Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:39:01.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Effect of Distraction on Schizophrenic Performance (1) Perception and Immediate Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Andrew McGhie
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of St. Andrews, Queen's College, Dundee
James Chapman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of St. Andrews, Queen's College, Dundee
J. S. Lawson
Affiliation:
From the Royal Dundee Liff Hospital and the Department of Psychiatry, University of St. Andrews

Extract

In recent years an increasing number of workers investigating schizophrenic behaviour have concluded that many of the symptoms found in schizophrenia are related to a disturbance in the selective and inhibitory functions of attention. One of the earliest statements of this argument is found in Norman Cameron's (1938, 1939, 1944) concept of “over-inclusion”, which he used to describe the schizophrenic patient's tendency to include many elements irrelevant to the central idea in his thinking. Shakow (1962) reached the following conclusions in summarizing his own psychological studies of schizophrenia—“It is as if, in the scanning process which takes place before the response to stimulus is made, the schizophrenic is unable to select out the material relevant for optimal response. He apparently cannot free himself from the irrelevant among the numerous possibilities available for choice.” Weckowicz and Blewett (1959), in their studies of alterations in perceptual constancy in schizophrenic patients, interpreted their findings as suggesting that the patient's basic difficulty was that of “an inability to attend selectively or to select relevant information“. Venables and his colleagues (1959, 1962, 1963), in a series of studies on the arousal level of schizophrenic patients, also concluded that many of the behavioural abnormalities demonstrated were due to variations in the range of attention. In a series of investigations carried out by Payne and his colleagues (1960, 1961, 1963) to develop Cameron's concept of over-inclusive thinking in schizophrenia, the authors utilized Broad-bent's (1958) model of selective attention to postulate that this form of thought disorder is basically due “to a defect in some hypothetical central filter mechanism, the function of which

is to screen out irrelevant data both internal … and external … to allow for the most efficient processing of incoming information”.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1965 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. London: Pergamon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, N. (1938). “Reasoning, regression and communication in schizophrenics.” Psychol. Monogr., 50, 211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, N. (1939). “Schizophrenic thinking in a problem-solving situation.” J. Ment. Sci., 85, 1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, N. (1944). “Experimental analysis of schizophrenic thinking”, in Language and Thought in Schizophrenia (ed. Kasanin, ). California: University Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, J. A., and McGhie, A. (1962). “A comparative study of disordered attention in schizophrenia.” J. Ment. Sci., 108, 455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, R. (1964). “Acoustic confusions in immediate memory.” Brit. J. Psychol., 55, 75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, A. L. (1960). Experimental Design in Psychological Research. New York: Holts, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Payne, R. W., and Hewlett, J. H. G. (1960). “Thought disorder in psychotic patients”, in Experiments in Personality (ed. Eysenck, ). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Payne, R. W., (1961). “Cognitive abnormalities”, in Handbook of Abnormal Psychology (ed. Eysenck, ). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Payne, R. W., Ancevich, S., and Laverty, S. G. (1963). “Over-inclusive thinking in symptom-free schizophrenics.” Canad. psychiat. Ass. J., 8, 225.Google Scholar
Shakow, D. (1962). “Segmental set—a theory of the formal psychological deficit in schizophrenia.” Arch. gen. Psychiat., 6, 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperling, D. (1960). “The information available in brief visual presentations.” Psychol. Monogr., 74, 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, P. H., and O'Connor, N. (1959). “Reaction times to auditory and visual stimulation in schizophrenic and normal subjects.” Quart. J. exp. Psychol., 11, 175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, P. H. and Wing, J. K. (1962). “Level of arousal and the subclassification of schizophrenia.” Arch. gen. Psychiat., 7, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, P. H. (1963). “Selectivity of attention, withdrawal and cortical activation.” Ibid., 9, 74.Google Scholar
Weckowicz, T. E., and Blewett, D. B. (1959). “Size constancy and abstract thinking in schizophrenic patients.” J. Ment. Sci., 105, 909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.