Parental mental health and childhood intellectual disability
It is estimated that between 1 in 50 and 1 in 20 families worldwide include at least one child with intellectual disability, defined as significant and persistent impairments in cognitive ability and adaptive function.1 Carer mental health risks are underrecognised, compounding family stress and entailing long-term consequences for well-being of the whole family.2 Mothers and fathers caring for a child with intellectual disability are at risk of psychiatric disorders including depression and anxiety.Reference Olsson and Hwang3–Reference Emerson, McCulloch, Graham, Blacher, Llwellyn and Hatton5 More recent accounts have shifted from a sole focus on adverse effects of childhood disability to a recognition that families have capacity for adaptation and resilience as well as risk, and that family experiences are diverse.Reference McConnell and Savage6 Sociocultural factors, child characteristics and parent characteristics may each contribute to this diversity. Epidemiological studies of families affected by intellectual disability in the UK have consistently highlighted the importance of social context for parental experience. Lone-parent households and families facing financial challenges are especially likely to include a child with intellectual disability, and socioeconomic indices can account for 50% of the elevated risk of poor self-esteem and self-efficacy in mothers.Reference Emerson, Hatton, Llewellyn, Blacher and Graham7 Similarly, matching for socioeconomic variables in the Millennium Cohort Study attenuated (but did not abolish) elevated psychiatric risk in fathers and mothers of young children with cognitive delay.Reference Emerson, McCulloch, Graham, Blacher, Llwellyn and Hatton5 Parental physical health and psychological resources may also be important determinants of risk and resilience.Reference Emerson, Hatton, Llewellyn, Blacher and Graham7,Reference Olsson and Hwang8 Prior level of depression is a key predictor of later depression within parents of children with intellectual disability;Reference Gallagher, Pilch and Hannigan9 conversely, positive parental characteristics, such as dispositional optimism, locus of control and sense of coherence, may help maintain well-being.Reference Baker, Blacher and Olsson10,Reference Oelofsen and Richardson11 Other studies highlight the heterogeneity of childhood neurodevelopmental phenotypes: salient features relevant to parental well-being include age and developmental stage, with adolescence identified as a particularly risky period;Reference Bolourian and Blacher12 presence of physical disability and illness; autism spectrum characteristics and affective disturbance or conduct problems.Reference Cohen, Zeedyk, Tipton, Rodas and Blacher13 Indeed, longitudinal studies have shown bidirectional relationships between child behaviour and parenting stress, which may be a magnification of transactional dynamics observed for children without intellectual disability.Reference Woodman, Mawdsley and Hauser-Cram14
The contribution of intellectual disability aetiology to family risks
In high-income countries, it is estimated that at least 60% of severe intellectual disability is associated with specific, identifiable genetic variants, encompassing large chromosomal abnormalities (i.e., aneuploidy, translocations), submicroscopic deletions and duplications (i.e. copy number variants; CNVs) and DNA sequence differences (i.e. single nucleotide variants; SNVs).Reference Gilissen, Hehir-Kwa, Thung, van de Vorst, van Bon and Willemsen15 The implementation of genome-wide technologies (chromosomal microarrays, whole-exome or whole-genome sequencing) is leading to a rapid increase in the proportion of children with a developmental disorder receiving a genetic diagnosis, i.e. identification of a genetic variant that either causes or contributes to intellectual disability.Reference Wright, FitzPatrick and Firth16 This provides new opportunities to investigate the heterogeneity of intellectual disability at all levels, from molecular mechanisms to social dynamics. One relatively unexplored question concerns the potential influence of intellectual disability aetiology on family outcomes and parental well-being.Reference Hodapp and Dykens17 Early studies reported better well-being in mothers of children with Down syndrome compared with mothers of children with autism or idiopathic developmental delay, although group differences attenuated over time and when contrasts in social and communicative abilities were taken into account.Reference Griffith, Hastings, Nash and Hill18 In a study of 150 children with fragile X syndrome and their families, behavioural problems of children with, and siblings without fragile X syndrome both predicted maternal distress, suggesting that child behaviour exerts a powerful influence within these families.Reference Hall, Burns and Reiss19 In a recent study, Cornelia de Lange syndrome, Smith Magenis syndrome and 1p36 deletion syndrome were associated with elevated risk of maternal depression, irrespective of the age or ability level of diagnosed children, whereas these background factors were the major determinants of maternal mental health for ten other genetic conditions.Reference Adams, Clarke, Griffith, Howlin, Moss and Petty20 Potential explanations for these associations include physiological or behavioural phenotypes (e.g. sleep disturbance, volatility or anxiety), or societal factors (e.g. variation in understanding and acceptance of different diagnoses). Turning to ‘new’ genetic diagnoses, CNVs in particular may be relevant to understanding family risks. A high proportion of these intellectual disability–associated variants are inherited from parents,Reference Girirajan, Rosenfeld, Coe, Parikh, Friedman and Goldstein21 who may or may not have neurodevelopmental difficulties themselves. CNVs are associated with lifespan mental health difficulties, thus potentially amplifying risks in both the parent and child.Reference Chawner, Owen, Holmans, Raymond, Skuse and Hall22 Rather than directly affecting parental well-being, genetic diagnosis is likely to act indirectly on parental well-being via its influence on children's functional outcomes and families’ experiences.
Aims
This study appraised the importance of genetic diagnoses, broadly categorised, alongside other risk factors for carer mental health in the context of childhood intellectual disability. Building on the results of previous studies (notably the study by Emerson;Reference Emerson4 see Supplementary Table 1), we assessed the influence of child characteristics, social factors and parental psychological appraisal (impact), and explored the direct and indirect effects of genetic diagnosis.
Method
Recruitment
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures were approved by London–Queen Square Research Ethics Committee (14/LO/1069).
Participants were recruited to the Intellectual Disability and Mental Health: Assessing the Genomic Impact on Neurodevelopment (IMAGINE-ID) study between 2014 and 2018. Recruitment to the study was by referral from any of the 22 UK regional genetics centres (75.5%), patient support groups (11.2%), other research projects (3.6%) and self-referrals via social media and online advertising (9.7%). At the time of recruitment to the study most families (92%) resided in England, with 4.3% residing in Scotland, 3.2% in Wales and 0.1% in Northern Ireland. The entry criteria for referral of an index child to the study were as follows: clinically documented developmental delay or intellectual disability, a molecular genetic diagnosis documented from an accredited diagnostic laboratory and at least 3 years of age at recruitment. Children meeting these entry criteria were identified retrospectively from regional genetics centres and research databases and prospectively in out-patient clinics. Parents or guardians were sent an invitation letter, enabling them to contact the study, provide consent and participate. Written informed consent was obtained from parents or guardians of all child participants aged <16 years, and an assent form was completed by the child where possible. Data presented in this paper were collected from a parent or carer of the index child via online data entry (78%), online with telephone support (5.2%), telephone interview (14.9%) or face-to-face interview (1.8%), according to parent/carer choice.
Diagnostic genetic reports were obtained from medical records or directly from families. Variants were broadly categorised as per Table 1, which describes frequency of genetic diagnosis types and variant inheritance for the sample. Where multiple pathogenic or likely pathogenic variants had been reported for a participant, categorisation was based on the variant with highest pathogenicity rating according to clinical laboratory report.
CNV, copy number variant; SNV, single nucleotide variant; DN, de novo; I, inherited; U, unknown; DQ, developmental quotient; Physical, physical disability score; Impact, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire impact score; Life events, number of negative life events in past 12 months; IMD, Index of Multiple Deprivation; EFQ, primary caregiver respondent score on the Everyday Feelings Questionnaire.
Assessments of parental well-being and impact
The primary respondent completed the ten-item Everyday Feeling QuestionnaireReference Uher and Goodman23 (EFQ) for themselves and for their partner. Items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (none of the time) to 4 (all of the time). Items were summed together to create a total score. Higher scores indicated higher levels of psychological distress (Cronbach α = 0.89). The respondent also completed the Impact Supplement of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ),Reference Goodman24 in which they rated the extent to which their child's difficulties caused distress, interfered with their home, school and social lives and were burdensome to the family. Items were summed together to create an impact score, with higher scores indicating a greater level of impact.
Assessments of child characteristics
The primary respondent completed the Development and Well-being Assessment (DAWBA)Reference Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward and Meltzer25 structured assessment. This measure has been used in large-scale studies of mental health among children with and without intellectual disability,Reference Emerson and Hatton26,Reference Ford, Goodman and Meltzer27 and its validity when administered online or by telephone has been established.Reference McEwen, Stewart, Colvert, Woodhouse, Curran and Gillan28 Developmental quotient was calculated from parental estimation of current mental age divided by chronological age (in lieu of standardised assessments, which were not feasible within the study protocol). The primary respondent provided information within the DAWBA about the presence or absence of physical disabilities in the target child, including difficulties with toileting, speech, vision, hearing, movement and seizures.Reference Ford, Goodman and Meltzer27 The number of disabilities was summed together to create a total score, with higher scores indicating a greater level of physical disability (mean 2.41, s.d. 1.87, ordinal α = 0.79). Children received three scores based on the DAWBA algorithms generating 70% likelihoods for 12 different psychiatric diagnoses: a binary score indicating whether or not the child had a likely diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD); a binary score indicating whether or not the child likely had a conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder (CD/ODD) and a binary score indicating whether or not the child likely had any other diagnosis.
Assessments of social circumstances
Children's postcodes were used to obtain the Index of Multiple Deprivation, which provides a measure of relative neighbourhood deprivation based on income, employment, education, health, crime, housing and living environment.Reference Smith29 Scores range from 1 (the most deprived decile) to 10 (the least deprived decile in England). The primary respondent completed a Recent Negative Life Events checklist for events that had occurred in the past 12 months, e.g., separation owing to marital difficulties.Reference Brugha, Bebbington, Tennant and Hurry30 The number of events was summed together to create a total ranging from 0 to 7 (ordinal α = 0.70); 49% of families had experienced 1 or more negative life events in the past 12 months. The respondent also provided information about whether they were solo parents, the number of children living at home and the number of adults living at home (UK Office for National Statistics definitions31).
Analysis
The primary analyses were conducted using structural equation modelling in Mplus version 8 for Mac OS (www.statmodel.com).Reference Muthén and Muthén32 We used a maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors in each of our models to account for the non-normal distribution of our indicators.Reference Kline33 We evaluated model fit using three primary criteria: comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) > 0.90, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.08.Reference Brown34 Table 1 shows the extent of missing data on each questionnaire. We used a full information approach under the assumption that data were missing at random so that all families were included. Model parameters and standard errors were estimated in Mplus, using all available data.
Results
Sample characteristics
The initial sample comprised 990 children and their parents. After siblings (n = 41), children with a developmental quotient of > 85 (n = 32) and children with no known genetic diagnosis (n = 29) were removed, the final sample comprised 888 children (505 males), aged 4–15 years (mean age 8.16 years, s.d. 2.89) with a mean developmental quotient of 55.35 (s.d. 21.36). Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for each of the study measures for the whole sample and by genetic diagnosis. There was a broad representation of socioeconomic circumstances within the cohort, although the distribution was shifted toward affluence relative to expectation for childhood intellectual disability (Supplementary Table 2). Of those respondents (n = 797) providing information about current parental relationship status, 135 (16.9%) were solo parents. The majority of respondents had families with one or two children (n = 591, 74.2%), and one-quarter (25.8%) had three or more children (n = 206). A fifth (19.6%) of the sample met the 70% likelihood criteria for ASD. A fifth (21.5%) of the sample met the 70% likelihood criteria for conduct disorder. Supplementary Table 3 shows the correlations between all study measures.
Caregiver distress: comparison between IMAGINE-ID and UK general population parents
We compared psychological distress (EFQ scores) in the IMAGINE-ID primary respondents and their partners with primary respondent parents and their partners in the general population.Reference Uher and Goodman23 The comparison data were collected from 5279 mothers and other caregivers of 8- to 19-year-old children as part of the 2007 follow-up of the 2004 UK Office for National Statistics survey of the mental health of children and young people in Great Britain.35 Summary statistics for analysis were provided by the first author ofReference Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward and Meltzer25 as a personal communication. The IMAGINE-ID primary respondents had higher levels of distress (mean 16.258, s.d. 7.39, 95% CI 16.09–17.07) than primary respondent parents from the general population (N = 5245, mean 12.545, s.d. 6.87, 95% CI 12.45–12.64, t(881) = 16.205, Cohen's d = 0.546). Likewise, partners were reported to have higher levels of distress (mean 14.18, s.d. 7.06, 95% CI 13.65–14.71) than partners in the general population (N = 3889, mean 10.894, s.d. 6.54, 95% CI 10.70–11.09, t(689) = 12.222, Cohen's d = 0.465). Where identity of the primary respondent was provided, levels of distress were higher in mothers (mean 17.01, s.d. 7.493, N = 543) than in fathers (mean 14.13, s.d. 6.71, N = 48, t(589) = 2.58, mean difference = 2.89, s.e. 1.119, 95% CI 0.69–5.09, Cohen's d = 0.405).
Model 1: predictors of caregiver distress (whole cohort)
We regressed caregiver distress (measured by the EFQ) onto child characteristics: age in years, gender (0 = male, 1 = female), developmental quotient, ASD (0 = no, 1 = yes), CD/ODD (0 = no, 1 = yes), other psychiatric disorder (0 = no, 1 = yes), physical disability, SDQ social impact score and four dummy variables representing genetic diagnosis (with CNV as the reference group). The dependent variable was also regressed onto five social circumstances variables: total negative life events in the past 12 months, socioeconomic status measured using the index of multiple deprivation, number of children in the household (0 = 1–2 children, 1 = 3 or more), lone parent status (0 = no, 1 = yes) and number of adults in the household (0 = 1–2 adults, 1 = 3 or more). We also regressed the social impact scores from the SDQ onto each of the child predictors and five social circumstances variables. All other predictor variables were permitted to correlate. Table 2 shows the direct and indirect effects on each of the dependent variables in model 1. Figure 1 shows a simplified path diagram for model 1.
Est.: robust maximum likelihood parameter estimates; DAWBA, development and well-being assessment; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; CD/ODD, conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder; Genetic, dummy variables with CNV group as reference group; CNV, copy number variant; SNV, single nucleotide variant; IMD, Index of Multiple Deprivation.
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
The model provided a good fit to the data (χ2(67) = 104.517, P = 0.002, CFI = 0.960, TLI = 0.941, RMSEA = 0.025, 90% CI 0.015–0.034), and accounted for 27.1% of the variance in SDQ social impact scores and 15.3% of the variance in caregiver distress. Older age, the presence of physical disability, ASD, CD/ODD and other psychiatric diagnoses on the DAWBA were uniquely associated with higher levels of perceived impact on children's home, school and social life. Children with SNV diagnosis had lower impact scores than children with CNV diagnosis. Children with other chromosomal diagnoses also had lower impact scores than children with CNV diagnosis. No other genetic contrast was significant. High impact score was associated with high parental distress. Negative life events and the presence of CD/ODD was also associated with high levels of distress in the primary caregiver.
We then examined the indirect effects between genetic diagnosis and caregiver distress via impact scores. There was an indirect effect of SNV diagnosis (relative to CNV) on caregiver distress via impact. Likewise, there was an indirect effect of chromosomal abnormalities (relative to CNV) on caregiver distress via impact. Specifically, relative to having CNV, having SNV or chromosomal diagnoses was linked with lower levels of perceived impact and, in turn, lower levels of caregiver distress.
Model 2: predictors of caregiver distress (CNV diagnoses only)
In our second model we examined the relations between CNV characteristics and caregiver distress in those families of children with CNV diagnoses only (n = 541). This model was identical to the previous model but included two dummy variables representing inheritance patterns (i.e. de novo (n = 159) versus familial inheritance (n = 162); and de novo versus unknown inheritance (n = 212)), two dummy variables indicating the type of CNV (i.e. deletion (n = 338) versus duplication (n = 183); and deletion versus other (n = 20)) and a continuous variable indicating CNV size (mean 2.396×106 base pairs, s.d. 3.446×106). The model provided an acceptable fit to the data (χ2(74) = 91.585, P = 0.0810, CFI = 0.957, TLI = 0.939, RMSEA = 0.022, 95% CI 0–0.036) and accounted for 24.7% of the variance in SDQ impact scores and 13.8% of the variance in caregiver distress. The main results are displayed in Table 3. Of note, there were no direct or indirect effects of any of the CNV characteristics on psychological distress in the primary caregiver or perceived impact.
Est., robust maximum likelihood parameter estimates; DAWBA, development and well-being assessment; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; CD/ODD, conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder; CNV, copy number variant; IMD, Index of Multiple Deprivation.
*P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001.
Discussion
This study represents the largest investigation to date of the well-being of caregivers in the context of childhood intellectual disability, and the first to include evaluation of genetic diagnosis as a potential predictor of risk. In keeping with previous studies, there is a broad distribution of parental emotional well-being, but this distribution is very significantly shifted toward elevated risk in parents caring for a child with intellectual disability. Our results also replicate Emerson'sReference Emerson4 finding that the social and psychological impact of the child's disability predict parental well-being. Moreover, across both studies, four aspects of a child's intellectual disability–associated phenotypes (i.e. ASD, CD/ODD, additional psychopathology, physical disability), rather than the severity of developmental delay predicted impact scores. The current study identifies genetic diagnosis as an additional predictor of parental well-being, mediated by the perceived impact of a child's difficulties on home, school and social life.
Impact or symptom severity?
Impact plays an important role within clinical decision making (e.g. in the DSM-5), but there remains debate regarding its measurement and interpretation. One perspective is that behavioural and emotional symptoms matter regardless of impact, just as physical symptoms require attention to enable efficient diagnosis and treatment.Reference Rutter36 Challenging this view, however, is evidence that even when symptom scores are taken into consideration, impact scores predict key outcomes such as contact with psychiatric services.Reference Stringaris and Goodman37 The SDQ impact scale amalgamates three components: distress and impaired function for the child, and perceived family burden, each of which may have different significance for parental well-being. Adding further complexity to the picture, impact scores differ in salience across psychiatric symptoms: some symptoms (e.g. a fear of heights) are, in most circumstances, completely manageable; in contrast, other symptoms (e.g. low mood, poor sleep) are themselves markers of impact, such that a global impact score for an individual with a variety of different symptoms can be difficult to interpret. Our finding that impact scores predicted parental well-being echoes findings related to cognitive impairment at the other end of the lifespan: well-being in carers of individuals with dementia is often more closely related to the behavioural or emotional impact of dementia than to the loss of cognitive function per se.Reference Waite, Bebbington, Skelton-Robinson and Orrell38 Together, these findings suggest that changes in impact scores, although complex, are a potentially valuable marker of intervention success, providing an index of the extent to which initiatives aimed at the affected individual have positive ripple effects on family members. Further studies are required to examine how parents' appraisals or coping strategies intervene between child characteristics and perceived impact, and the extent to which perceived impact could itself be a marker of parental well-being or an intervention target.
Parental well-being is influenced by genetic diagnosis of their child's intellectual disability
The genetic diagnosis underlying childhood intellectual disability made a significant direct contribution to impact, with parents of children with CNV-associated intellectual disability reporting higher perceived impact, indirectly influencing caregiver distress. Neither variant inheritance nor postcode-level socioeconomic factors explained the contrast in well-being between CNV and other diagnostic groups. One plausible explanation for the influence of genetic diagnosis on parental distress could be the elevated proportion of the CNV group (versus the remainder of this sample) who received a DAWBA likelihood diagnosis of either oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder. Strengthening the view that psychiatric phenotypes may contribute to impact, detailed face-to-face characterisation of a subset of individuals from the IMAGINE-ID cohort with specific recurrent CNV diagnosesReference Chawner, Owen, Holmans, Raymond, Skuse and Hall22 found extremely high levels of psychopathology and comorbidity. Hence, persistent complex emotional–behavioural difficulties for the CNV group may exert an especially negative long-term effect on parents and the wider family. However, in the current analysis, the effects of CNV diagnosis on impact and distress were found to be independent of phenotypic variations across the sample, suggesting that the association between CNV diagnosis and impact may reflect not the direct consequences of aetiology on phenotype, but rather between-group differences in the diagnostic journey of children and families in this cohort. Beyond diagnosis, the extent of support available to children and families may differ between groups: the SNV group on average have higher physical disability scores and more severe intellectual disability, which may result in a more comprehensive and sustained package of educational, medical and social support, with a consequential influence on parental appraisal of impact. Here, the literature on violations of parental expectations with regards to the impact of premature birthReference Macey, Harmon and Easterbrooks39 may provide a useful analogy. That is, if the CNV group is often characterized by mild-to-moderate intellectual disability coupled with prominent behavioural problems, parents in this group are likely to face both greater violations of expectations and reduced tolerance from the community, leading to greater problems of uncertainty and social isolation. Further work both within IMAGINE-ID and other cohorts is needed to tease apart these alternative proposals.
Limitations
This study did not include children with intellectual disability of unknown origin, or intellectual disability of acquired origin; these comparison groups would provide a broader context for understanding the role of aetiology in parental experience. The study applied extremely broad categorisation of genetic diagnoses, insensitive to the distinct spatiotemporal expression patterns, molecular mechanisms, biological pathways, and polygenic and environmental interactions of each individual's rare disorder. Several potential sources of ascertainment bias deserve mention. First, the number of eligible families who received information about the study but did not consent or participate is not known, hence we cannot estimate a response rate or assess factors influencing participation. Second, participants from deprived areas were underrepresented in the cohort. This may be explained by socioeconomic status (SES)-related-related differences in access to genetic testing, or in the likelihood of volunteering for this research study and completing data entry. A further possibility is that SES predicts the likelihood of a genetic diagnosis underlying intellectual disability: deprivation shifts the balance between mild and severe intellectual disabilityReference Emerson40 and may thus be associated with higher likelihood of multifactorial or acquired causes. Nevertheless, the sample was sufficiently diverse to test for association between parental well-being and neighbourhood characteristics and, echoing results from previous studies,Reference Emerson4,Reference Emerson, Hatton, Llewellyn, Blacher and Graham7 we did not find any such association. That is, we found no evidence that SES confounds the effect of genetic diagnosis on parental impact and well-being.
Several limitations in data collection also deserve note. Between-study differences in the definition and measurement of distress among parents caring for children with intellectual disability precluded comparison of effect sizes across studies. Online data collection precluded psychometric assessment to objectively confirm the presence of intellectual disability and its severity in the diagnosed child. A total of 22% of respondents opted to complete assessments with telephone support or via face-to-face interview, which may influence self-report via social desirability effects.Reference Zhang, Kuchinke, Woud, Velten and Margraf41 The study did not measure household-level poverty, in particular household income, material hardship and receipt of welfare benefits, which limited our ability to investigate socioeconomic contributors to parental well-being. In addition, we had no measure of parental physical health, an important predictor of mental health in previous studies,Reference Emerson, Hatton, Llewellyn, Blacher and Graham7,Reference Olsson and Hwang8 and potentially of heightened significance for parents with inherited genomic variants. Further, the limited number of fathers involved as primary respondents in our study made it difficult to interpret the observed informant effects, namely a weaker contrast in well-being between IMAGINE-ID parents and general population when the primary respondent was a father. Note also that reliance on proxy ratings for partners limited the independence of measures, and ability to investigate convergence or divergence between parents. For these reasons, future studies should seek to gather information directly from more than one parent/carer in each family. The current study design is cross-sectional, and future longitudinal work is needed to clarify the relations between impact and distress, establish a causal pathway integrating genetic, developmental and family factors, and identify modifiable factors that could be effective intervention targets.
In summary, this study confirms the high risk of emotional distress associated with caring for a child with intellectual disability. Our findings are consistent with previous studies showing that children's physical and behavioural characteristics and parental appraisal of impact contribute to variation in parents' well-being. A novel contribution is the finding that genetic diagnosis type exerts an additional influence. Further work is needed to elucidate the mechanisms underlying this association; these are likely to be complex, reflecting not only intellectual disability phenotype, but also the evolving narrative of parental experiences both within and outside the family. Given rapid increases in access to genetic testing and diagnostic yields, understanding how genetic diagnoses may be associated with parental mental health is clinically important. Future research should address the trajectories of parental experience before, at and after receiving a genetic diagnosis for their child, to determine optimal timings of family risk assessment and interventions to promote long-term well-being of individuals and families.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.38.
Funding
This work was supported by the UK Medical Research Council (grant number G101400 to K.B.), UK Medical Research Council and Medical Research Foundation (grant number MR-N022572-1 to the IMAGINE-ID study; Principle Investigators: David H. Skuse, F Lucy Raymond, Jeremy Hall, Marianne Van den Bree, Michael J. Hall) and the Baily Thomas Charitable Trust (to K.B.).
Acknowledgements
We thank the IMAGINE-ID study families, as well as all IMAGINE-ID research scientists, study and data managers, and clinical and administrative staff who make the study possible. Data entry of household structure variables was carried out by Annabel Holmes and Sophie Frear.
Data availability
Authors' access to the study data was permitted via a data access agreement. The data associated with the manuscript is not currently publicly available.
Author contributions
K.B., R.T.D. and C.H. formulated the research questions, designed and carried out the analysis and wrote the article. The IMAGINE-ID consortium designed the wider study, recruited participants, and collected and processed the data. E.N.-C. and F.L.R. reviewed and amended the manuscript.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.