Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:01:14.943Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effectiveness of a volunteer befriending programme for patients with schizophrenia: randomised controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2019

Stefan Priebe*
Affiliation:
Professor, Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry (WHO Collaborating Centre for Mental Health Services Development), Queen Mary University of London, UK
Agnes Chevalier
Affiliation:
Trial Manager, Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry (WHO Collaborating Centre for Mental Health Services Development), Queen Mary University of London, UK
Thomas Hamborg
Affiliation:
Statistician, Pragmatic Clinical Trials Unit, Centre for Primary Care and Public Health, Queen Mary University of London, UK
Eoin Golden
Affiliation:
Volunteer Coordinator, Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry (WHO Collaborating Centre for Mental Health Services Development), Queen Mary University of London, UK
Michael King
Affiliation:
Professor, Division of Psychiatry, Faculty of Brain Sciences, University College London, UK
Nancy Pistrang
Affiliation:
Emeritus Professor, Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, UK
*
Correspondence: Stefan Priebe, Academic Unit, Newham Centre for Mental Health, LondonE13 8SP, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Befriending by volunteers has the potential to reduce the frequent social isolation of patients with schizophrenia and thus improve health outcomes. However, trial-based evidence for its effectiveness is limited.

Aims

To conduct a randomised controlled trial of befriending for patients with schizophrenia or related disorders.

Method

Patients were randomised to a befriending programme for 1 year or to receive information about social activities only (trial registration: ISRCTN14021839). Outcomes were assessed masked to allocation at the end of the programme; at 12 months and at a 6-month follow-up. The primary outcome was daily time spent in activities (using the Time Use Survey (TUS)) with intention-to-treat analysis.

Results

A total of 124 patients were randomised (63 intervention, 61 active control) and 92 (74%) were followed up at 1 year. In the intervention group, 49 (78%) met a volunteer at least once and 31 (49%) had more than 12 meetings. At 1 year, mean TUS scores were more than three times higher in both groups with no significant difference between them (adjusted difference 8.9, 95% CI −40.7 to 58.5, P = 0.72). There were no significant differences in quality of life, symptoms or self-esteem. However, patients in the intervention group had significantly more social contacts than those in the control group at the end of the 12-month period. This difference held true at the follow-up 6 months later.

Conclusions

Although no difference was found on the primary outcome, the findings suggest that befriending may have a lasting effect on increasing social contacts. It may be used more widely to reduce the social isolation of patients with schizophrenia.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2019

People with schizophrenia tend to be more socially isolated than other groups in the population.Reference Giacco, Palumbo, Strappelli, Catapano and Priebe1, Reference Harley, Boardman and Craig2 Social isolation in turn is linked to higher levels of symptoms, poor quality of life and worse treatment outcomes.Reference Giacco, McCabe, Kallert, Hansson, Fiorillo and Priebe3Reference Eklund and Hansson5 Specific symptoms may contribute to this social isolation: negative symptoms can affect motivation and ability to socialiseReference Giacco, McCabe, Kallert, Hansson, Fiorillo and Priebe3, Reference Worswick, Dimic, Wildgrube and Priebe6 and positive symptoms can lead to an active avoidance of social situations.Reference Hansen, Torgalsboen, Melle and Bell7 Social isolation may also be driven by social exclusion, prejudice and a tendency for others to distance themselves.Reference Angermeyer and Dietrich8 In contrast to this, there are volunteers who give up their spare time to support people with schizophrenia,Reference Toner, Fabisch, Priebe and Klug9 often in the form of ‘befriending’ delivered through a dedicated programme.Reference Hallett, Klug, Lauber and Priebe10Reference Priebe, Omer, Giacco and Slade12 Befriending programmes generally involve a relationship between two individuals with regular input over a pre-specified period of time that is initiated and supervised by a third party.Reference Thompson, Valenti, Siette and Priebe13 Befriending has been suggested to have benefits for the patients and volunteers, as well as for society at large through promoting social cohesion and social capital. Despite being widespread, the effects of befriending for patients have scarcely been researched.Reference Hallett, Klug, Lauber and Priebe10, Reference Toner, Hickling, Pinto da Costa, Cassidy and Priebe11

Meta-analyses assessing similar interventions for physical or mental health conditions suggested positive effects on depressive symptoms,Reference Mead, Lester, Chew-Graham, Gask and Bower14 and on overall patient-reported outcomes.Reference Siette, Cassidy and Priebe15 However, effect sizes were small and trial-based evidence was inconclusive, particularly with respect to behavioural outcomes. Only two trials were conducted with patients with severe mental illness and these were with diagnostically mixed samples. One used a matched-control design and found a positive effect of befriending on perceived social support and non-significant trends on other outcomes.Reference McCorkle, Rogers, Dunn, Lyass and Wan16 The second reported increased engagement in social activities following the intervention, but also found this improvement in the control group, with no significant difference between them.Reference Sheridan, Drennan, Coughlan, O'Keeffe, Frazer and Kemple17 Against this background, we established a befriending programme with clearly defined quality criteria, including systematic training of all volunteers and regular reminders to all participants, and conducted a randomised controlled trial (RCT, trial registration: ISRCTN14021839) of its effectiveness for socially isolated people with schizophrenia. We tested whether befriending would reduce social isolation and lead to improvements in other health and social outcomes.

Method

Study design and participants

We conducted a parallel groups RCT in community-based mental health services in London, UK, between August 2015 and August 2017 (full details in the published protocol).Reference Priebe, Pavlickova, Eldridge, Golden, McCrone and Ockenden18 Patient-participants were recruited from 15 community services across the London boroughs of City and Hackney, Tower Hamlets and Newham. Eligible patients were aged 18–65 years; had a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia or related disorders (ICD–10: F20–29);19 had been in the care of the service for at least 1 month; were not current in-patients; expressed a willingness to participate in regular befriending for a year; had sufficient command of English to converse with a volunteer; and were physically able to engage in a range of community activities. Eligible patients also had a defined level of social isolation, measured on the Time Use Survey (TUS),Reference Hodgekins, French and Birchwood20 of spending less than 60 min per day in social or recreational activities. Exclusion criteria were having already received befriending in the past 2 years; current participation in another research study; and posing a potential risk to the volunteer because of a significant history of violence.

Patients meeting the inclusion criteria that could be established from medical records were identified from service case-loads. Clinicians obtained permission from identified patients to be approached by researchers, who met the patients and established further eligibility criteria. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after a full explanation of study procedures.

Volunteer-participants were recruited from various sources including flyers in local community centres and universities. Eligible volunteers were 18 years or older and had sufficient command of English. Exclusion criteria were the receipt of treatment from secondary mental health services in the past year, in order to distinguish befriending from peer support; a current professional role in mental health services; and any unspent criminal convictions. Eligibility was established through an application form, interviews and a criminal records check. The initial training was for 2 full days, covering general information about the programme, symptoms of schizophrenia, responsibilities and boundaries in befriending, and resources for supervision and support.

Simple 1:1 randomisation with randomly varied block lengths of 4 and 6 was used to allocate patients to the intervention or control arms. This was done by the registered Pragmatic Clinical Trials Unit at Queen Mary University of London via a dedicated website, only accessible by an unmasked researcher who in turn informed participants.

Procedures

The intervention was developed through mapping the policies and the practices of existing programmes, and through discussions with experts including volunteer managers, volunteers and patients.Reference Priebe, Pavlickova, Eldridge, Golden, McCrone and Ockenden18

Patient-participants allocated to the intervention were contacted after randomisation to arrange an initial meeting with the volunteer coordinator to establish their interests and preferences for a volunteer. After this, the volunteer coordinator arranged and facilitated an initial ‘matching’ meeting with a volunteer selected on the basis of their preferences and availability. During this meeting both parties were provided with an activity booklet, containing options for free or inexpensive activities in the local area. Following this initial meeting the volunteer and patient were asked to meet weekly for a year and encouraged to engage in joint activities. Patients and volunteers were invited to inform the volunteer coordinator within the first month if they wanted to be ‘matched’ with someone different, which would be arranged as soon as possible depending on the pool of suitable volunteers at that time. Monthly social events including food and/or an activity (for example a picnic in a park, an art workshop) were organised by the programme to provide opportunities for different volunteers and patients to meet and interact.

Volunteers reported the occurrence, length and content of meetings to the coordinator, either via text message or over the telephone. Both parties were regularly reminded of weekly meetings and could request supervision to problem-solve any emerging challenges in the relationship. Volunteer retention was encouraged and facilitated as far as possible; however, in cases where they had to drop-out, the patient was given the option of starting a relationship with a new volunteer.

Patients allocated to the active control condition were met by an unmasked researcher who provided them with an activity booklet and spoke to them about activities they might like to engage in.

Outcomes were assessed at baseline, at the end of the programme at 12 months and after a further 6 months’ follow-up. We aimed for all researchers conducting outcome assessments to remain masked to allocation and an unmasked researcher regularly reminded patient-participants to conceal their allocation status. For patients in the intervention, follow-up assessments were arranged at least 1 week following the last meeting with a volunteer. This was organised by the volunteer coordinator to ensure that measures of social outcomes did not include meetings with the volunteer.

Outcomes

The primary outcome was average time spent engaging in activities in min per day at 12 months, measured with an adapted version of the TUS applied to the past 4 days. The TUS was developed for the general population and had previously been used with patients with schizophrenia.Reference Hodgekins, French and Birchwood20

Secondary outcomes were: (a) social contacts defined as the number of different people met across the past 4 days and assessed using the Social Contacts Assessment;Reference Giacco, Palumbo, Strappelli, Catapano and Priebe1 (b) observer-rated symptoms of schizophrenia assessed with both the Positive and Negative Syndrome ScaleReference Kay, Flszbein and Opfer21 and the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms;Reference Kring, Gur, Blanchard, Horan and Reise22 (c) self-rated depressive symptoms on the Beck Depression Inventory;Reference Beck, Steer and Brown23 (d) subjective quality of life measured as the mean of the 12 satisfaction items on the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life;Reference Priebe, Huxley, Knight and Evans24 (e) the objective social situation using the Objective Social Outcomes Index (SIX);Reference Priebe, Watzke, Hansson and Burns25 and (f) self-esteem measured on the Self-Esteem Rating Scale- Short From.Reference Lecomte, Corbière and Laisné26

In the intervention group the number and duration of volunteer–patient meetings within the 1-year period were documented. Patients who had at least 13 meetings, representing an average of one meeting a month, were defined as ‘compliers’. This figure was set a priori and did not include attendance at social events without the volunteer.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were carried out in accordance with an analysis plan that was signed off prior to data extraction. The analyses were conducted using Stata (version 14.2).

The required sample size was 84. Assuming 20% attrition, a total of 106 patients needed to be recruited in order to achieve 80% power to detect a standardised effect of 0.6 at two-sided 5% significance level. This was assumed to reflect an increase of 45 min of social activities per day, that is, more than double the expected baseline average of less than 45 min. The effect of missing data was accounted for by imputing data using multiple imputations by chained equations, with a Markov chain Monte Carlo sampler burn-in of ten draws to obtain ten complete data-sets.

Each outcome was compared at 12 months between intervention and control groups using a linear regression model, adjusting for baseline score of that outcome only. For the primary outcome (TUS) the analysis on the imputed data-set was the primary analysis, with the complete case analysis as a sensitivity analysis. A further sensitivity analysis was undertaken using quantile (median) regression with robust standard error estimation to assess the influence of outlier observations.

Given that patients could drop-out of the intervention and not be lost to follow-up, a complier average causal effect (CACE) analysis was conducted.Reference Dunn, Maracy and Dowrick27 The CACE estimate was obtained by a two-stage least square instrumental variable regression, adjusting for baseline scores, under the assumption of monotonicity, exclusion restriction and the stable unit treatment value assumption.

Secondary outcomes were analysed on a complete case basis. Because the SIX is considered a ranking scale, the quantile (median) regression was used for the treatment effect estimate. The treatment effect of the count variable of number of social contacts was estimated using negative binomial regression. For those outcomes that showed significant differences between the groups at the end of the programme at 12 months, regression models, adjusting for baseline scores, were conducted to compare the groups at the 6-month follow-up.

Ethics statement

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving participants/patients were approved by the Camden and Kings Cross Research Ethics Committee (15/LO/0674).

Results

A total of 1245 patients were screened. Of those who were screened as eligible and met with a researcher, 55% consented to participate in the study. At this stage, a number of patient-participants were excluded because they already engaged in social activities for 60 or more min per day, decided to withdraw prior to randomisation or had provided invalid consent. This resulted in a sample of 124 patients, with 63 randomised to the intervention and 61 to the active control. After 1-year, data for analysis were obtained from 46 patients in each group. Researchers became unmasked during two of the interviews at 12 months, once before and once after the assessment of the primary outcome. The CONSORT flow diagram is available in supplementary Fig. 1, available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.42.

Baseline characteristics of patients are shown in Table 1. Patients were largely men with an average length of illness of 14.5 years and of varied ethnic origin.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics

a. Data not obtained for one control and one intervention participant.

b. Data not obtained for one control participant.

Fifty-one volunteers were recruited and met with at least one patient. Most were women (73%, n = 37), from a white ethnic group (65%, n = 33) and with previous volunteering experience (71%, n = 36). The average age of volunteers was 27.8 (s.d. = 8.9) with a modal age of 21 years. Most volunteers were in full-time or part-time employment (59%, n = 30), with some in full-time education (22%, n = 11) or unemployed (10%, n = 5) and only one had retired.

Of the 63 patients allocated to the intervention, 14 never met a volunteer (22%). For five patients further contact at that stage revealed that they did not meet all eligibility criteria (one had previous befriending experience, one had a history of violence, three lost capacity to consent to the intervention) and nine were no longer interested once the reality of being matched with a volunteer became apparent.

Among those with an initial ‘matching’ meeting (n = 49) implementation of the programme was limited. Only half of the intended sample achieved the pre-defined threshold of at least 13 meetings (Fig. 1). For those who had at least one meeting, the median number of meetings across the year was 14 (range 1–42) with a mean duration of 90 min. Overall, 15 social events were organised and attended by a mean of six patients (range 3–9). Five patients were re-matched and thus had two volunteers over the course of the year. For four of these patients this was because their volunteer could no longer meet their commitment and had to drop-out of the programme. For the other patient their original volunteer was a woman and they expressed a strong preference for a befriender of the same gender.

Fig. 1 Befriending programme flow diagram.

In the intention-to-treat analysis, there was no significant difference between the intervention and active control groups on the primary outcome, that is, the time patients spent in activities at 12 months (adjusted difference 8.9, 95% CI −40.7 to 58.5, P = 0.72). Patients in the intervention group increased their activity from 20 to 81 min per day in the primary analysis with imputed data, which represents a larger difference than the one that was considered to be clinically meaningful for the sample size calculation. However, a similar increase (from 17 to 70 min) was found in the control group (Table 2.) The analysis of complete cases and analyses using quantile regression produced similar results. The CACE analysis exploring the association between compliance in the intervention arm and the primary outcome was non-significant (adjusted difference  9.2, 95% CI −68.9 to 87.2, P = 0.82).

Table 2 Primary outcome analysis

IQR, interquartile range.

a. Adjusted for baseline levels of Time Use Survey (TUS).

Data for secondary outcomes were available for 69–74% of patients (Table 3). For symptoms, quality of life and self-esteem there were no significant differences between the groups. However, patients in the intervention group had significantly more social contacts (adjusted difference 0.52, 95% CI 0.04–0.99, P = 0.03) and more favourable SIX scores (adjusted difference 2.45, 95% CI 1.06–5.67, P = 0.04). Since the SIX contains one item about having met a friend in the past week (no, 0; yes, 1), we conducted a post hoc analysis to determine whether that item drove the improvement on the SIX. The difference on that item alone was significant (intervention group: 19/62 = 31% at baseline and 22/45 = 49% at follow-up; control group: 23/61 = 38% at baseline, and 14/46 = 30% at follow-up, P = 0.04). Without that item, there was no significant difference between the groups (adjusted difference 0.92, 95% CI 0.36–2.33, P = 0.86).

Table 3 Secondary outcomes

a. Adjusted for baseline levels of the outcome.

b. Ordered logistic regression model estimate.

PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; CAINS, Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MANSA, Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life

* P < 0.05.

The analyses comparing the groups at the 6-month follow-up showed that patients in the intervention group still had significantly more social contacts (adjusted difference  0.73, 95% CI 0.05–1.40, P = 0.04), and better scores on the SIX (adjusted difference 3.05, CI 1.13–8.20, P = 0.03).

Patient perspectives

Patients reported various positive experiences of the programme, to which they assigned a range of benefits such as feeling more understood or self-confident, and having a greater sense of belonging to society at large.

‘I feel more confident about myself. I feel like people understand me more, that there is somebody that can be there. And she always talks to me about my feelings and makes me feel better.’ (Patient 30)

‘She thought that schizophrenics were interesting people who were worthy of getting to know and being treated with respect and not patronised or looked down upon or treated as if they were kind of kiddies. That was very therapeutic.’ (Patient 131)

Negative experiences of the programme were usually found among those who dropped out, either because they felt uncomfortable with the volunteer with whom they had been matched, or because the volunteer was unreliable and had themselves, dropped out.

‘I feel sometimes embarrassed because he's too young, he is younger than me and I want somebody… like at the most who is five years younger than me, four or five.’ (Patient 4)

‘I felt like I was pushing her to come without her being willing to come and meet me and go to the library and stuff like that. I feel a bit bad about that because for me it felt like she didn't want to be here. Like she was being forced to come and meet with me.’ (Patient 85)

Discussion

In this trial of a befriending programme for patients with schizophrenia, time spent in activities increased substantially in both arms but with no differential benefit for befriending. In terms of secondary outcomes, patients in the befriending programme had significantly more social contacts after 1 year – as reflected across two measures – and this difference held true 6 months after the end of the programme. This advantage was not associated with significant improvements in symptoms, quality of life or self-esteem. Both patients and volunteers engaged with the programme variably, and overall there were fewer meetings between patients and befrienders than envisaged. About a quarter of patients either never met a volunteer or did not proceed beyond the initial matching stage, and practically none had as many meetings as planned in the programme design.

Strength and limitations

This is the largest known RCT of befriending in a diagnostically homogeneous group of patients with schizophrenia.Reference McCorkle, Rogers, Dunn, Lyass and Wan16, Reference Sheridan, Drennan, Coughlan, O'Keeffe, Frazer and Kemple17 The befriending programme was carefully designed and implemented including both selection interviews and training for volunteers, frequent reminders, access to supervision and the offer of regular social events. A range of observer-rated and self-rated outcomes were assessed, and the programme organisers ensured that patients' accounts of social activities at the end of the programme were not influenced by meetings with the volunteers themselves. Finally, the positive findings on increased social contacts were found on two measures and at two points of time (i.e. at the end of the programme and 6 months later).

The study also has a number of weaknesses. The chosen primary outcome of time spent in social activities showed a more than threefold increase in both groups. Levels of time spent in social activities appear to have been lower at baseline and higher at follow-up compared with those reported in a trial with patients in early intervention services, in any group and at any point of time.Reference Fowler, Hodgekins, French, Marshall, Freemantal and McCrone28 This might raise questions about the validity of the measurement of this outcome. There was a protocol violation for five patients who had to be excluded from the intervention following randomisation. The follow-up rate of 74% is slightly lower than the average in trials on psychosocial interventions in patients with schizophrenia, although similar in both groups.Reference Szymczynska, Walsh, Greenberg and Priebe29 Moreover, the trial may have been influenced by specific contextual factors affecting the volunteers, such as the predominant recruitment through advertising in universities. This resulted in many of the volunteers being in their early twenties and much younger than the patients they befriended. Finally, the trial focused on gains that patients had achieved through befriending, whereas potential benefits for volunteers and for the wider community were not assessed.

Comparison with the literature

A meta-analysis across physical and mental health conditionsReference Siette, Cassidy and Priebe15 and a quasi-experimental study in the USAReference McCorkle, Rogers, Dunn, Lyass and Wan16 suggested a positive effect of befriending on patient-reported outcomes. Our RCT did not identify such benefits (i.e. depressive symptoms, subjective quality of life and self-esteem). Another departure from previous studies is a group difference on a behavioural outcome. The higher number of social contacts in the intervention group potentially reflects a reduction in social isolation and a gain not previously demonstrated in other studies. At baseline, patients had contacts with an average of one to two different people over a period of 4 days. Befriending increased this by an average of 0.5 people, albeit with considerable variation. Whether this increase represents a relevant change in the social life of patients with psychosis is difficult to judge, but should be encouraging given that the benefit was upheld 6 months later.

Our primary result is similar to an Irish trial with patients with severe mental illnessesReference Sheridan, Drennan, Coughlan, O'Keeffe, Frazer and Kemple17 in which marked improvements were found in both intervention and control groups. In that trial, both groups received financial support for social activities – in addition to the befriending provided to the intervention group – so the authors argued that the improvement across groups was because of the provision of financial support. In our trial, the control group only received information about social activities. This information, discussed on one occasion, is unlikely to explain a more than threefold increase in time spent in social activities 1 year later. One can only speculate as to whether this was a general Hawthorne effect of participating in a research study, or whether patients' accounts were influenced by a social desirability bias, motivating patients in both groups to inflate their reports at follow-up, whereas at baseline, reports of spending too much time in activities would have made them ineligible for the trial. Retrieving data on social contacts that included counting face-to-face meetings with different people or answering one dichotomous question about whether they had met a friend may have been less sensitive to such bias.

The considerable variability in the uptake and use of the programme may be explained by a recent survey of out-patients with psychotic and affective disorders.Reference Toner, Cassidy, Chevalier, Farreny, Leverton and da Costa30 Patients expressed a wide range of preferences for befriending in terms of frequency and content of meetings (i.e. talking or engaging in joint activities), the duration of the programme and the background of the volunteer. Similarly, volunteers also vary in their motivations, expectations and aims for befriending.Reference Hallett, Klug, Lauber and Priebe10, Reference Toner, Hickling, Pinto da Costa, Cassidy and Priebe11, Reference Klug, Toner, Fabisch and Priebe31 As they are not salaried staff, they cannot necessarily be expected to follow instructions and may not feel obliged to adhere to a fixed programme. Finally, it is difficult to judge the impact of large differences in age and background, present in many of the patient–volunteer dyads, which may have hindered the development of close social connections. Overall, one may conclude that a one-size-fits-all befriending programme is likely to have limited adherence.

Implications for research and practice

These findings have implications for both practice and research. In practice, patients and volunteers should be offered programmes with sufficient flexibility to accommodate their varying initial preferences and changes in preferences over time. This may be in regards to the duration of the relationship, the frequency of meetings, the personal motivations of the volunteer etc.

For research, the question arises as to whether RCTs are the most appropriate method for evaluating befriending programmes. Trials focus on gains achieved at the end of an intervention period. Yet, in the case of befriending relationships over 12 months, there may be experiences during that period that are not reflected in changed outcome criteria at the 1-year point, but are still important for patients, perhaps making overall participation worthwhile. In this trial we obtained data on the uptake, use and outcomes of befriending that are of interest, without considering outcomes in a control group. A first task for future research may be to collect such data from a large number of befriending programmes currently in practice. This may provide important information on the characteristics of patients and volunteers in befriending programmes, how often and for how long they meet; the characteristics of patients, volunteers and programmes that predict more enjoyable and longer lasting relationships; the nature of the experiences of patients and volunteers; and changes in outcome criteria, both self-report and behavioural. Given that many programmes are run by voluntary organisations with limited capacity for data documentation, this research might be difficult to conduct. Yet, collecting such data on a large scale may help to better understand the potentials and limitations of befriending. This is important as reviews and large surveys suggest that volunteers for befriending programmes with people with severe mental illnesses can be recruited from groups with very different characteristics and that there is a large pool of potential volunteers who could provide valuable input to patients.Reference Hallett, Klug, Lauber and Priebe10, Reference Toner, Hickling, Pinto da Costa, Cassidy and Priebe11, Reference Klug, Toner, Fabisch and Priebe31 Supporting their commitment and activities with the best possible evidence may be seen as a priority for research in public mental health.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) under its Programme Grants for Applied Research (Reference Number RP-PG-0611-20002). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the NIHR or the Department of Health in the UK. The funder had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation or writing of the manuscript. The authors had full access to the data, and S.P. had final responsibility for the decision to submit to publication.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to gratefully acknowledge the support of the funder, participants and the wider study team.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.42.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest: None.

References

1Giacco, D, Palumbo, C, Strappelli, N, Catapano, F, Priebe, S.Social contacts and loneliness in people with psychotic and mood disorders. Compr Psychiatry 2016; 66: 5966.10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.12.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Harley, EW, Boardman, J, Craig, T.Friendship in people with schizophrenia: a survey. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2012; 47: 1291–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Giacco, D, McCabe, R, Kallert, T, Hansson, L, Fiorillo, A, Priebe, S.Friends and symptom dimensions in patients with psychosis: a pooled analysis. PloS One 2012; 7: e50119.10.1371/journal.pone.0050119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4Becker, T, Leese, M, Clarkson, P, Taylor, R, Turner, D, Kleckham, J, et al. Links between social networks and quality of life: an epidemiologically representative study of psychotic patients in South London. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1998; 33: 299304.10.1007/s001270050058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Eklund, M, Hansson, L.Social network among people with persistent mental illness: associations with sociodemographic, clinical and health-related factors. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2007; 53: 293305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6Worswick, E, Dimic, S, Wildgrube, C, Priebe, S.Negative symptoms and avoidance of social interaction: a study of non-verbal behaviour. Psychopathology 2018; 51: 19.10.1159/000484414CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Hansen, CF, Torgalsboen, AK, Melle, I, Bell, MD.Passive/apathetic social withdrawal and active social avoidance in schizophrenia: difference in underlying psychological processes. J Nerv Ment Dis 2009; 197: 274–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Angermeyer, MC, Dietrich, S.Public beliefs about and attitudes towards people with mental illness: a review of population studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006; 113: 163–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Toner, S, Fabisch, K, Priebe, S, Klug, G.Attitudes towards severe mental illness and social distance: a survey of volunteer befrienders in Austria. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2018; 64: 470–5.10.1177/0020764018776346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Hallett, C, Klug, G, Lauber, C, Priebe, S.Volunteering in the care of people with severe mental illness: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 2012; 12: 226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Toner, S, Hickling, L, Pinto da Costa, M, Cassidy, M, Priebe, S.Characteristics, motivations and experiences of volunteer befrienders for people with mental illness: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. BMC Psychiatry 2018; 18: 378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Priebe, S, Omer, S, Giacco, D, Slade, M.Resource-oriented therapeutic models in psychiatry: conceptual review. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 204: 256–61.10.1192/bjp.bp.113.135038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Thompson, R, Valenti, E, Siette, J, Priebe, S.To befriend or to be a friend: a systematic review of the meaning and practice of “befriending” in mental health care. J Ment Health 2016; 25: 71–7.10.3109/09638237.2015.1021901CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14Mead, N, Lester, H, Chew-Graham, C, Gask, L, Bower, P.Effects of befriending on depressive symptoms and distress: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 196: 96101.10.1192/bjp.bp.109.064089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Siette, J, Cassidy, M, Priebe, S.Effectiveness of befriending interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open 2017; 7: e014304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16McCorkle, BH, Rogers, ES, Dunn, EC, Lyass, A, Wan, YM.Increasing social support for individuals with serious mental illness: evaluating the compeer model of intentional friendship. Community Ment Health J 2008; 44: 359–66.10.1007/s10597-008-9137-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Sheridan, AJ, Drennan, J, Coughlan, B, O'Keeffe, D, Frazer, K, Kemple, M, et al. Improving social functioning and reducing social isolation and loneliness among people with enduring mental illness: Report of a randomised controlled trial of supported socialisation. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2015; 61: 241–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18Priebe, S, Pavlickova, H, Eldridge, S, Golden, E, McCrone, P, Ockenden, N, et al. Effectiveness of one-to-one volunteer support for patients with psychosis: protocol of a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 2016; 6: e011582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
20Hodgekins, J, French, P, Birchwood, M, et al. Comparing time use in individuals at different stages of psychosis and a non-clinical comparison group. Schizophr Res 2015; 161: 188–93.10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21Kay, S, Flszbein, A, Opfer, L.Positive and negative scale for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13: 463–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22Kring, AM, Gur, RE, Blanchard, JJ, Horan, WP, Reise, SP.The clinical assessment interview for negative symptoms (CAINS): final development and validation. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170: 165–72.10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Brown, GK.Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation 1996.Google Scholar
24Priebe, S, Huxley, P, Knight, S, Evans, S.Application and results of the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA). Int J Soc Psychiatry 1999; 45: 712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25Priebe, S, Watzke, S, Hansson, L, Burns, T.Objective Social Outcomes Index (SIX): a method to summarise objective indicators of social outcomes in mental health care. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008; 118: 5763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26Lecomte, T, Corbière, M, Laisné, F.Investigating self-esteem in individuals with schizophrenia: relevance of the Self-Esteem Rating Scale-Short Form. Psychiatry Res 2006; 143: 99108.10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Dunn, G, Maracy, M, Dowrick, C, et al. Estimating psychological treatment effects from a randomised controlled trial with both non-compliance and loss to follow-up. Br J Psychiatry 2003; 183: 323–31.10.1192/bjp.183.4.323CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Fowler, D, Hodgekins, J, French, P, Marshall, M, Freemantal, N, McCrone, P, et al. Social recovery therapy in combination with early intervention services for enhancement of social recovery in patients with first-episode psychosis (SUPEREDEN3): a single-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet Psychiatry 2018; 5: 4150.10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30476-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29Szymczynska, P, Walsh, S, Greenberg, L, Priebe, S.Attrition in trials evaluating complex interventions for schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res 2017; 90: 6777.10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Toner, S, Cassidy, M, Chevalier, A, Farreny, A, Leverton, M, da Costa, MP, et al. Preferences for befriending schemes: a survey of patients with severe mental illness. BMC Psychiatry 2018; 18: 64.10.1186/s12888-018-1643-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31Klug, G, Toner, S, Fabisch, K, Priebe, S.Characteristics and motivations of volunteers providing one-to-one support for people with mental illness: a survey in Austria. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2018; 53: 841–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Baseline characteristics

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Befriending programme flow diagram.

Figure 2

Table 2 Primary outcome analysis

Figure 3

Table 3 Secondary outcomes

Supplementary material: File

Priebe et al. supplementary material

Priebe et al. supplementary material 1

Download Priebe et al. supplementary material(File)
File 222.7 KB
Supplementary material: Image

Priebe et al. supplementary material

Priebe et al. supplementary material 2

Download Priebe et al. supplementary material(Image)
Image 827.8 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.