Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:57:48.230Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Common mental disorders in a young urban population in Colombia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Trudy Harpham*
Affiliation:
London South Bank University, London, UK
Simon Snoxell
Affiliation:
London South Bank University, London, UK
Emma Grant
Affiliation:
London South Bank University, London, UK
Carlos Rodriguez
Affiliation:
Fundaps (Consulting Foundation in Health and Social Development Programmes), Cali, Colombia
*
Professor Trudy Harpham, London South Bank University, 103 Borough Road, London SE1 0AA, UK. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

There are few studies of mental ill health among young people in developing countries.

Aims

To measure the prevalence of common mental disorders among low-income young people in the city of Cali, Colombia and to examine associations with violence and social capital.

Method

The Self-Reporting Questionnaire was administered to 1057 young people aged 15–25 years. Social capital, violence, alcoholism and socio-demographic variables were also measured.

Results

We found 255 young people (24%) with common mental disorders. Being a woman, having limited education and experiencing high levels of violence were the main risk factors for common mental disorders. Social capital did not emerge as a risk factor.

Conclusions

A large burden of mental ill health among young people was found; this requires urgent interventions and more research on the mechanisms which link mental health and violence.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

‘There remains a need to further increase understanding of the critical factors and influences which contribute to young people's mental health.’ (Reference Donald, Dower and LuckeDonald et al, 2000)

Although the heavy burden of mental ill health in developing countries has been recognised (World Health Organization, 1994), there are few studies on the mental health of young people. In a number of developing countries, political and economic conflict creates violent situations for young people in both the public and private spheres. This paper focuses upon one such area: Cali, Colombia. It presents the prevalence of mental ill health among low-income young people and examines associations with socio-demographic factors, social capital, alcoholism and violence. The hypotheses were that experience and perceptions of violence were directly associated with common mental disorders in young people, and that social capital was inversely associated with common mental disorders.

METHOD

Cali is one of the major cities in Colombia, with a population of approximately two million. It is a city in profound economic crisis, and has great inequalities across its 21 boroughs. The focus of the current study is the large very poor area located on the eastern fringe of the city and known as Aguablanca. This area has a population of around 500 000. The city, and particularly Aguablanca, has high homicide rates. In 2001, Cali's homicide rate was 90 per 100 000 (Observatorio Social, 2003). Cali's young people are particular victims of this violence. In 2002, 44% of the city's homicide victims were aged between 15 and 25 years (the age range of this study) (Observatorio Social, 2003). In Aguablanca, the unemployment rate is approximately 25%, 28% live in poverty (defined as less than US$3.20 per person per day), and around 30% of the total population of the borough are between 10 and 24 years of age (Municipality of Cali, 2000).

As this research was part of a quasi-experimental intervention study, the sampling was based on expected reductions in prevalence of key variables. Using a formula for the comparison of two proportions and given the possibility of cluster effects and refusals, a final sample size of 1168 was arrived at by assuming: a prevalence of key violence variables of 50% (as is the practice of CISALVA, the Violence Research Institute of the University of Valle, Cali); objectives of 10% reduction in the incidence of violence; and 30% reduction in perceptions of violence (Stat-Calc software, version 5.4 for Windows; AcaStat Software, Ashburn, Virginia, USA). Neighbourhood maps indicating dwellings were obtained from the city planning department. After field visits to update the maps, clusters of 20 dwellings were marked on the map; 116 clusters were randomly selected. If an eligible respondent (15-25 years old who had lived in the borough for at least 1 year) did not reside in the selected dwelling, the dwelling next door was approached until a young person within the selected age range was found. In each eligible dwelling, only one young person was interviewed. In dwellings where more than one eligible person was resident, the respondent was selected randomly by choosing the one whose first name began with a letter closest to the start of the alphabet. There was a 4% refusal rate, and a further 65 questionnaires were not administered because an upsurge in violence in the area threatened the safety of the field staff.

Interviewers were male and female local residents (from outside the study area) and over 18 years of age. The 37 interviewers were trained for 4 days and were monitored by 15 supervisors. Interviews (in Spanish) were conducted in private in the respondent's home in 2002.

Probable cases of common mental disorders (sometimes referred to as mental ill health below) were measured by the Self-Reporting Questionnaire (SRQ-20) recommended by the World Health Organization for assessing the prevalence of depression and anxiety at the community level, where diagnoses of specific illness are not required (World Health Organization, 1994). The instrument, which consists of 20 ‘yes or no’ questions, was administered by an interviewer, as it was anticipated that literacy rates might be low among some young people. Researchers had previously determined a cut-off point of 7/8 for the SRQ-20 in Armero, Colombia (Reference Lima, Pai and SantacruzLima et al, 1991). As this cut-off point was validated against DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), it was used in the current study. Although the validation in Armero was in the context of a disaster, the 7/8 cut-off has also emerged in numerous other studies using the SRQ-20 among low income, urban populations in Latin America (Reference Harpham, Reichenheim and OserHarpham et al, 2003).

A wide range of socio-demographic, social capital and violence variables was also measured. The violence measurement tool was based on the ACTIVA questionnaire applied in many cities in Latin America (Reference OrpinasOrpinas, 1999). Questions cover: norms regarding violence inside and outside the home; the respondent's perpetration of violence against children, within the family and against a partner; being a victim and witness of violence outside the home; the respondent's perpetration of violence against non-household members; and consequences of violence in terms of feelings of insecurity and precautions taken. The social capital instrument drew on various sources, including the Social Capital Assessment Tool (developed by the World Bank, Reference Krishna and SchraderKrishna & Schrader, 1999) and the World Values Survey (http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org). Questions were designed to assess different dimensions of social capital, including: trust in institutions, the wider community and immediate neighbours and friends; perceptions of social cohesion concerning neighbourly unity and mutual respect; perceptions of solidarity involving support and reciprocity; perceptions of informal social control, meaning willingness and capacity to participate in collective action for communal benefit; and perception of civic participation. Social capital was measured at the individual level, not the ecological level. Attitudinal or perception variables were measured by a five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’. Behavioural variables were coded ‘never’, ‘once’ or ‘more than once’ in relation to a specific recall period (for example, ‘in the past year’).

The CAGE instrument was used to test probable alcoholism. Validated in Spain, CAGE consists of four simple binary questions combined to form a scale in which answering positively to more than one item is defined as a ‘high probability of alcoholism’ or a ‘case’. The instrument has high sensitivity (65-100%) and specificity (88-100%) (Reference Martin Centeno and OjanoMartin Centeno & Ojano, 2001). It is named CAGE because of the four items incorporated: cutting down drinking; annoyance at others' concern about drinking; guilt about drinking; and needing an eye-opener in the morning.

Reliability was tested by reapplying questionnaires to 5% of respondents within 2 days, using different interviewers. Spearman's correlation coefficient indicated that a good level of reliability had been achieved; 70% of items had coefficients >0.70 with the following differences between sections of the questionnaire: in the social capital instrument 67% of items had a coefficient >0.70, in the violence instrument 97% of items had a coefficient >0.70, for mental health 70% of items were >0.70 and for CAGE 75% were >0.70.

Factor analysis was used on the violence and social capital variables, respectively. In the social capital factor analysis, group membership was not included, as the dimension consisted of only one item (number of non-violent groups participated in). When a variable was measured by a Likert scale, if 80% or more of the sample fell within the ‘strongly agree/agree’ or ‘strongly disagree/disagree’ categories then the item was excluded owing to non-discriminatory power.

The association of demographic factors, social capital factors and violence factors with common mental disorder (case/non-case) was examined by logistic regressions.

The research also included 16 focus groups of young people from 15 to 25 years interviewed in Spanish in early 2004. Focus groups were divided by age and led by professionals experienced in working with young people. For reasons of sensitivity, discussions did not concentrate on the mental health of the participants but rather on their views of social capital and violence in the family and neighbourhood.

RESULTS

The overall prevalence of common mental disorders was 24% (255/1057). Tables 1, 2 and 3 show the percentage of probable cases and non-cases of common mental disorders in relation to socio-demographic variables and alcohol misuse, social capital and violence variables, respectively. Tables 2 and 3 also indicate which variables (items) went into which factors in the factor analyses of social capital and violence.

Table 1 Probable cases (n=255) and non-cases (n=802) of common mental disorders in relation to socio-demographic variables and alcohol misuse

Socio-demographics/alcohol misuse Non-cases % Cases %
Women 51 77
15-20 years old 69 77
Religious 26 25
Have children 33 41
In no fixed employment 84 91
Not completed 67 77
secondary school
Overcrowded housing1 83 78
Female-headed household 37 40
Family from Cali 56 52
Alcohol misuse 15 21

Table 2 Probable cases (n=255) and non-cases (n=802) of common mental disorders in relation to social capital variables

Dimension Statement Non-cases % who agree with statement1 Cases % who agree with statement1
Trust in institutions2 The church provides a service whenever the community needs it 95 92
The school provides a service whenever the community needs it 84 80
The health centre provides a service whenever the community needs it 77 76
The police provide a service whenever the community needs it 43 37
The borough council provides a service whenever the community needs it 71 73
The neighbourhood council provides a service whenever the community needs it 58 53
Trust in people3 There are neighbours in whom you can confide personal matters 45 45
There are neighbours with whom you can leave the keys of your house 87 87
There are neighbours with whom you can leave a family member 91 88
There are neighbours to whom you can lend things without worrying 67 51
The majority of neighbours don't gossip maliciously about people 20 13
The majority of neighbours would return a lost wallet if they find it 23 23
The majority of neighbours don't take advantage of people 38 33
People trust other neighbours 50 35
Social cohesion4 People know each other 90 87
People get on with each other 59 50
People are close 39 33
People often visit each other 53 55
Solidarity4 There are neighbours who would support you if something unfortunate happened to you 90 90
There are neighbours who would help you financially if you lost your income 48 41
There are neighbours who would give you information about the neighbourhood 87 85
There are neighbours who would tell you about a work opportunity 77 70
There are neighbours who would take care of other neighbours' children 86 81
There are neighbours who would be willinng to help other neighbours 87 81
Social control People would get together to intervene in problems between neighbours 52 58
People would get together to improve the security of the area 72 74
People would get together to demand better health services 80 80
People would get together to improve the image of the neighbourhood 80 80
People would get together to prevent rubbish accumulation 83 81
People would get together to prevent drug use in public places 61 59
People would get together to demand better schools 77 81
People would get together to report corruption within institutions 68 67
Civic participation5 Local residents frequently talk to authorities or local organisations 73 73
People in this area vote in the local elections for mayor 94 93
People in this area actively participate in electroal campaigns 88 83
People participate in marches 72 69
People vote in borough and neighbourhood elections 88 84

Table 3 Probable cases (n=255) and non-cases (n=802) of common mental disorders in relation to violence variables

Dimension Statement Non-cases % who agree with statement1 Cases % who agree with statement1
Norms of violence inside the home1,5 It is necessary to shout at a child 53 61
It is acceptable for an adult to hit a child 50 48
It is acceptable for a man to slap his partner 11 14
It is acceptable for a woman to slap her partner 31 37
If a woman is unfaithful to her partner, he has the right to hit her 21 28
If a man is unfaithful to his partner she has the right to hit him 24 34
Norms of violence outside the home1 A person has the right to kill to defend his/her family 28 31
A person has the right to kill to defend his/her house 24 28
It is acceptable to kill people who are a threat to the community 25 34
If the authorities are weak, people have the right to take the law into their own hands 42 43
Carrying a weapon makes a person safer 28 30
A person has the right to steal to feed his/her family 23 26
Violence against children2,3 Shouted angrily at a child 55 72
Smacked a child 43 55
Hit a child with a belt, cable or other hard object 23 31
Violence within the family2 Shouted angrily at an adult member of the fmaily 41 58
Been angrily shouted at by a family member 66 82
Threatened to hurt an adult member of the family 7 13
Someone in the family has threatened to hurt him/her 11 26
Hit an adult family member 5 13
Someone in the family has hit him/her 17 32
Partner violence1,4 Shouted furiously at a partner 51 67
Been furiously shouted at by a partner 44 63
Victim of violence outside the home2 Been insulted by someone in the area 37 54
Been hit 6 15
Been wounded 4 8
Been robbed 19 19
Someone from the neighbourhood has threatened to hurt him/her or someone in his/her family 18 32
Witness of violence outside the house2 Seen someone being robbed in the neighbourhood 74 80
Seen someone being attacked or hit 67 77
Know someone in the neighbourhood who has been threatened 41 57
Know someone in the neighbourhood who has been wounded 54 71
Know someone in the neighbourhood who has been murdered 60 74
Know someone in the neighbourhood who has been a victim of sexual abuse 16 29
Perpetrator of violence outside the home2 Insulted anyone 41 58
Threatened to harm anyone 9 16
Hit another person 20 32
Used force to make anyone do something which s/he wanted them to do 6 11
Consequences of violence outside the home2 Not been able to enjoy the recreational activities to which s/he is accustomed 93 88
It is safer to move somewhere else 93 88
It is dangerous to go out at night 93 88
The house's security should be improved 96 95
There are more places in the neighbourhood that should be avoided 93 89

Logistic regression

Table 4 presents the results of the logistic regression.

Table 4 Mental health logistic regression models

Model Exp(B) (95% Cl)
1
Constant 0.15***
Gender: female 4.10*** (2.87-5.89)
Education: completed secondary and other advanced studies 0.48*** (0.30-0.75)
Works in informal sector for living 1.58** (1.10-2.26)
Works in formal sector for living 0.69** (0.403-1.17)
Education: some secondary 0.75** (0.50-1.18)
Alcohol misuse 1.57* (1.06-2.32)
2
Constant 0.21***
Gender: female 3.75*** (2.59-5.42)
Works in informal sector for living 1.64** (1.13-2.37)
Education: completed secondary and other advanced studies 0.52** (0.32-0.84)
Poor-quality housing 1.48* (1.05-2.11)
Migrant 1.37* (1.01-1.87)
High ‘thin’ trust1 0.81* (0.66-1.00)
32
Constant 0.01***
Gender: female 4.03*** (2.75-5.89)
Feels consequences of violence 1.51*** (1.21-1.89)
Victim of family violence 1.77*** (1.33-2.36)
Victim of neighbourhood violence 1.81*** (1.36-2.40)
Education: completed secondary and other advanced studies 0.49** (0.30-0.80)
Migrant 1.41* (1.02-1.94)
Works in informal sector for living 1.51* (1.3-2.21)

Model 1

In the first model, only demographic variables and alcohol use were entered. The population categories most at risk of common mental disorders are women, those with low education, those working in the informal sector (i.e. in irregular, insecure, often unskilled employment) and those with alcohol misuse.

Model 2: adding social capital

Social capital factors (see Table 2) and the group membership scale were added into the second model. The criterion for entry (P value) was also changed from 0.05 to 0.06, slightly decreasing the confidence level. Alcoholism dropped out, and coming from outside Cali and living in inadequate housing entered the model as risk factors. People who had high levels of ‘thin trust’ (general trust in people from the neighbourhood, as opposed to ‘thick trust’ which refers to a concentration of trust among a few specific people) were found to be less likely to suffer from mental health problems. Socio-demographic risk factors remained the same as in model 1.

Model 3: adding violence

Model 3 incorporates all the violence factors (see Table 3). Housing quality and thin trust (the only significant social capital factor) dropped out. Being a woman, having a low level of education, earning a living in the informal sector (as opposed to the formal sector), and being a migrant remained as risk factors. Three violence items entered as risk factors: being a victim of neighbourhood violence, experiencing violence in the family and suffering the consequences of violence.

Summary

The models demonstrate that being a woman, having a low level of education and experiencing high levels of violence (both in the home and in the public sphere) are risk factors for common mental disorders. Owing to the prominence of the gender factor, we conducted a separate multivariate analysis, not reported in full here, of the relationship between gender and violence. Gender was not independently associated with witnessing, being a victim of, or suffering the consequences of violence, but men were significantly more likely to perpetrate violence.

DISCUSSION

Comparing the quantitative and qualitative findings

Given the growing number of studies that link poverty with common mental disorders it is perhaps surprising that few of the proxy variables for poverty (except lack of education and working in the informal sector) emerge as significant in the quantitative research. In the qualitative research, poverty emerged as a major cause of tensions in the public and domestic domains. Here one young person describes tensions in the family:

‘There are households that have good relations inside, and there are the majority where there are problems, and honestly in those moments, the distorting factor is money, because they are all living on top of one another, so there is no money, they are not working, so the bills come in and so the mother gets desperate and starts being sarcastic and the kids answer back and this is what leads to the breakdown in household harmony’.

The fact that being a migrant was associated with mental ill-health is in accordance with focus group participants who often attributed the severe strain experienced by migrants to poverty and discrimination (although interestingly family origins or ethnicity did not emerge as a significant risk factor). Other socio-demographic variables, such as age and religion, were not associated with caseness either. Being a woman was the most important demographic risk factor - a finding that matches other studies (see below).

The prevalence of alcohol misuse was 15%, which is high compared with a CAGE study of Colombians of 12-60 years, which found 7% to be probable alcoholics (Centro de Estudies e Información en Salud, 1996). Indeed focus group participants spoke openly and frequently of alcohol problems in their families and among neighbours. However, alcoholism did not emerge as significant in the final model.

In terms of social capital, focus group participants tended to describe relatively low generalised trust among residents of the community, higher trust in immediate neighbours, relatively high solidarity and social cohesion and moderate levels of social control and civic participation. Although they made few specific links with mental health, it is noteworthy that gossip between neighbours was often blamed for causing stress and frustration among young people. For one focus group participant such gossip is based on ‘anger, envy, men and women’ (Commune 14, 19-25 years). Another complains: ‘they are envious of us because we have a house’ (Commune 14, 19-25 years). In the regression, none of the social capital factors was ultimately associated with common mental disorders.

Although having norms that condoned violence was not associated with common mental disorders, the perpetration of violence, whether in the home, against a child or partner, or on the street, was associated with poor mental health. Reports of perpetration of violence, victimisation and witnessing violence inside and outside the home indicate that violence is a part of everyday life for many young people: 25% had hit a child with a hard object in the past month; 20% had been hit by someone in their family during the past year; 23% had hit someone in the past year; and 63% knew someone who had been murdered in the past year. Discussions in focus groups suggest that these figures may even be under-reported, especially in terms of perpetration of violence and violence in the family. There does arise the question of whether there is a cultural norm of boasting about violence. The responses to the questions about norms of violence (which often condemned violence) suggested that this bias was not likely, and interviewers certainly did not perceive any. Being a victim of violence within the family was associated with common mental disorders. During focus groups concentrating on family issues, young people themselves expressed the stresses that family violence imposed on them and their siblings. Similarly, being a victim of violence outside the home was associated with mental ill health. The consequences of violence (feelings of insecurity) were also associated significantly with common mental disorders. Nearly all focus group participants expressed feelings of insecurity.

Putting the results into context - comparisons with other studies

In many developed countries the major burden of disease for young people is mental ill health (Reference Moon, Meyer and GranMoon et al, 1999). In developing countries there are few good-quality data on the burden of mental ill health among young people, relative to other risks such as accidents, homicides or infectious diseases. More population-based surveys of young people (a neglected age group in international health apart from the consideration of the reproductive health of young women) covering both physical and mental health are needed. The 24% prevalence of mental ill health found in our study is high compared with other low-income, mixed-gender, mixed-age urban populations in developing countries (Reference Harpham, Reichenheim and OserHarpham et al, 2003). The poverty and demographic profile of the low-income urban young person in Cali are similar to those of young people in many other Latin American cities (and in some cities from other developing countries acknowledged to have striking similarities to Latin American cities - for example, Johannesburg in South Africa). The violence levels are probably higher than in many cities, but the relationships identified between violence and mental health in our study may be generalisable to other young people living in increasingly violent environments. Studies such as this begin to explore the associations between different factors and the mental health of young people. In terms of socio-demographic variables, this study confirms the common finding that young women (as in Reference Donald, Dower and LuckeDonald et al, 2000), young people with alcohol misuse (Manchester City Council, 2004) and less educated young people have higher levels of mental ill health.

We found that experiencing violence is associated with mental health problems. This confirms the findings of Mazza & Reynolds (Reference Mazza and Reynolds1999) and the Canadian Mental Health Association (2003). Some health researchers have linked this association to the experience of overwhelming shame and humilitation and suggest that vulnerability to violence is influenced by psychological defence mechanisms (Reference GilliganGilligan, 2000). It is also tempting to speculate that the huge economic inequities faced in Latin American cities, especially those with pronounced poverty such as Cali, can lead to anger, frustration and hopelessness, which can prompt both violence and depression/anxiety. This is perhaps particularly true for young people. The mechanisms linking violence and mental ill health are likely to be numerous and have reverse causalities built in. For example, experiencing violence may cause depression which, in turn, may increase long-term difficulties (e.g. cause unemployment or break up with partner). One may speculate that this spiralling down may prompt different norms regarding the acceptability of violent behaviour and thus may, in the long term, lead to violent behaviour itself. This area of the linkages between violence and mental health among young people in conditions of poverty requires more qualitative research to inform potential intervention programmes.

Our findings were less definite concerning the relationship between social capital and mental health in young people. Some research has shown links between social capital and mental health (Reference McKenzie, Whitley and WeichMcKenzie et al, 2002). However, Lomas (Reference Lomas1998), Harpham et al (Reference Harpham, Grant and Thomas2002) and Edmondson (Reference Edmondson2003) concur that although there are hypotheses that social capital may influence mental health, there is a lack of empirical research. We found that in the context of conflict in Cali, violence may have a greater influence than social capital on the mental ill health of young people. In less violent environments, the importance of social capital in influencing mental ill health may be more pronounced.

Action to reduce violence and improve mental health

In contrast to the public health approach, which mainly aims to reduce individual risk factors through behaviour modifications, a new approach to violence reduction is to strengthen social capital (Reference Moser and SchraderMoser & Schrader, 1999). This follows from the multi-level study of Sampson et al (Reference Sampson, Raudenbush and Earls1997) in Chicago, which found that high levels of social cohesion were linked to reduced violence. In terms of policy and practice, can social capital be strengthened to reduce violence and improve the mental health of young people? We are working with youth, community based organisations and the municipal department in Cali to test this theory.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. The burden of mental ill health among low-income urban young people in Cali, Colombia is particularly high for women and those with limited schooling. Interventions could target these groups.

  2. Mental ill health is associated with a wide range of experiences of violence. Interventions to reduce violence may improve mental health.

  3. Mental health is associated with some elements of social capital, which suggests that a social model of intervening to improve mental health is appropriate.

LIMITATIONS

  1. Only one community/geographical area was studied, therefore the relative importance of contextual (community) versus compositional (individual) effects could not be examined (i.e. there was no multi-level modelling).

  2. As the research was not longitudinal it was not possible to identify causal relations. Does depression cause violence or violence cause depression? Reverse causality is a potential problem, i.e. those who are depressed may evaluate their surroundings negatively and have reduced coping skills that may lead to violence.

  3. There is a potential bias due to the fact that individuals who are depressed and/or anxious may tend to perceive things (cohesion, trust and support) in a way that might not reflect real (‘objective’) levels of these features of a community.

Acknowledgements

Research was funded by the United States National Institute of Mental Health and the Office of Behavioural Social Sciences Research (exploratory/development grant no. 1 R21 MH62895-01).

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Canadian Mental Health Association (2003) Violence and Mental Illness. Toronto: CMHA. Available at: http://www.cmha.ca/english/info_centre/mh_pamphlets/mh_pamphlet_vmi.htm.Google Scholar
Centro de Estudios e Informacionen Salud (CEIS) de la Fundación Santafe de Bogotá (1996) Segunda Encuesta Nacional Sobre Consumo de Alcohol, Tabaco y Sustancias Psicoactivas en Colombia. Bogotá: CEIS.Google Scholar
Donald, M., Dower, J., Lucke, J., et al (2000) The Queensland Young Peoples Mental Health Survey Report. Queensland: University of Queensland.Google Scholar
Edmondson, R. (2003) Social capital: a strategy for enhancing health? Social Science and Medicine, 57, 17231733.Google Scholar
Gilligan, I. (2000) Violence in public health and preventative medicine. Lancet, 355, 18021803.Google Scholar
Harpham, T., Grant, E. & Thomas, E. (2002) Measuring social capital within health surveys: key issues. Health Policy and Planning, 17, 106111.Google Scholar
Harpham, T., Reichenheim, M., Oser, R., et al (2003) Measuring mental health in a cost-effective manner. Health Policy and Planning, 18, 344349.Google Scholar
Krishna, A. & Schrader, E. (1999) Social Capital Assessment Tool. Washington, DC: World Bank.Google Scholar
Lima, B., Pai, S., Santacruz, H., et al (1991) Psychiatric disorder among poor victims following a major disaster – Armero, Colombia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 47, 717.Google Scholar
Lomas, J. (1998) Social capital and health: implications for public health and epidemiology. Social Science and Medicine, 47, 11811188.Google Scholar
Manchester City Council (2004) Mental Health. Manchester, UK: MCC. Available at: http://www.manchester.gov.uk/health/jhu/intelligence/reports/mental.htm.Google Scholar
Martin Centeno, A. & Ojano, C. (2001) El medico de atencion primaria en la prevenciony tratamiento de los problemas relacionados con el consumo de alcohol. Medicina General, 32, 233240.Google Scholar
Mazza, J.J. & Reynolds, W. M. (1999) Exposure to violence in younginner-city adolescents: relationships with suicidal ideation, depression, and PTSD symptomatology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 203213.Google Scholar
McKenzie, K., Whitley, R. & Weich, S. (2002) Social capital and mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 280283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, L., Meyer, P. & Gran, J. (1999) Australia's Young People: Their Health and Well-Being. Canberra: Australia Institute of Health and Welfare.Google Scholar
Moser, C. & Schrader, E. (1999) A Conceptual Framework for Violence Reduction. Washington DC: World Bank.Google Scholar
Municipality of Cali (2000) Cali en Cifras. Cali: Municipal Department.Google Scholar
Observatorio Social, Secretaria de Gobierno, Convivencia y Seguridad, AlcaldíAa Santiago de Cali (2003) Informe: Primer Semestre 2003 en la Ciudad de Cali. Santiago de Cali Colombia: Observatorio Social, Secretaria de Gobierno, Convivencia y Seguridad, Alcaldía Santiago de Cali.Google Scholar
Orpinas, P. (1999) Who is violent? Factors associated with aggressive behaviours in Latin America and Spain. Revista Panamericana de la Salud Publica, 5, 232244.Google Scholar
Sampson, R., Raudenbush, S. & Earls, F. (1997) Neighbourhoods and violent crime: a multi-level study ofcollective efficacy. Science, 227, 918924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (1994) User's Guide to the Self-Reporting Questionnaire (SRQ). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Probable cases (n=255) and non-cases (n=802) of common mental disorders in relation to socio-demographic variables and alcohol misuse

Figure 1

Table 2 Probable cases (n=255) and non-cases (n=802) of common mental disorders in relation to social capital variables

Figure 2

Table 3 Probable cases (n=255) and non-cases (n=802) of common mental disorders in relation to violence variables

Figure 3

Table 4 Mental health logistic regression models

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.