Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T05:21:51.179Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Discrimination and delusional ideation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

I. Janssen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology Maastricht University
M. Hanssen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology Maastricht University
M. Bak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology Maastricht University
R. V. Bijl
Affiliation:
Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
R. De Graaf
Affiliation:
Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
W. Vollebergh
Affiliation:
Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
K. McKenzie
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
J. Van Os*
Affiliation:
Maastricht University and Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
*
Professor Jim van Os, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Maastricht University, PO Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands. Tel: +31 43 387 5443; fax: +31 43 387 5444; e-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

In the UK and The Netherlands, people with high rates of psychosis are chronically exposed to discrimination.

Aims

To test whether perceived discrimination is associated longitudinally with onset of psychosis.

Method

A 3-year prospective study of cohorts with no history of psychosis and differential rates of reported discrimination on the basis of age, gender, disability, appearance, skin colour or ethnicity and sexual orientation was conducted in the Dutch general population (n=4076). The main outcome was onset of psychotic symptoms (delusions and hallucinations).

Results

The rate of delusional ideation was 0.5% (n=19) in those who did not report discrimination, 0.9% (n=4) in those who reported discrimination in one domain, and 2.7% (n=3) in those who reported discrimination in more than one domain (exact P=0.027). This association remained after adjustment for possible confounders. No association was found between baseline discrimination and onset of hallucinatory experiences.

Conclusions

Perceived discrimination may induce delusional ideation and thus contribute to the high observed rates of psychotic disorder in exposed minority populations.

Type
Preliminary Report
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2003 

There are consistent reports of high rates of affective and non-affective psychotic disorder and psychotic and psychosis-like symptoms in groups of people of African and Caribbean origin in the UK and The Netherlands (Reference Harrison, Owens and HoltonHarrison et al, 1988; Reference Selten and SijbenSelten & Sijben, 1994; Reference Sharpley and PetersSharpley & Peters, 1999; Reference Johns, Nazroo and BebbingtonJohns et al, 2002). These populations are subject to racial discrimination (Reference Chahal and JulienneChahal & Julienne, 1999) and there are claims that the association between ethnic group and psychosis is confounded by racial discrimination (Reference Littlewood and LipsedgeLittlewood & Lipsedge, 1997; Reference Boydell, van Os and McKenzieBoydell et al, 2001). A national UK study has reported increased rates of common mental disorder and psychosis in those who say they have been the victims of racial discrimination (Reference Karlsen and NazrooKarlsen & Nazroo, 2002). However, a large study in the USA has shown that perceived discrimination is commonplace, and although people who described themselves as ‘US non-Hispanic Black’ are more likely to suffer from discrimination, it was perceived discrimination itself, not discrimination linked to ethnic group, that was associated with non-psychotic mental illness (Reference Kessler, Mickelson and WilliamsKessler et al, 1999). The same may apply to psychotic illness. Psychosis-like phenomena such as delusional ideation and isolated hallucinations are prevalent in the general population (Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000) and constitute a risk factor for the development of clinical psychotic disorders (Reference Poulton, Caspi and MoffittPoulton et al, 2000; Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2001). Immigrant groups with high rates of psychotic disorder, such as African—Caribbeans living in the UK (Reference Harrison, Owens and HoltonHarrison et al, 1988), also have higher rates of these psychosis-like phenomena (Reference Sharpley and PetersSharpley & Peters, 1999; Reference Johns, Nazroo and BebbingtonJohns et al, 2002). A theory that integrates these findings is that chronic experience of discrimination, regardless of ethnic group, might give rise to a paranoid attributional style and higher rates of psychosis-like phenomena, which put individuals at risk of developing clinical psychotic states (Reference McKenzie, van Os and FahyMcKenzie et al, 1995; Reference Gilvarry, Walsh and SameleGilvarry et al, 1999). However, a direct link between discrimination and psychotic phenomena needs to be made if such a theory is to be entertained.

Our hypothesis was that people who reported that they had been the subject of perceived discrimination would be more likely subsequently to develop psychotic symptoms, regardless of their ethnic origin. We tested this hypothesis using data from a longitudinal, random population sample of 7076 individuals interviewed for the presence of psychotic symptoms.

METHOD

Sample

This study is a part of The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS), a longitudinal study of the prevalence, incidence, course and consequences of psychiatric disorders in the Dutch general population (van Os et al, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2000, Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijl2001). The local ethics committee approved the study proposal. Participants were contacted at three points in time: 1996 (baseline), 1997 (T 1, assessing the period between baseline and T 1) and 1999 (T 2, assessing the period between T 1 and T 2). A multi-stage, stratified, random sampling procedure was used to identify the sample. First, 90 municipalities were sampled randomly. Second, households were randomly selected. Third, the individual living in the household with the most recent birthday and aged between 18-64 years was asked to participate. Those living in institutions, including psychiatric hospitals, were not included in the sampling frame. Only those fluent in Dutch could participate so the proportion from ethnic minorities was limited. All participants received an introductory letter from the Ministry of Health.

A total of 7076 participants were enlisted at baseline. The response rate was 69.7%. No difference in psychiatric morbidity based on the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12; Reference GoldbergGoldberg, 1978) was found between responders and non-responders. At T 1, 5618 persons participated; this reduced to 4848 at T 2.

Risk-set

We were interested in whether perceived discrimination at baseline was associated with psychotic symptoms at 3-year follow-up. Because of this, we restricted our sample to those individuals who at baseline had a lifetime rating of no psychotic symptoms (or psychosis-like experiences) on all the individual items of a psychosis screening interview and had been interviewed for the presence of psychotic symptoms at T 2. This was done for three reasons. First, this skewed the sample towards individuals with true first-ever occurrence of psychotic experiences at T 2. Second, it decreased bias due to the influence of baseline psychotic or psychosis-like symptoms on the reporting of discrimination (for example, individuals with paranoid symptoms may perceive, and report, more discrimination). Finally, it reduced the possibility of reversed causality, i.e. ‘strange’ individuals with psychosis-like symptoms being the subject of discrimination. Our risk-set included 5838 persons at baseline. Of these, 4067 (70%) were interviewed at T 2.

Instruments

The participants were interviewed at home with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI), version 1.1 (Reference Smeets and DingemansSmeets & Dingemans, 1993) at baseline, T 1 and T 2. The CIDI generates DSM—III—R diagnoses (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) and is designed for trained interviewers who are not clinicians. Interviewers read out questions in a standardised way and record respondents' answers, making the CIDI essentially a self-report instrument. The CIDI psychosis section consists of 17 core psychosis items (G1-G13, G15, G16, G20 and G21). These psychosis items correspond to classic psychotic symptoms, including persecution, thought interference, auditory hallucinations and passivity phenomena. Both clinically relevant stressful psychotic experiences and subclinical psychosis-like experiences are rated by the CIDI (Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000). For example, the experience of sometimes hearing a friendly voice in the absence of any secondary delusional ideation would be rated as a non-clinical psychosis-like experience, whereas the experience of hostile voices for which the person was seeking help because of distress would be rated as a clinical symptom. Personality tests completed at baseline were the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, a ten-item, four-point Likert scale yielding a global self-esteem score (Reference RosenbergRosenberg, 1965), the 14-item Groningen Neuroticism Scale (Reference OrmelOrmel, 1980) and a five-item mastery scale indicating locus of control (Reference Pearlin and SchoolerPearlin & Schooler, 1978).

Final assessment of incident psychotic symptoms

As psychotic symptoms may be difficult to diagnose by lay interviewers, further clinical interviews, guided by the findings of the CIDI interview on psychotic symptoms, were conducted over the telephone by an experienced clinician (psychiatrist, senior psychiatric trainee or psychologist) with all individuals who had evidence of psychosis on any of the CIDI psychosis items at T 2 (Reference Krabbendam, Janssen and BakKrabbendam et al, 2002). The proportion of eligible individuals at T 2 who were successfully reinterviewed by the clinician was 74.4%.

The T 2 diagnosis of psychotic symptoms by clinicians at telephone interview was made using the three items of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS): ‘unusual thought content’, ‘hallucinations’ and ‘conceptual disorganisation’ (Reference Overall and GorhamOverall & Gorham, 1962). Each symptom was scored on a range from 1 (absent) to 7 (very severe). The BPRS items ‘unusual thought content’ and ‘hallucinations’ represent the positive symptoms for psychosis. Delusional ideation was defined as a rating greater than 1 for the BPRS ‘unusual thought content’ item; presence of hallucinations as a rating greater than 1 for the BPRS item ‘hallucinations’.

Perceived discrimination

At the baseline interview, participants were asked if they had experienced discrimination over the past year because of their skin colour or ethnicity; gender; age; appearance; disability; or sexual orientation. Participants answered ‘yes’ or ‘no’ to each of the six questions.

Statistical analysis

The rate of onset of delusional ideation was tabulated as a function of level of baseline perceived discrimination and statistically evaluated using Fisher's exact test. Adjustment for confounders was made using logistic regression in the STATA statistical program (StataCorp, 2001), yielding odds ratios. Perceived discrimination score was analysed as a three-level variable: 0, no discrimination (86%); 1, reported discrimination in one domain (11%); 2, reported discrimination in more than one domain (3%).

The following confounders of the association between perceived discrimination and psychosis were selected a priori, guided by the literature: gender, age (five groups), urbanicity of place of residence (three levels), level of education (four levels), unemployment, single marital status and presence of any baseline CIDI-generated DSM—III—R diagnosis. Level of education, unemployment and single marital status are risk factors for psychotic illness that could arguably also be associated with higher rates of discrimination from others (i.e. reversed causality). Because psychotic illness may be preceded by non-psychotic illness and there are increased rates of discrimination in people with any mental health problems, we adjusted for the presence of any DSM—III—R diagnosis at baseline and, to increase sensitivity, for the presence of CIDI depressive symptoms E1 (persistent low mood over a period of 2 weeks) and E2 (persistent low mood over a period of 2 years). In order to take into account the effect of personality traits that might facilitate reporting of discrimination or the development of paranoid attributions, we also corrected for baseline self-esteem, neuroticism and locus of control.

The NEMESIS study had no precise measures of ethnicity and, as explained above, those who could not speak Dutch fluently were excluded. Country of birth was used as proxy of minority status, defined dichotomously as 0, ‘born in The Netherlands’, and 1, ‘other’. However, as none of the 213 individuals born outside The Netherlands had BPRS presence of hallucinations and only one had evidence of BPRS delusional ideation, the proxy measure of minority status was not adjusted for in the analyses.

Missing data sensitivity analyses

As not all of the eligible participants at T 2 who had displayed evidence of psychosis were interviewed again (the clinical re-interview rate at T 2 was 74.4%, as described above), we conducted analyses to examine whether differential attrition could have biased any findings. This was done by multiple imputation of missing values of perceived discrimination at baseline (n=4 missing) and BPRS delusional ideation and hallucinatory experiences at T 2 (n=1793 missing) using the HOTDECK command in STATA. The HOTDECK procedure is used several times within a multiple imputation sequence since missing data are imputed stochastically rather than deterministically. One thousand imputation sequences were run, yielding 1000 data-sets in which the average logistic regression effect size of perceived discrimination on delusional ideation and hallucinatory experience was estimated within the HOTDECK procedure. Imputation of missing values was stratified by the following important determinants: age, gender, urbanicity, unemployment, country of birth, single marital status, any DSM—III—R baseline diagnosis, and level of education.

RESULTS

Effect of perceived discrimination

The mean age of the risk-set at T2 was 41.4 years (s.d. 11.8), and 2144 (53%) were women. The rates of baseline perceived discrimination were: skin colour or ethnicity 75 (2%), gender 182 (4%), age 261 (6%), appearance 80 (2%), disability 77 (2%) and sexual preference 13 (0.3%). Gender, urbanicity, single marital status, educational level, unemployment and baseline DSM—III—R diagnosis were associated with perceived discrimination (Table 1). The rate of delusional ideation was 0.5% in those who did not report discrimination, 0.9% in those who reported discrimination in one domain and 2.7% in those who reported it in more than one domain (exact P=0.027; OR=2.1, 95% CI 1.2-3.8). This association remained after adjustment for demographic variables, non-psychotic DSM—III—R diagnosis at baseline and indicators of premorbid social adjustment (OR=2.3, 95% CI 1.2-4.2); it also remained after adjustment for the previous confounders and additionally for baseline CIDI depressive symptoms E1 and E2, and the baseline personality measures of neuroticism, self-esteem and locus of control (OR=2.1, 95% CI 1.1-4.0). No association was found between baseline discrimination and onset of hallucinatory experiences.

Table 1 Baseline experience of discrimination during the preceding year related to age, gender, minority status, urbanicity of residence, level of education, unemployment, baseline diagnosis and single marital status

Discrimination score χ2 d.f.
0 1 2
n % n % n %
Total 3521 86 432 11 114 3
Gender
Male 1708 89 170 9 45 2
Female 1813 85 262 12 69 3 15.8*** 2
Age (years)
18-24 218 81 37 14 13 5
25-34 900 86 110 11 32 3
35-44 981 87 122 11 26 2
45-54 801 86 99 11 30 3
55-64 621 89 64 9 13 2 13.4 8
Minority status
Born in Netherlands 3346 87 401 10 103 3
Other 175 81 31 14 11 5 8.0* 2
Urbanicity
Lowest 640 90 67 9 7 1
Middle 1643 87 198 10 50 3
Highest 1238 85 167 11 57 4 18.3** 4
Level of education
Lowest 1062 84 156 12 46 4
Medium low 278 87 32 10 8 3
Medium high 1290 87 152 10 38 3
Highest 852 89 84 9 19 2 14.5* 6
Employment status
Unemployed/disabled 141 67 52 25 18 8
Other 3380 88 380 10 96 2 77.3*** 2
Marital status
Never been married 797 81 146 15 46 5
Other 2724 88 286 9 68 2 42.4*** 2
Any baseline diagnosis
No DSM—III—R diagnosis 2337 89 230 9 52 2
DSM—III—R diagnosis 1184 82 202 14 62 4 47.0*** 2

Multiple imputation of both baseline discrimination and follow-up delusional ideation and hallucinatory experience revealed a similar effect of perceived discrimination at baseline on delusional ideation at follow-up (OR=1.9, 95% CI 1.1-3.4), and no effect on hallucinatory experiences (OR=1.2, 95% CI 0.5-2.9).

DISCUSSION

The findings suggest that perceived discrimination predicts, in a dose—response fashion, incident delusional ideation.

Study limitations

There are several methodological limitations. First, there were no suitable measures of ethnicity, and only individuals with sufficient proficiency in Dutch were included. This selection might have led to exclusion of members of minority groups with the lowest levels of acculturation and, possibly, with discrimination.

It could be argued that our findings may represent residual confounding by ethnic group (being exposed to more discrimination and having higher rates of psychosis). However, even when we restricted the sample to individuals who were born in The Netherlands and whose father and mother had both been born in The Netherlands (thus effectively excluding current immigrant groups living in The Netherlands), the effect size of perceived discrimination on delusional ideation was not reduced (OR=2.0, 95% CI 1.0-4.0). Another test of this possible bias is to exclude reported discrimination on the basis of skin colour from the discrimination measure in the logistic regression. Excluding these individuals increased rather than decreased the adjusted OR (OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.3-4.8), again indicating that this bias was not operating. These limitations, therefore, should not undermine the main finding reported in this study that perceived discrimination at baseline is associated with onset of delusional ideation.

Table 2 Odds ratios for the onset of psychosis at final assessment (T 2) after experience of discrimination during the year preceding baseline measurement

BPRS delusional ideation BPRS hallucination
Presence Absence Presence Absence
n % n % n % n %
Baseline level of discrimination
0 19 0.5 3485 99.5 19 0.5 3485 99.5
1 4 0.9 424 99.1 1 0.2 427 99.8
2 3 2.7 4 97.3 2 1.8 111 98.2
Fisher's exact test P=0.027 P=0.13
Odds ratio
Linear trend1 (95% CI) 2.1 (1.2-3.8) 1.3 (0.6-3.0)
Trend adjusted2 (95% CI) 2.3 (1.2-4.2) 1.2 (0.5-2.7)

Second, as in all longitudinal population surveys, there was substantial attrition between baseline and T 2. However, detailed analyses of attrition in this sample suggest that attrition was largely random as far as baseline psychopathology is concerned (Reference de Graaf, Bijl and Smitde Graaf et al, 2000), justifying our method of multiple imputation as a means to conduct sensitivity analyses. Post hoc analyses with the main exposure in this study — perceived discrimination — confirmed the randomness of missing values: the mean baseline discrimination score in those who were interviewed at T 2 was 0.17, s.d. 0.48 (n=4067; proportion who reported discrimination at baseline in at least one domain 13.4%), and in those who were not interviewed 0.17, s.d. 0.48 (n=1767; proportion who reported discrimination in at least one domain 13.4% — rounded figures coincidentally are similar).

Third, the measurement of perceived discrimination was not refined. Respondents were unable to report the frequency and degree of perceived discrimination, whether this was day-to-day minor incidents or major life-altering incidents or assaults. In addition, our measure of perceived discrimination was broad and included a whole range of discriminatory perceptions, including gender and age. However, previous work has suggested that mental health variables are sensitive to perceived discrimination, regardless of type of discrimination (age, gender, racial, etc.) (Reference Kessler, Mickelson and WilliamsKessler et al, 1999).

Fourth, the statistical resolution of the study was limited, with only 26 cases of onset of broadly defined delusional ideation, in 7 of which some degree of discrimination was reported at baseline. The effect of perceived discrimination was nevertheless statistically precise, and was present after stringent exclusion of all individuals who at baseline had evidence of any level of psychotic experiences that could have interfered with subjective report of discrimination. It is therefore unlikely that the association between perceived discrimination and psychosis was biased by psychosis-prone or psychosis-prodromal individuals reporting more discrimination.

In order to exclude the possibility of reversed causality (premorbid social and cognitive deficits associated with psychotic illness giving rise to discrimination), we adjusted for employment status, marital status and level of education, three variables that have been shown to be sensitive indicators of premorbid deficits in psychotic illness (Reference van Os, Takei and Castlevan Os et al, 1995). Adjustment did not reduce the discrimination parameter and even increased it, making it unlikely that the effect of perceived discrimination is confounded to a large degree by premorbid social competence. Similarly, adjustment for non-psychotic diagnoses and symptoms as possible precursors of later psychotic illness also did not affect the results, nor did adjustment for possible confounding measures of personality.

Possible mechanisms

Previous work has shown associations between perceived racial and non-racial discrimination and non-psychotic poor mental health (Reference Kessler, Mickelson and WilliamsKessler et al, 1999; Reference Mays and CochranMays & Cochran, 2001). A cross-sectional national study in the UK has demonstrated an association between perceived discrimination and the rate of psychotic and non-psychotic mental illness in non-White ethnic minority groups (Reference Karlsen and NazrooKarlsen & Nazroo, 2002). Our study is, as far as we are aware, the first study to provide prospective evidence of a link between perceived discrimination and delusional ideation, suggestive of a causal link.

There is growing evidence that cognitive attributions play a part in the onset of psychotic symptoms such as delusions (Reference Bentall, Corcoran and HowardBentall et al, 2001). Chronic experience of discrimination may have effects on attributions of daily events (Reference Gilvarry, Walsh and SameleGilvarry et al, 1999), thus facilitating an understandably paranoid attributional style (Reference Sharpley and PetersSharpley & Peters, 1999). Such effects may be particularly important over a life course, thus forming a plausible explanation for the high observed rates in second-generation immigrants. Perceived discrimination (the exposure used in this study) does not have to be proved to reflect identifiable acts of discrimination. For the argument by which cognitive mechanisms result in psychosis this may not be relevant, as subjective perception of discrimination alone is the prerequisite for the development of a paranoid attributional style. As explained above, it is unlikely that our measure of discrimination was merely a measure of paranoid personality, as all individuals with any level of paranoid ideation at baseline (CIDI has four items measuring paranoid symptoms, G1, G2, G3 and G4, which are sensitive to picking up paranoid ideation; Reference van Os, Hanssen and Bijlvan Os et al, 2000) were excluded from the study.

Discrimination and contextual effects

If discrimination has a role in the onset of psychosis, it is likely to be modified by a range of contextual factors. Studies within the African American population show that the amount of discrimination that they encounter depends on how dark their skin is (Reference WilliamsWilliams, 1999) and that more US non-Hispanic Blacks report often encountering day-to-day discrimination than do non- Hispanic Whites, with ‘other’ ethnic groups in between the two (Reference Kessler, Mickelson and WilliamsKessler et al, 1999). The impact of perceived discrimination on the risk of hypertension in African Americans depends on their social class and coping style. Ignoring discrimination raises the risk of hypertension in high-income African Americans but lowers the risk in those with low incomes (Reference Krieger and SidneyKrieger & Sidney, 1997). Mortality rates in US states have been shown to be associated with the degree to which Whites disrespect Blacks. Although the effect on Blacks was alarming and significant, there was also a demonstrable effect on Whites. High rates of racial disrespect in an area are thus associated with lower life expectancy for Whites and Blacks (Reference Kennedy, Kawachi and LochnerKennedy et al, 1997). Further work should attempt to take into account such contextual effects.

Discrimination, ethnic group and psychosis

This study focused on the possible effect of perceived discrimination on the onset of delusional ideation, regardless of ethnic group, but could not establish to what degree the reported associations between minority ethnic group, psychotic disorder and psychotic symptoms are actually confounded by perceived discrimination. There is nevertheless validity to this supposition. First, the increase in the rate of psychotic illness in ethnic minority groups is not static: in electoral wards it has been shown to be related to the proportion of the ward population that are from ethnic minority groups. The incidence rate is lower when the proportion of the population from ethnic minorities is higher. These data are thought to reflect decreased exposure to, or increased protection from, racial discrimination in areas with relatively high proportions of ethnic minorities (Reference Boydell, van Os and McKenzieBoydell et al, 2001). Second, it has been shown, albeit in a study of non-psychotic mental illness, that it was perceived discrimination itself and not ethnic group that was associated with poor mental health (Reference Kessler, Mickelson and WilliamsKessler et al, 1999). Third, the psychosis outcome in this study, delusional ideation, has been shown to be more prevalent in African—Caribbean people living in the UK (Reference Sharpley and PetersSharpley & Peters, 1999), and it is a known risk factor for more severe psychotic states (Reference Poulton, Caspi and MoffittPoulton et al, 2000). On the other hand, it is unlikely that discrimination is the sole factor contributing to the observed excess risk in ethnic minority groups. Higher rates of isolated hallucinations have also been reported in ethnic minority groups (Reference Johns, Nazroo and BebbingtonJohns et al, 2002), but the results of the current study failed to find an effect of discrimination on hallucinatory experiences.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Experience of discrimination may be a social risk factor for delusional ideation, but not for hallucinatory experiences.

  2. Reports of excess risk of psychosis among ethnic minorities may be confounded by experience of discrimination.

  3. Chronic experience of discrimination on the basis of appearance may have effects on attributions of daily events, facilitating an understandably paranoid attributional style.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The incidence of psychosis was low and the statistical resolution limited.

  2. The exposure was based on perceived discrimination rather than observable acts of discrimination.

  3. The study focused on the effect of perceived discrimination on the onset of psychosis, but could not establish to what degree perceived discrimination confounds the association between ethnic minority group and psychosis.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

This research was supported by the Dutch Ministry of Health.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM–III–R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P., Corcoran, R., Howard, R., et al (2001) Persecutory delusions: a review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 11431192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boydell, J., van Os, J., McKenzie, K., et al (2001) Incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities in London: ecological study into interactions with environment. BMJ, 323, 1336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chahal, K. & Julienne, L. (1999) ‘We Can't All Be White!’ Racist Victimisation in the UK. London: YPS.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R., Bijl, R. V., Smit, F., et al (2000) Psychiatric and sociodemographic predictors of attrition in a longitudinal study: The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). American Journal of Epidemiology, 152, 10391047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilvarry, C. M., Walsh, E., Samele, C., et al (1999) Life events, ethnicityand perceptions of discrimination in patients with severemental illness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 600608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, D. P. (1978) General Health Questionnaire (12–item). Windsor: NFER–Nelson.Google Scholar
Harrison, G., Owens, D., Holton, A., et al (1988) A prospective study of severe mental disorder in A fro-Caribbean patients. Psychological Medicine, 18, 643657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, L. C., Nazroo, J. Y., Bebbington, P., et al (2002) Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 174178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsen, S. & Nazroo, J. Y. (2002) Relation between racial discrimination, social class, and health among ethnic minority groups. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 624631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, B. P., Kawachi, I., Lochner, K., et al (1997) (Dis)respect and black mortality. Ethnicity and Disease, 7, 207214.Google ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Mickelson, K. D. & Williams, D. R. (1999) The prevalence, distribution, and mental health correlates of perceived discrimination in the United States. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 40, 208230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krabbendam, L., Janssen, I., Bak, M., et al (2002) Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for psychosis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krieger, N. & Sidney, S. (1997) Prevalence and health implications of anti-gay discrimination: a study of black and white women and men in the CARDIA cohort. Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults. International Journal of Health Services, 27, 157176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Littlewood, R. & Lipsedge, M. (1997) Aliens and Alienists. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mays, V. M. & Cochran, S. D. (2001) Mental health correlates of perceived discrimination among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 18691876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenzie, K., van Os, J., Fahy, T., et al (1995) Psychosis with good prognosis in A fro-Caribbean people now living in the United Kingdom. BMJ, 311, 13251328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ormel, J. (1980) Moeite met Leven of een Moeilijk Leven? Groningen: University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Overall, J. E. & Gorham, D. R. (1962) The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychological Report, 10, 799812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearlin, L. I. & Schooler, C. (1978) The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulton, R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., et al (2000) Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study [in process citation]. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 10531058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. (1965) Society and the Adolescent Self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selten, J. P. & Sijben, N. (1994) First admission rates for schizophrenia in immigrants to The Netherlands. The Dutch National Register. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 29, 7177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharpley, M. S. & Peters, E. R. (1999) Ethnicity, class and schizotypy. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 507512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeets, R. M. W. & Dingemans, P. M. A. J. (1993) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI), Version 1.1. Amsterdam/Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
StataCorp (2001) STATA Statistical Software: Release 70. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Takei, N., Castle, D. J., et al (1995) Premorbid abnormalities in mania, schizomania, acute schizophrenia and chronic schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 30, 274278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., et al (2000) Strauss (1969) revisted: apsychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophrenia Research, 45, 1120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., et al (2001) Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms: an urban–rural comparison. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 663668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. R. (1999) Race, socioeconomic status, and health. The added effects of racism and discrimination. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896, 173188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Baseline experience of discrimination during the preceding year related to age, gender, minority status, urbanicity of residence, level of education, unemployment, baseline diagnosis and single marital status

Figure 1

Table 2 Odds ratios for the onset of psychosis at final assessment (T2) after experience of discrimination during the year preceding baseline measurement

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.