with a very small proportion of those with ASC (1%). Within the parent group, 38% of fathers compared with 18% of mothers fell into Class S. Half of the sample of mothers fell into Class E compared with 20% of fathers.

Fig. 2 Gender and group membership in each class.

ASC, autism spectrum condition.

Discussion

Structural equation modelling including CFA, LCA and FMM analyses in a large sample of individuals with ASC, parents and controls indicated that the characteristics of autism, as measured in a sample spanning the full spectrum of genetic liability, are best described by a two-factor three-class mixture model. The quantitative nature of autistic traits is best captured by two moderately correlated latent factors representing systemising and empathy. In addition, three homogeneous latent classes of individuals could be identified by their mean scores on measures of empathy, systemising and autistic traits. Class one displayed superior performance on systemising, with significantly lower scores on both self-reported and performance-based tests of empathy (Class S). Class three demonstrated the opposite effect, showing increased scores on empathy tasks and lower performance on self-report measures of systemising (Class E). Class two appeared to be more balanced in terms of both empathy and systemising propensity (Class B).

The results provide support for the E–S theory, indicating that empathy and systemising are two separate constructs that together may partly provide a cognitive explanation of the characteristics of autism. The findings also lend indirect support to the current DSM-5 diagnostic criteria, including social and communication impairment (represented in the current study by difficulty with empathy) and repetitive behaviours and narrow interests (represented here by high systemising scores). EQ items map onto the social and communication domain assessing difficulties in social-emotional reciprocity, non-verbal communication and relationships. For example, ‘I can easily tell if someone else wants to enter a conversation’, ‘I am quick to spot when someone in a group is feeling awkward or uncomfortable’ and ‘friendships and relationships are just too difficult, so I tend not to bother with them’. The restricted or repetitive behaviour or interests domain in DSM-5 contains four criteria relating to stereotyped and repetitive movement or use of objects, insistence on sameness, fixated interests and hyper- or hyporeactivity to sensory stimuli. The first three of these DSM-5 criteria may be (partly) accounted for by a drive to systemise. By engaging in stereotyped and repetitive actions, insisting on sameness and focusing on circumscribed interests, the world becomes more predictable and therefore easier to negotiate. It is as yet less clear how sensory reactivity relates to empathy and systemising. A recent study reported an association between sensory sensitivity and autistic traits, with greater sensitivity associated with more traits on the autism spectrum. Reference Tavassoli, Hoekstra and Baron-Cohen33 However, further research is needed to comprehensively understand the association between sensory reactivity, empathy and systemising.

The two-factor structure found in the current study is consistent with previous factor analytic studies directly assessing autistic characteristics via diagnostic instruments, suggesting that the autism phenotype follows a dyadic structure comprising social communicative difficulties and non-social autistic traits. Reference Frazier, Youngstrom, Speer, Embacher, Law and Constantino3,Reference Mandy, Charman and Skuse5,Reference Grove, Baillie, Allison, Baron-Cohen and Hoekstra14,Reference Mandy, Charman, Puura and Skuse34 However, the two factors identified in the present study were moderately correlated (r = −0.49), indicating that empathy and systemising are not entirely independent.

The E–S theory posits that there are five different cognitive profiles that can be identified based on empathy and systemising. Type E (E>S) are individuals with stronger empathy than systemising ability; Type S (S>E) comprises individuals with systemising ability that is stronger than their empathy skills; Type B (E = S) includes individuals with similar empathy and systemising ability; Extreme Type E (E>>S) comprises individuals with above average empathy who have difficulty with systemising; and last, Extreme Type S (S>>E) includes individuals with above-average systemising who have difficulty with empathy. Reference Baron-Cohen13 Individuals with autism are thought to be represented by the Extreme Type S cognitive profile, with varying combinations of the other cognitive profiles in the general population. Reference Baron-Cohen13 Using factor mixture modelling techniques, the current study identified classes that map very well onto the Type S, B and E profiles outlined in this theory.

Class S was characterised by low empathy and high systemising scores. It consisted predominantly of individuals with autism (both males and females), with only a very small proportion of the control sample falling into this class. The observation that the majority of individuals with ASC in this study (90%) were identified within Class S follows the predictions of the E–S theory, with autism being characterised by an interest in systemising and difficulties with empathy. However, the finding that a small proportion of individuals with ASC also fell into the other two classes identified highlights the heterogeneity of autism: not every individual with ASC displays the Type S cognitive profile and shows superior systemising and impaired empathy. Approximately 20% of Class S comprised parents of a child with ASC, suggesting that these individuals display the BAP. Proportionally, more fathers than mothers fell into Class S (38% v. 18% respectively). This is consistent with previous research suggesting that the BAP is more common in male relatives. Reference Scheeren and Stauder35 The presence of parents in Class S has clinical implications, as these parents may be especially well served by clinical advice and guidance provided in a systematic, factual manner. Parents who show difficulties with empathy themselves may also benefit from advice on how to manage and improve their own relationships with others.

A large proportion of parents, primarily mothers, were also represented in Class E, characterised by low systemising and high empathy scores. This finding highlights that, even though the BAP is common in parents of a child with ASC, certainly not all parents show these characteristics. Approximately half of the mothers and 20% of fathers did not display any characteristics of the BAP. The stronger representation of mothers than fathers in Class E further supports the notion of the BAP being more common in male than female relatives.

A small proportion of individuals with ASC (approximately 10%) fell into Class B, represented by equivalent scores on empathy and systemising. It is surprising that a proportion of individuals with ASC would fall into this class. However, further analyses within this class suggested a number of important differences. These individuals with an ASC diagnosis displayed similar scores on the EQ and SQ as the rest of Class B, and therefore increased self-reported empathy, and decreased systemising abilities compared with the rest of the ASC sample. However, this subsample of individuals scored significantly lower on the social interaction items of the AQ (mean difference 12.1, P<0.01) and higher on the attention to detail items (mean difference 2.7, P<0.01) than the rest of Class B. They also scored lower on both performance-based measures of empathy (Eyes mean difference 2.3, P<0.01; KDEF mean difference 82.5, P<0.01), displaying equivalent scores to individuals in Class S and the ASC sample. This indicates that although the self-report EQ and SQ scores suggest these individuals have equivalent empathy and systemising abilities, their scores on performance-based measures highlight a difficulty with empathy. Interestingly, the 10% of individuals with ASC who fell in Class B were also significantly younger and had a lower IQ score than the other individuals in this class. It could be that limited insight or understanding of their difficulties had an impact on scores on self-report measures, whereas their difficulties with empathy were picked up by the relatively poor performance on the KDEF and Eyes tasks. This highlights the importance of the use of performance-based measures in research and clinical practice as well as the potential impact of cognitive ability and age on self-report measures.

The current study identified homogeneous subgroups based on levels of empathy, systemising and autistic traits in a sample spanning the entire spectrum of vulnerability to autism, from general population controls to individuals with a clinical diagnosis. Most previous studies aiming to define phenotypic subgroups in autism included clinical samples only. These studies generally highlighted that distinct groups can be identified based on either the presence of an ASC diagnosis, Reference Frazier, Youngstrom, Speer, Embacher, Law and Constantino3 or severity of symptoms. Reference Georgiades, Szatmari, Boyle, Hanna, Duku and Zwaigenbaum8,Reference Georgiades, Boyle, Szatmari, Hanna, Duku and Zwaigenbaum9 A recent study in a large sample of children with a DSM-IV diagnosis of pervasive developmental disorder identified six classes of individuals, including three groups displaying impairment on both social and non-social domains, as well as three classes with impairment on only one of the two symptom dimensions. Reference Greaves-Lord, Eussen, Verhulst, Minderaa, Mandy and Hudziak36 One class comprised individuals displaying social and communication difficulties, but no restricted repetitive behaviours, suggesting that, if these individuals had been diagnosed using current DSM-5 criteria rather than the DSM-IV, the new DSM-5 diagnosis of Social Communication Disorder (SCD) 1 may have been appropriate. Our study, using a sample spanning from controls to individuals with autism, rather than a clinical sample only, did not identify a separate class characterised by low empathy and average systemising, a pattern that would perhaps be expected for individuals with SCD. Nevertheless it would be of interest to study empathy and systemising in individuals with SCD and explore any potential differences in these traits compared to individuals with autism.

The findings of this study should be interpreted in the light of some limitations. Although the gender ratio was balanced for the control and ASC groups, there was a larger proportion of mothers (n = 298) than fathers (n = 141) in the parent group. Future research would benefit from the inclusion of more fathers for comparison. Second, this study included similar numbers of males and females, which is different from the documented gender ratio for ASC diagnoses of approximately 4:1. Reference Chakrabarti and Fombonne37 This relative over-representation of female participants with ASC is potentially due to the volunteer bias of online recruitment, with females being more likely to volunteer for research participation than men. Reference Todd, Davis and Cafferty38,Reference Rosenthal and Rosnow39 However, given that males and females with ASC were equally distributed over the different classes (Class S: males = 170, females = 155; Class B: males = 21, females = 14; Class E: males = 2, females = 1), it is unlikely that this had a significant impact on the analyses and results of the study.

Third, this study only included performance-based measures of empathy abilities; no performance-based measure of systemising was available to compare against the self-report questionnaire data. It should be stressed that as this study included self-rated measures, all individuals with ASC were high functioning. The results from this study can therefore not be generalised to individuals with ASC and intellectual disability. Given that the data were collected online, it was also not possible to verify diagnoses of autism. However, there is evidence to suggest that clinical diagnoses of ASC reported by online volunteers are generally reliable. Reference Lee, Marvin, Watson, Piggot, Law and Law40

In conclusion, this study assessed the quantitative nature of empathy, systemising and autistic traits among individuals on the spectrum, first-degree relatives and general population controls. Results highlighted a two-factor three-class model in which two dimensions based on systemising and empathy were identified. This provides indirect support for the new diagnostic criteria outlined in DSM-5, which follow a dyadic rather than a triadic structure and include a dimensional rather than a categorical approach. Three meaningful classes were defined based on mean scores on empathy, systemising and autistic traits. Taken together, these results support the quantitative approach to autistic traits and confirm that even with the use of quantitative measures, meaningful subgroups can be identified.

Footnotes

Joint senior authors.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5). APA, 2013.Google Scholar
2 Constantino, JN, Gruber, CP, Davis, S, Hayes, S, Passanante, N, Przybeck, T. The factor structure of autistic traits. J Child Psychol Psyc 2004; 45: 719–26.Google Scholar
3 Frazier, TW, Youngstrom, EA, Speer, L, Embacher, R, Law, P, Constantino, J. Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012; 51: 2840.Google Scholar
4 Ingram, GD, Takahashi, TN, Miles, JH. Defining autism subgroups: a taxometric solution. J Autism Dev Disord 2008; 38: 950–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Mandy, WPL, Charman, T, Skuse, DH. Testing the construct validity of proposed criteria for DSM-5 Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012; 51: 4150.Google Scholar
6 Frazier, TW, Youngstrom, EA, Sinclair, L, Kubu, CS, Law, P, Rezai, A. Autism spectrum disorders as a qualitatively distinct category from typical behavior in a large, clinically ascertained sample. Assessment 2010; 17: 308–20.Google Scholar
7 Lubke, GH, Muthén, B. Investigating population heterogeneity with factor mixture models. Psychol Med 2005; 10: 2139.Google Scholar
8 Georgiades, S, Szatmari, P, Boyle, M, Hanna, S, Duku, E, Zwaigenbaum, L, et al. Investigating phenotypic heterogeneity in children with autism spectrum disorder: a factor mixture modeling approach. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013; 54: 206–15.Google Scholar
9 Georgiades, S, Boyle, M, Szatmari, P, Hanna, S, Duku, E, Zwaigenbaum, L, et al. Modeling the phenotypic architecture of autism symptoms from time of diagnosis to age 6. J Autism Dev Disord 2014; 44: 3045–55.Google Scholar
10 Constantino, JN. The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatr Res 2011; 69: 55R62R.Google Scholar
11 Sucksmith, E, Roth, I, Hoekstra, RA. Autistic traits below the clinical threshold: re-examining the Broader Autism Phenotype in the 21st century. Neuropsychol Rev 2011; 21: 360–89.Google Scholar
12 Piven, J, Palmer, P, Jacobi, D, Childress, D, Arndt, S. Broader autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154: 185–90.Google Scholar
13 Baron-Cohen, S. Autism: the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Ann NY Acad Sci 2009; 1156: 6880.Google Scholar
14 Grove, R, Baillie, A, Allison, C, Baron-Cohen, S, Hoekstra, RA. Empathizing, systemizing, and autistic traits: latent structure in individuals with autism, their parents and general population controls. J Abnorm Psychol 2013; 122: 600–9.Google Scholar
15 Raven, J, Raven, JC, Court, JH. Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section 1: General Overview. Harcourt Assessment, 2003.Google Scholar
16 Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Skinner, R, Martin, J, Clubley, E. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord 2001; 31: 5.Google Scholar
17 Hoekstra, RA, Bartels, M, Cath, DC, Boomsma, DI. Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): a study in Dutch population and patient groups. J Autism Dev Disord 2008; 38: 1555–66.Google Scholar
18 Wheelwright, S, Baron-Cohen, S, Goldenfeld, N, Delaney, J, Fine, D, Smith, R, et al. Predicting Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) from the Systemizing Quotient-Revised (SQ-R) and Empathy Quotient (EQ). Brain Res 2006; 1079: 4756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord 2004; 34: 163–75.Google Scholar
20 Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Hill, J, Raste, Y, Plumb, I. The “Reading the mind in the eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42: 241–51.Google Scholar
21 Lundqvist, D, Flykt, A, Öhman, A. The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces. Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section: Karolinska Institutet, 1998.Google Scholar
22 Sutherland, A, Crewther, DP. Magnocellular visual evoked potential delay with high autism spectrum quotient yields a neural mechanism for altered perception. Brain 2010; 133: 2089–97.Google Scholar
23 Hagenaars, J, McCutcheon, AL. Applied Latent Class Analysis. Cambridge University Press, 2002.Google Scholar
24 Meredith, W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika 1993; 58: 525–43.Google Scholar
25 Lubke, GH, Muthén, B. Performance of factor mixture models as a function of model size, covariate effects, and class-specific parameters. Struct Equ Modeling 2007; 14: 2647.Google Scholar
26 Muthén, B. Latent variable hybrids: overview of old and new models. In Advances in Latent Variable Mixture Models (eds Hancock, GR, Samuelsen, KM): 124. Information Age Publishing, 2008.Google Scholar
27 Nylund, KL, Asparouhov, T, Muthen, BO. Deciding on the number of classes in Latent Class Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Modeling 2007; 14: 535–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28 Gebregziabher, M, Shotwell, MS, Charles, JM, Nicholas, JS. Comparison of methods for identifying phenotype subgroups using categorical features data with application to autism spectrum disorder. Comput Stat Data Anal 2012; 56: 114–25.Google Scholar
29 Lo, Y, Mendell, NR, Rubin, DB. Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika 2001; 88: 767–78.Google Scholar
30 Ramaswany, V, Desarbo, WS, Reibstein, DJ, Robinson, WT. An empirical pooling approach for estimating marketing mix elasticities with PIMS data. Market Sci 1993; 12: 103–24.Google Scholar
31 IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. IBM Corp, 2012.Google Scholar
32 Sucksmith, E, Allison, C, Baron-Cohen, S, Chakrabarti, B, Hoekstra, RA. Empathy and emotion recognition in people with autism, first-degree relatives and controls. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51: 98105.Google Scholar
33 Tavassoli, T, Hoekstra, RA, Baron-Cohen, S. The Sensory Perception Quotient (SPQ): development and validation of a new sensory questionnaire for adults with and without autism. Mol Autism 2014; 5: 29.Google Scholar
34 Mandy, W, Charman, T, Puura, K, Skuse, D. Investigating the cross-cultural validity of DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder: evidence from Finnish and UK samples. Autism 2014; 18: 4554.Google Scholar
35 Scheeren, AM, Stauder, JE. Broader autism phenotype in parents of autistic children: Reality or myth? J Autism Dev Disord 2008; 38: 276–87.Google Scholar
36 Greaves-Lord, K, Eussen, ML, Verhulst, FC, Minderaa, RB, Mandy, W, Hudziak, JJ, et al. Empirically based phenotypic profiles of children with pervasive developmental disorders: interpretation in the light of the DSM-5. J Autism Dev Disord 2013: 43: 1784–97.Google Scholar
37 Chakrabarti, S, Fombonne, E. Pervasive developmental disorders in pre-school children. JAMA 2001; 285: 3093–9.Google Scholar
38 Todd, M, Davis, KE, Cafferty, TP. Who volunteers for adult development research? Research findings and practical steps to reach low volunteering groups. Int J Aging Hum Dev 1983; 18: 177–84.Google ScholarPubMed
39 Rosenthal, R, Rosnow, RL. The Volunteer Subject. John Wiley and Sons, 1975.Google Scholar
40 Lee, H, Marvin, AR, Watson, T, Piggot, J, Law, JK, Law, PA. Accuracy of phenotyping of autistic children based on internet implemented parent report. Am J Med Genet 2010; 153: 1119–26.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Mean scores across groups on questionnaire data evaluating empathy, systemising and autistic traits

Figure 1

TABLE 2 CFA, LCA, and FMM results for empathy, systemising and autistic traits

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Mean scores on self-report and performance tasks across classes.AQatt, Attention to detail factor of the Autism Spectrum Quotient; AQsoc, Social interaction factor of the Autism Spectrum Quotient; EQ, Empathy quotient; Eyes, Reading the Mind in the Eyes task; KDEF, Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces task; SQ, Systemising Quotient Revised.

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Gender and group membership in each class.ASC, autism spectrum condition.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.