Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-bmd9x Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-29T21:11:08.818Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Biological research on mental pain, social pain and other pains not primarily felt in the body: methodological systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2025

Etienne K. Duranté
Affiliation:
Université de Paris Cité and Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Inserm, INRAE, Center for Research in Epidemiology and StatisticS (CRESS), Paris, France Centre d′Épidémiologie Clinique, AP-HP, Hôpital Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France
Alexandre Ribeiro
Affiliation:
Université de Paris Cité and Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Inserm, INRAE, Center for Research in Epidemiology and StatisticS (CRESS), Paris, France Centre d′Épidémiologie Clinique, AP-HP, Hôpital Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France
Lucie Gaspard-Boulinc
Affiliation:
Institut Curie, PSL Research University, Paris, France
Isabelle Boutron
Affiliation:
Université de Paris Cité and Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Inserm, INRAE, Center for Research in Epidemiology and StatisticS (CRESS), Paris, France Centre d′Épidémiologie Clinique, AP-HP, Hôpital Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France
Chantal Henry
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire, GHU Paris Psychiatrie & Neurosciences, Paris, France Université Paris-Cité, Paris, France
Anne-Cecile Petit
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire, GHU Paris Psychiatrie & Neurosciences, Paris, France Université Paris-Cité, Paris, France Institut Pasteur, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
Josselin Houenou
Affiliation:
NeuroSpin, CEA, Université Paris-Saclay, Gif-sur Yvette, France APHP, CHU Mondor, DMU IMPACT, INSERM U955 Team ‘Neuropsychiatrie Translationnelle’, IMRB, Université Paris Est Créteil, Créteil, France
Cedric Lemogne
Affiliation:
Université de Paris Cité and Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Inserm, INRAE, Center for Research in Epidemiology and StatisticS (CRESS), Paris, France Department of Psychiatry, AP-HP, Hôpital Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France
Astrid Chevance*
Affiliation:
Université de Paris Cité and Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Inserm, INRAE, Center for Research in Epidemiology and StatisticS (CRESS), Paris, France Centre d′Épidémiologie Clinique, AP-HP, Hôpital Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France
*
Correspondence: Astrid Chevance. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Researchers explore the biology of painful experiences not primarily felt in the body (‘non-physical pain’), sometimes referred to as mental, social or emotional pain. A critical challenge lies in how to operationalise this subjective experience for biological research, a crucial process for translating findings into clinical practice.

Aims

To map studies investigating biological features of non-physical pain, focusing on their conceptual features (i.e. terms and definitions of non-physical pain) and methodological characteristics (e.g. experimental paradigms and measures).

Method

This methodological systematic review searched reports of primary research on the biological features of non-physical pain across Embase, MEDLINE and Web of Science. Using a meta-research approach, we synthetised results on terms, definitions, populations, experimental paradigms, confounders, measures of non-physical pain and investigation methods (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging).

Results

We identified 92 human studies, involving 7778 participants. Overall, 59.1% of the studies did not report any definition of non-physical pain, and 82% of studies did not use a specific measure. Regarding the possibility of translating results to clinical settings, most of the human studies involved only healthy participants (71.7%) and the seven different experimental paradigms used to induce non-physical pain had unknown external validity. Confounders were not considered by 32.4% of the experimental studies. Animal studies were rare, with only four rodent studies.

Conclusions

Biomedical studies of non-physical pain use heterogeneous concepts with unclear overlaps and methods with unknown external validity. As has been done for physical pain, priority actions include establishing an agreed definition and measurement of non-physical pain and developing experimental paradigms with good external validity.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

A range of different terms are used by researchers to refer to experiences described as painful yet without being primarily felt in the body, such as ‘mental pain’ Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1 , ‘social pain’ Reference Eisenberger2,Reference Eisenberger and Lieberman3 , ‘emotional pain’ Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1 , ‘psychological pain’ Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1,Reference Meerwijk and Weiss4,Reference Conejero, Olié, Calati, Ducasse and Courtet5  and ‘psychache’ Reference Shneidman6 ., For instance, Shneidman, after reading hundreds of suicide letters, defined ‘psychache’ as a pain ‘felt in the mind’ caused by deprived needs; Meerwijk and Weiss defined ‘psychological pain’ as a ‘lasting unsustainable feeling’ and developed a self-reported measurement instrument in the form of a questionnaire. In the field of neuroscience, Eisenberger et al forged in 2003 the term ‘social pain, further defined in 2004 as describing ‘the pain experienced upon social injury when social relationships are threatened, damaged or lost’. Reference Eisenberger and Lieberman3,Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Williams7 Although physical pain might be associated with such experiences, it is secondary to the painful experience that is primarily felt in the mind. Hence, these pains are distinct from psychogenic pain, which is a physical pain (primarily felt in the body) caused by psychological events. In this body of research, the adjectives ‘mental’, ‘psychological’, ‘emotional’, ‘social’, etc. associated with the word ‘pain’ characterise both phenomenal aspects (i.e. where it is painful) and aetiopathogenesis (i.e. what caused the pain). For clarity and brevity in the present work, we will subsume all these different terms used by researchers to refer to pain not primarily felt in the body (e.g. mental pain, psychological pain, psychache, social pain) with the portmanteau word ‘non-physical pain’.

Historical background

In psychiatry, notions of pain not primarily felt in the body trace back to the 19th century in Europe. Reference Kendler8 Although some psychiatrists of that time defined ‘mental pain’ as a core symptom of melancholia (e.g. Maudsley, Griesinger, Séglas), others identified it as a common symptom in mental disorders allowing separation of normal from pathological experiences (e.g. Guislain). Reference Kendler8 This has a posterity in the 20th century, in particular in the description of melancholia in Ey et al’s classic textbook of psychiatry, first published in 1960. Reference Ey, Bernard, Brisset, Ey, Bernard and Brisset9 More recently, clinical research on non-physical pain has focused on the link with suicide. Reference Conejero, Olié, Calati, Ducasse and Courtet5,Reference Ducasse, Holden, Boyer, Artéro, Calati and Guillaume10,Reference Verrocchio, Carrozzino, Marchetti, Andreasson, Fulcheri and Bech11 Following Shneidman’s work, different measurement scales for non-physical pain have been developed for the purpose of clinical research in psychiatry and suicidology and have led to two main results: non-physical pain might be a symptom associated with several mental disorders (e.g. depressive disorders, borderline personality disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, schizophrenia) and it can be a predictor of suicide. Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1,Reference Ducasse, Holden, Boyer, Artéro, Calati and Guillaume10,Reference Zanarini and Frankenburg12Reference Ducasse, Courtet and Olié16

One might argue that in the terms ‘mental pain’, ‘psychological pain’, etc., pain is to be understood metaphorically. However, researchers have investigated whether they share biological features with physical pain. Reference Eisenberger2,Reference Eisenberger and Lieberman3,Reference Ducasse, Courtet and Olié16Reference Wager, Atlas, Lindquist, Roy, Woo and Kross19 Conducting research on the biology of non-physical pain raises many challenges, such as conceptualising a subjective experience for the purpose of experimental research, developing models of non-physical pain in humans – and non-humans – and the question of how to measure this pain. Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1,Reference Gerber, Chang and Reimel20,Reference Corns21 A critical challenge lies in how to effectively operationalise this deeply subjective human experience for biological research, a process crucial for translating findings into clinical practice.

The current study

This study adopts an epistemological approach to describe what concepts and methods have been used in contemporary biological research to investigate non-physical pain. Hence, the objective of this study is to comprehensively map studies investigating biological features of non-physical pain, focusing on their conceptual features (i.e. terms and definitions) and methodological characteristics (e.g. experimental paradigms, measures, controls). Research mappings are methodological overviews that allow for an assessment of a research field by identifying key concepts, methods, studied populations, etc. Reference Campbell, Tricco, Munn, Pollock, Saran and Sutton22Reference Cragg, Kramer, Borisoff, Patrick and Ramer26 They have been shown to help researchers understand the landscape of existing research, identify gaps and highlight trends or patterns in a particular area. In doing so, they provide material to think about how research is conducted and propose ways forward. Methodological appraisal of biological investigations of non-physical pain is scarce and non-systematic. Reference Rotge, Lemogne, Hinfray, Huguet, Grynszpan and Tartour17,Reference Mwilambwe-Tshilobo and Spreng27Reference Vanhollebeke, Aers, Goethals, Raedt, Baeken and Mierlo29 Hence, inquiring about the concepts and methods mobilised by contemporary researchers investigating the biological features of non-physical pain might have critical influences on the broader understanding of pain, suffering and the potential development of treatment. At this point, we acknowledge that ‘non-physical pain’ is neither a concept nor a construct but a practical category referring to pain not primarily felt in the body. Investigating whether non-physical pain is a real phenomenon or naturalistic entity is beyond the scope of this research mapping study.

Method

We conducted a methodological systematic review of published original studies exploring the biological research of non-physical pain, compliant with the PRISMA guidelines. Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann and Mulrow30 The protocol of the study is available on Zenodo (https://zenodo.org/doi/10.5281/zenodo.6984681).

Eligibility criteria

We included published reports of primary research studies that investigated at least one biological feature (hereafter, biomarker) hypothetically associated with non-physical pain, the subjective experience of pain not primarily felt in the body. In the context of our study, we defined a biomarker of non-physical pain as any ‘defined characteristic that is measured as an indicator of normal biological processes, pathogenic processes, or biological responses to an exposure’ at any level (genetic, molecular, cellular, neural circuits, and physiology or pathophysiology) for which the authors of the study hypothesised an involvement in non-physical pain. 31

We excluded conference talks, posters, dissertations, textbook chapters and all secondary research (i.e. evidence synthesis such as narrative review, systematic review, meta-analysis). We excluded studies investigating close concepts of non-physical pain yet not explicitly mentioning a reference to pain in their definition (e.g. stress, negative emotions). If a published report included several studies, we applied the eligibility criteria at the study level, and not only at the report level. For instance, DeWall et al Reference DeWall, MacDonald, Webster, Masten, Baumeister and Powell32 included two different studies, only one of which met our eligibility criteria.

In articles reporting systematic secondary research (i.e. systematic evidence synthesis such as systematic review, meta-analysis) identified through our search strategy, we screened their reference lists in search of additional primary research that might have been overlooked by our search strategy.

Information sources and search strategy

In compliance with Cochrane guidelines, we contacted an information specialist of the Medical Library of Paris Cité University, who checked the relevance of the databases for the topic, the list of keywords and the search queries for each database. Reference Lefebre, Glanville, Briscoe, Featherstone, Littlewood, Marshall, Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li and Page33

We searched three databases, which are the most relevant for biology-related publications – MEDLINE, Embase and Web of Science – from their date of inception to 6 November 2023.

Based on our previous systematic review of clinical studies on non-physical pain, we anticipated heterogeneity in the terms and definitions of non-physical pain. Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1 We therefore developed a pilot iterative search strategy based on the inclusion of different synonyms, using a bibliographic visualising tool showing the overlap between keywords, and comparing different combinations of keywords. Reference Lefebre, Glanville, Briscoe, Featherstone, Littlewood, Marshall, Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li and Page33Reference Affengruber, van der Maten, Spiero, Nussbaumer-Streit, Mahmić-Kaknjo and Ellen36 Starting from the keywords used in our previous systematic review, which used strict synonyms ‘mental pain’, ‘psychological pain’, ‘psychache’ and ‘psychic pain’, Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1 we added ‘psychalgia’, a strict historical synonym. Reference Kendler8 This research retrieved only around 30 papers addressing biological features. We therefore decided to expand the keywords by asking experts in cognitive neuroscience, who recommended that we add ‘social pain’ and ‘social distress’ because they are considered as non-physical pain in this field. Reference Eisenberger2, Reference Olié, Doell, Corradi-Dell’Acqua, Courtet, Perroud and Schwartz37 We also included ‘emotional pain’, which was formalised in psychological science by Bolger and has been used as a synonym of non-physical pain in various works. Reference Tossani38Reference Bolger41 Also, we included ‘spiritual pain’ and ‘soul pain’, coined by Saunders as one of the dimensions of total pain. Reference Clark42Reference O’Neill and Mako48 Finally, to allow for a broader search, we included the keywords ‘mental suffering’, ‘psychological suffering’, ‘emotional suffering’, ‘painful feelings’ and ‘hurt feelings’, as our pilot search strategy showed that they were used as synonyms in biological publications. We also investigated the notion of ‘psychological distress’ through MEDLINE but an examination of the context of the use of these words in a sample of approximately 200 articles allowed us to conclude that most of the time it is used as a synonym for symptoms of anxiety or depression experienced in response to adverse events, which are outside the scope of this review. We did not include ‘social rejection’ or ‘social exclusion’ in our search equations, as these two terms refer to experimental paradigms or objective facts, and not to the subjective experience of non-physical pain. To evaluate the validity of this search strategy, we used a programming tool based on text mining and network co-occurrence with different bibliographic files from different keywords combinations on two databases (MEDLINE and Web of Science) and found keywords similar to those identified with the previous methods. Reference Grames, Stillman, Tingley and Elphick49

The final search query consisted of 15 terms: ‘mental pain’, ‘psychological pain’, ‘psychache’, ‘psychalgia’, ‘psychic pain’, ‘emotional pain’, ‘spiritual pain’, ‘soul pain’, ‘mental suffering’, ‘psychological suffering’, ‘emotional suffering’, ‘painful feelings’, ‘hurt feelings’, ‘social pain’ and ‘social distress’. Search equations for each database are displayed in Supplementary material 1, available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2024.292.

Selection of the relevant articles

After removal of duplicates, two investigators (E.K.D. with either A.R. or A.C.) independently screened the articles by title and abstract using the Covidence systematic review software. 50 The eligibility criteria were refined during the selection of the first 5% of the sample. Disagreements were resolved by discussion and consensus. The same procedure was applied to the screening of full-text articles. We also screened the references included in systematic reviews and meta-analyses identified through our search strategy.

Data extraction

Procedure

A data charting form listing all extracted data was jointly developed by E.K.D., L.G.-B. and A.C. The form was tested and refined by the three authors on 5% of the sample. One investigator (E.K.D. or A.R.) extracted the information from all full text. A second investigator (A.C.) checked 70% of the extracted data.

Characteristics of the studies

For each included published report, we extracted the journal name, year of publication, author(s), title and continent of the first author’s institution. We extracted the type of study subjects/participants (rodents or humans), the design (observational or experimental), the population (setting, health status, sample size). We also extracted whether one of the aims of the study was to investigate the shared physiological basis of non-physical pain with physical pain. Finally, we extracted the name of each biomarker that the authors reported to have measured in association with non-physical pain, as reported in the methods and/or results section of their study, as well as the investigation methods used (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalogram (EEG)). For studies using fMRI, we investigated whether they used whole brain analysis and/or analyses on specified regions of interest (ROIs) and/or functional connectivity analysis (with or without connectivity graphs).

Conceptual features

We extracted the terms used to refer to non-physical pain (pain not primarily felt in the body). We also extracted the definition corresponding to each term, if any, and potential associated references. By ‘definition’, we broadly understand any statement describing the non-physical pain either by its nature, attributes and/or function or by examples. For instance, if an article reported a definition of social pain by Eisenberger and a definition of psychological pain by Shneidman, we extracted both, as the author reported them, and their corresponding references.

Methodological characteristics

For observational studies, we extracted the exposure to non-physical pain (for instance some authors considered depression or suicidality as an exposure to non-physical pain) and the design regarding measurement at multiple time points (e.g. prospective cohort) or a single time point (e.g. cross-sectional design). For experimental studies, we extracted the name and description of the experimental paradigm used to induce or mimic non-physical pain and any reference to an external validation study. We extracted whether a control for potential confounders was used in the paradigm to assess the specificity of the biomarker regarding non-physical pain (e.g. if the effect of surprise linked to the unannounced induction of pain was considered). As an important component of the paradigm, we specifically extracted whether the duration of the induction of the non-physical pain was reported. Finally, for both observational and experimental studies, we extracted the name of any instrument used to measure non-physical pain. For experimental studies, we also extracted whether the timeline to the measurement of non-physical pain after induction/mimicking was reported.

Analysis and synthesis of results

Regarding the analysis of conceptual features, we calculated the number of different terms used per study. Two investigators (E.K.D. and A.C.) performed an inductive thematic qualitative content analysis of the definitions of the different non-physical pain retrieved. Reference Hsieh and Shannon51 For each term (e.g. social pain, mental pain), we then proposed a typology of their corresponding definitions based on the most frequent themes retrieved (themes cited by more than 10% of the definitions attached to the corresponding term).

Regarding the analysis of methodological characteristics, one researcher (E.K.D.) inductively classified the experimental paradigms depending on the nature of the task inducing non-physical pain and calculated the frequency and percentage of use across the sample. The researcher classified all unique biomarkers in overarching categories using biological and semantic considerations. This classification was checked by another researcher (L.G.-B. or J.H.). One researcher (E.K.D.) classified the investigation methods depending on their technical nature and their proximity to other investigation methods (e.g. ‘SNP genotyping method via MALDI-TOF’ and ‘SNP analysis’ were classified as ‘SNP genotyping’). This classification was checked by another researcher (L.G.-B. or J.H.).

We then analysed methodological patterns of investigations of biomarkers across experimental studies by identifying each unique combination of an experimental paradigm plus a categorised or uncategorised investigation method plus a biomarker category. We used a Sankey diagram to visualise these methodological patterns, created with the ggalluvial library. Reference Brunson52

Regarding measures of non-physical pain, we categorised each of them as a self-reported or an observer-reported measure. Using the validation paper reported by the authors, we extracted the construct it aimed to measure (e.g. the Positive and Negative Affect Scale measures the presence of positive and negative emotions).

Results

The extraction on 6 November 2023 retrieved 6605 records after removal of duplicates, with 193 full texts searched, resulting in the final inclusion of 88 published reports of primary research studies published between 2003 and 2023 (Fig. 1 and Supplementary material 2). We additionally screened the references of four meta-analyses and three systematic reviews to identify potential additional studies (Supplementary material 3). This screening did not identify any supplemental studies to be included in our screening process. The 88 included reports covered 96 distinct studies, with 4 studies (2 articles) involving rodents and 92 studies (86 articles) involving humans.

Fig. 1 Study flowchart.

a. References of systematic secondary research publications were screened in search of additional references of primary research publications that could have been overlooked by the search strategy within the 3 databases.

b. A single report could discuss more than one study.

Studies investigating non-physical pain in animal studies

Two articles reported on four studies performed with wild-type rats. All explored the shared biological basis of non-physical pain with physical pain, focusing on the involvement of the endocannabinoid system and on the effect of morphine treatment in their experimental model. Reference Schneider, Pätz, Spanagel and Schneider53,Reference Ruiz-Salas, De La Casa, Torres and Papini54 To induce non-physical pain, one article reported on a social rejection paradigm and measured non-physical pain as an acute aversive emotional reaction via changes in social interactions (attack behaviour, abnormal vocalisations). Reference Schneider, Pätz, Spanagel and Schneider53 The other used non-social aversive feedback (reward downshift with sucrose) and measured non-physical pain by a drop in sucrose consumption. Reference Ruiz-Salas, De La Casa, Torres and Papini54 Regarding the terms and definitions of non-physical pain used in these studies, one article used ‘social pain’ and ‘social distress’ without providing a clear definition. Reference Schneider, Pätz, Spanagel and Schneider53 The other used the term ‘psychological pain’ and defined it as ‘a negative emotion triggered by reward loss’, following Papini et al. Reference Ruiz-Salas, De La Casa, Torres and Papini54,Reference Papini, Fuchs and Torres55

Studies investigating non-physical pain in humans

Among the 92 human studies, 73.9% (68/92) were experimental (induced non-physical pain in people) and the remainder were observational (26.1%, 24/92), of which 87.5% (21/24) had a cross-sectional design (Table 1). Notably, 27.2% (25/92) of the studies aimed to investigate shared biological bases of non-physical pain with physical pain, corresponding to 20.6% (14/68) of the experimental studies and 45.8% (11/24) of the observational studies.

Table 1 Characteristics of the human studies (n = 92)

a. A single report could discuss more than one study, and different studies might have been conducted in the same sample. The 86 reports with human participants covered 92 studies with human participants.

b. Some studies included both healthy and not healthy participants.

c. Some studies included participants with several health disorders.

Overall, the 92 human studies investigated 297 unique biomarkers classified into 14 overarching categories and one ‘Other’ category (Supplementary material 4). The three most investigated overarching categories of biomarker were the prefrontal cortex (44/92, 47.8%), the cingulate cortex (37/92, 40.2%) and the insula (31/92, 33.7%) (Supplementary material 5, Table A). At the biomarker level, the most investigated were the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (24/92, 26.1%), the anterior insula (18/92, 19.6%), and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (16/92, 17.4%) (Supplementary Material 5, Table B). These biomarkers were explored using 28 unique investigation methods, of which fMRI was the most frequently used (38/92, 41.3%), followed by EEG (17/92, 18.5%) and protein expression and kinetic assays (10/92, 10.9%) (Supplementary materials 6 and 7). Concerning the 38 studies utilising fMRI, 71.1% (27/38) used whole brain analysis, 76.3% (29/38) used analyses on specified regions of interest (ROIs) and 28.9% (11/38) used functional connectivity analysis. Of the 11 utilising functional connectivity analysis, 27.3% (3/11) used connectivity graphs.

Overall, the studies encompass 7778 participants and had an average sample size of 78 (s.d. = 105), varying from a minimum of 10 to a maximum of 762 participants. The experimental studies mostly involved healthy individuals (83.8%, 57/68), whereas the observational studies involved more people with mental disorders (62.5%, 15/24). In fact, these 15 observational studies considered mental disorders as an exposure to non-physical pain, whereas the other 9 used the lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic, loneliness or social exclusion as the exposure. Finally, 25% (6/24) of the observational studies framed non-physical pain as a common experience of human life and involved participants from the general population.

Table 2 displays the 7 different types of experimental paradigm used to induce non-physical pain across the 68 experimental studies. All had unknown external validity, i.e. it was neither established nor discussed to what extent the non-physical pain deemed to be induced by the paradigm corresponded to non-physical pain observed in clinical populations. The most used paradigm category was the cyberball paradigm (40/68, 58.8%), followed by negative social judgement tasks (12/68, 17.6%) and by paradigms triggering memories of non-physical pain (6/68, 8.8%). Across the 68 experimental studies, 32.4% (22/68) did not report any control for potential confounders in the investigation of the association of the biomarker with non-physical pain. Finally, 17.6 % (12/68) did not report the duration of the induction of non-physical pain. Figure 2 represents in the form of an alluvial plot the 46 unique methodological patterns, i.e. combinations of an experimental model of non-physical pain plus a method of investigation plus a category of biomarker. The most frequent patterns were ‘cyberball–fMRI–cingulate cortex’ and ‘cyberball–fMRI–prefrontal cortex’ (19/68, 27.9%).

Table 2 The seven experimental paradigms used across the 68 human experimental studies and their external validity in clinical populations a

a. Each experimental paradigm is presented with a short description and an example from a study retrieved by the systematic review.

Fig. 2 Methodological patterns of investigation in experimental studies of non-physical pain.

The Sankey diagram displays the 416 methodological patterns identified across the 68 human experimental studies. Each line represents a methodological pattern, i.e. a combination of an experimental paradigm (first column), an investigation method (second column) and a biomarker (third column for the category and fourth column for the biomarker). Widths of the lines are proportional to the frequency of use of the methodological patterns across the studies. For instance, the use of the cyberball paradigm to investigate the prefrontal cortex using fMRI was the most frequently used methodological pattern (36/416, 8.7%). More precisely, the dlPFC was investigated by 8.33% (3/36) of the investigations of the prefrontal cortex using the cyberball as an experimental paradigm and fMRI as an investigation method.

ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; BP, blood pressure; dACC, dorsal ACC; dlPFC, dorsolateral PFC; dmPFC, dorsomedial PFC; ECG, electrocardiogram; EEG, electroencephalogram; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; IL-6, interleukin 6; LPP, late positive potential; MRS, magnetic resonance spectroscopy; N2, P3 and P3b, event-related potential components; NIRS, near-infrared spectroscopy; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PAG, periacqueductal gray; PET/SPECT, positron emission tomography and/or single-photon emission computed tomography; PFC, prefrontal cortex; pgACC, pregenual ACC; rTMS/TMS, repetitive high frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation and/or transcranial magnetic stimulation; sgACC, subgenual ACC; SII, secondary somatosensory cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; SMA, supplementary motor area; vACC, ventral ACC; vlPFC, ventrolateral PFC; vmPFC, ventromedial PFC; vPFC, ventral PFC.

Table 3 presents the different instruments used to measure non-physical pain. Of the 92 studies, 9.8% (9/92) did not measure it at all. Only 18% (17/92 – 14 observational studies and three experimental studies) used one or more scales that were purposively developed to measure non-physical pain, such as the Physical and Psychological Pain Visual Analog Scale (PPP-VAS), the Orbach and Mikulincer Mental Pain Scale (OMMPS) and the Three-Dimensional Psychological Pain Scale (TDPPS). Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1 Overall, 23.9% (22/92) of the studies used one or more ad hoc measures of non-physical pain, i.e. measures developed by the authors for the purpose of their study. Furthermore, 50% of studies (46/92) used one or more measurement instruments for other constructs, such as social anxiety, depression, loneliness or threats of fundamental needs. For instance, the Need Threat Questionnaire (NTQ) was the most frequently used by experimental study (31/59, 52.5%). Reference Williams, Cheung and Choi63 Finally, 54.2% of the experimental studies (32/59) did not report the timeline for the outcome measure of non-physical pain after the mimicking or induction of the pain.

Table 3 Measures of non-physical pain in human studies (n = 92)

ASI, Addiction Severity Index; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; BFNE, Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation; LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; MSR, Mehrabian Sensitivity to Rejection scale; NTQ, Need Threat Questionnaire; OMMPS, Orbach and Mikulincer Mental Pain Scale; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Scale; PAS, Psychache Scale; PPP-VAS, Physical and Psychological Pain Visual Analogue Scale; STAI, State Trait Anxiety Inventory; TDPPS, Three-Dimensional Psychological Pain Scale.

a. Some studies used multiple measures.

Regarding the terms and definitions of non-physical pain used, the expression ‘social pain’ (61/86, 70.9%) was most common, followed by ‘psychological pain’ (25/86, 29.1%) and ‘social distress’ (23/86, 26.7%) (Supplementary material 8, Table A). Experimental studies tended to use ‘social pain’ (55/64, 85.9%) and ‘social distress’ (22/64, 34.4%) more, whereas observational studies used ‘psychological pain’ (15/21, 71.4%) and ‘mental pain’ (6/21, 28.6%) more. Regarding the consistency of the terms used within a same study, 58.2% (50/86) used 2 or more terms to refer to non-physical pain (Supplementary material 8, Table B). Overall, only 41.9% (36/86) of the publications reported a definition. Table 4 displays the different definitions of non-physical pain retrieved across these 36 articles. Despite heterogeneity, all definitions agreed on the core feature of non-physical pain being a negative subjective emotional state.

Table 4 Terms and definitions of non-physical pain retrieved in human studies (n = 86)

a. The 86 human publications used 11 terms to refer to non-physical pain, of which only 4 received a definition (social pain, psychological pain, mental pain and spiritual pain) across 36 studies. For mental pain and spiritual pain, we report the only definition retrieved. For social pain and psychological pain, which had several definitions, we synthesised their thematic content based on the qualitative content analysis. The comprehensive results of the qualitative content analysis are reported in Supplementary material 8 Table C.

Discussion

This methodological systematic review proposed a comprehensive research mapping of the field of studies investigating biological features of non-physical pain. It identified 92 human studies and an additional sample of 4 studies in rodents published in the two past decades. Human studies investigated a total of 297 candidate biomarkers with a repeated pattern of investigation ‘social pain–cyberball–fMRI–cingulate cortex and/or prefrontal cortex’.

We identified 9 conceptual and methodological challenges to improving research on the biological basis of non-physical pain, for which we propose 11 ways forward (See Table A1 under Appendix). Four of these challenges are explored here.

Conceptual weakness of existing definitions

A first concern regards the conceptual weakness of the field. The distinctions between various terms related to non-physical pain are not clearly defined at the level of individual studies. In fact, 58.2% of the studies use two or more different terms without clarifying how they are similar or different. Additionally, when looking at the sample of studies as a whole, even a single term can be defined in different ways across studies. Additionally, 58.1% of the human studies did not clearly define non-physical pain. The fact that publications use multiple terms with unclarified meaning or that the same term can have several definitions contributes to conceptual confusion. Besides the resulting fragmentation of the research field, this also compromises the validity and interpretability of results, as shown for other concepts such as empathy. Reference Hopwood, Morey and Markon68,Reference Bollen69 Although all definitions retrieved by our study agreed on one core feature of non-physical pain – a negative subjective emotional state – we do not conclude that it should be an operational definition of non-physical pain. Rather, further research that would aim at developing an operational definition would need to at least complement the findings of our study with (a) clinical investigations of non-physical pain (e.g. transcultural studies exploring how people use the word pain or phenomenological descriptions of the lived experience) and (b) examination of other research fields (such as clinical psychology or philosophy). Reference Corns21,Reference Borelli, Bigi, Potenza, Artioli, Eliardo and Mucciarini70,Reference Kugelmann71

Lack of valid measurement instruments

A second concern regards the measurement of non-physical pain. Most of the studies measured non-physical pain using a tool designed to measure constructs other than non-physical pain (e.g. social anxiety, depression, loneliness). For example, the most frequently used measurement instrument was the Need Threat Questionnaire (NTQ), a scale measuring threatened fundamental needs rather than non-physical pain. Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Williams7,Reference Gerber, Chang and Reimel20,Reference Williams, Cheung and Choi63,Reference Macdonald and Leary64 Only 15 studies used a patient-reported outcome developed to measure the construct of non-physical pain. It is worth reminding that, in our previous systematic review, the 10 tools used were assessed to have uncertain content validity using the COSMIN framework. Reference Charvet, Boutron, Morvan, Le Berre, Touboul and Gaillard1 Just as problematic, the second most frequently used measures were developed ad hoc, with little to no reporting of their development or the validation of their psychometric properties. Reference Elson, Hussey, Alsalti and Arslan72 Additionally, the use of measures tailored for an experimental paradigm questions the possibility of translating the results to non-physical pain observed in clinical populations or even in the general population outside of the context of the paradigm. This is also the case for the NTQ, tailored by Williams et al for social psychology experiments based on cyber-ostracism paradigms (foremost the cyberball), and of course for all other ad hoc measures. A final problem with measurement was that 10% of the human studies did not assess whether non-physical pain was present in participants after experimental induction.

Experimental paradigms and their translationality

A third concern regards the experimental paradigms used to induce non-physical pain. The cyberball paradigm was by far the most commonly used. However, several methodological considerations have been raised regarding the extrapolation of the state induced. Reference Perini, Gustafsson, Hamilton, Kämpe, Zetterqvist and Heilig73,Reference Weschke and Niedeggen74 It remains unclear whether the pain experienced by people during the cyberball paradigm, or in the other experimental paradigms, is like the non-physical pain experienced either in the context of mental disorders (e.g. non-physical pain in depression) or in non-pathological contexts (e.g. grief, romantic rejection). The repeated use of the ‘social pain–cyberball–fMRI–NTQ’ conceptual and methodological framework tends to forge ‘social pain’ as the state induced by the cyberball experiment. One may speculate that frequent use of the cyberball paradigm may stem from the consistency of its effects on the activity of some brain regions. However, such selection of experimental tasks may lead to a critical lack of reliability of task-based measures at the individual level, a drawback referred to as a ‘reliability paradox’. Reference Hedge, Powell and Sumner75 Put simply, this paradox might have thrived because seeking for replicable task effects results in selecting tasks with low inter-individual variability, which entails poor ability to capture inter-individual differences. Although the studies retrieved in our review investigated acute non-physical pain triggered by a short exposure to social rejection, there is a need to further explore forms of non-physical pain, in particular chronic forms that might be closer to those experienced by people with mental disorders or triggered by other events. Reference Conejero, Olié, Calati, Ducasse and Courtet5,Reference Nobile, Lutz, Olie, Courtet and Baca-Garcia76Reference Jollant, Perreira, Fiori, Richard-Devantoy, Lutz and Belzeaux78 This certainly needs the development of new paradigms and measures derived from refined clinical characterisation of non-physical pain.

Questionable animal models of non-physical pain

A fourth concern regards animal models used in studies to dissect pathophysiological mechanisms involved in non-physical pain. Acknowledging that animals could have equivalent experience, it is challenging to develop a convincing animal model of non-physical pain, as it is for many other psychological constructs derived from human experience (e.g. depression, anhedonia, suicidality). As an echo of the research in humans marked by the cyberball paradigm, of the four studies identified in animals, two used a social defeat paradigm in rodents. Reference Schneider, Pätz, Spanagel and Schneider53 In these studies, non-physical pain is extrapolated from experiences of acute stress caused by a short experience of social defeat. However, the model of social defeat was initially developed to enable an animal model of depression characterised by marked negative and sustainable emotional biases. Reference Planchez, Surget and Belzung79 Of note, behaviours characterised with the social defeat paradigm can be referred to as hopelessness in animal research, but rarely with the common terms used to refer to non-physical pain, which may explain the paucity of animal studies collected in our search. Reference Planchez, Surget and Belzung79 Moreover, this paradigm was meant to chronically expose the subjects to social defeat rather than to a short exposure and therefore we might need a better characterisation of the emotional states induced by an acute and a chronic exposure to social defeat (difference and similarities) to link it to non-physical pain in human. This echoes the general concerns about the robustness of research on psychiatric biomarkers and the necessary caution regarding the translational aspect of the behaviours and dimensions assessed. Reference Abi-Dargham, Moeller, Ali, DeLorenzo, Domschke and Horga80Reference Hirst, Klasen, Rhodes, Macleod and Andrews84

The reproducibility crisis

A final concern regards the incomplete reporting of methods and methodological features, which are known factors of the ‘reproducibility crisis’. Reference Munafò, Nosek, Bishop, Button, Chambers and Percie Du Sert85,Reference Munafò, Chambers, Collins, Fortunato and Macleod86 Previous meta-research initiatives have tackled the issues of improving research practices in life sciences, with the development of new guidelines for the reporting, designs and evaluation of animal studies, as well as their evidence synthesis. Reference Hooijmans and Ritskes-Hoitinga87,Reference Hooijmans, De Vries, Ritskes-Hoitinga, Rovers, Leeflang and IntHout88

Limitations

Our study has several limitations. First, regarding our search strategy, we included 15 terms referring to the state of non-physical pain but, contrary to other studies, we did not include terms referring to situations or paradigms triggering non-physical pain (e.g. grief, social rejection, social exclusion), as these two terms referred to experimental paradigms or objective facts, and not to the subjective experience of non-physical pain. Reference Rotge, Lemogne, Hinfray, Huguet, Grynszpan and Tartour17,Reference Tan, Duan, Liu, Qiao and Luo89 To our knowledge, there are no scientific guidelines for the mapping of conceptual and methodological features across studies. To compensate, we provide an extensive description of our mapping procedure in the interests of reproducibility of our analysis of conceptual features and methodological characteristics.

Future research

The biomedical literature investigates pains that are not primarily felt in the body, based on the search of shared biological features with physical pain. This research field is hampered by a weak conceptualisation and lack of measurement. Additionally, the use of experimental paradigms to induce non-physical pain and measures with unknown external validity leads to results that cannot be translated in clinical practice. Acknowledging that it is critical to have biological investigation of the breadth of human suffering, this methodological systematic review proposed ways forwards to improve research. Overall, the priority seems to be the development of an agreed international definition and measure of non-physical pain to be used both in observational and experimental research and in clinical practice to improve the consistency and translationality of the results. The foundation of a scientific terminology is to use unambiguous, stable terms that are universally adopted, even if this comes at the cost of some reductionism. It is worth mentioning that, as semantic heterogeneity occurs frequently in psychopathological concepts, attempts to harmonise the terminology relating to non-physical pain would inadvertently rely on auxiliary psychological notions which in turn will require operational clarification. Hence, a certain degree of circularity will probably have to be tolerated for any potential definition of non-physical pain. Nevertheless, concerning for instance physical pain, the agreement on a definition of physical pain in 1976 by the International Association for the Study of Pain accelerated both research and the development of therapeutic interventions. Reference Raja, Carr, Cohen, Finnerup, Flor and Gibson90

Supplementary material

The supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2024.292.

Data availability

Qualitative and quantitative extracted data will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

E.K.D. conceptualised the study, designed the method, performed the analysis, gathered the data, created the figures and wrote the original draft; A.R. and L.G.-B. gathered the data and reviewed the content; I.B. and C.L. designed the methods and revised the paper; C.H., J.H. and A.-C.P. performed the analysis and revised the paper; A.C. supervised the project conceptualised the study, designed the method, performed the analysis, gathered the data and critically reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

A.C. received funding from the Fondation Bettencourt Schueller for a research programme on non-physical pain.

Declaration of interest

A.C.-P. received funding for conference activities from Janssen-Cilag, outside the submitted work. C.H. received fees for conference activities from Janssen-Cilag, Sanofi-Aventis and Lundbeck, outside the submitted work. C.L. reports non-financial support from Nordic Pharma France, outside the submitted work. J.H. received speaker fees from Eisai, outside of the scope of this work. A.C. is a member of the BJPsych editorial board and did not take part in the review or decision-making process of this paper.

Appendix

Table A1 Challenges and ways to improve research on the biological basis of non-physical pain

References

Charvet, C, Boutron, I, Morvan, Y, Le Berre, C, Touboul, S, Gaillard, R, et al. How to measure mental pain: a systematic review assessing measures of mental pain. Evid Based Mental Health 2022; 25: e4.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, NI. The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2012; 13: 14.Google ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, NI, Lieberman, MD. Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn Sci 2004; 8: 294300.Google ScholarPubMed
Meerwijk, EL, Weiss, SJ. Toward a unifying definition of psychological pain. J Loss Trauma 2011; 16: 402–12.Google Scholar
Conejero, I, Olié, E, Calati, R, Ducasse, D, Courtet, P. Psychological pain, depression, and suicide: recent evidences and future directions. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2018; 20: 33.Google ScholarPubMed
Shneidman, ES. Suicide as psychache. J Nerv Ment Dis 1993; 181: 145–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, NI, Lieberman, MD, Williams, KD. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 2003; 302: 290–2.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS. Melancholia as psychalgia: the integration of psychophysiological theory and psychopathologic observation in the mid-19th century. Mol Psychiatry 2023; 28: 230–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Ey, H, Bernard, P, Brisset, Ch. États dépressifs et crises de mélancolie. In Manuel de psychiatrie (Sixième Édition) [Depressive states and attacks of melancholy. In Manual of Psychiatry (6th edn)] (ed Ey, H, Bernard, P, Brisset, Ch): 167203. Elsevier Masson, 1989 (https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9782294711589500096).Google Scholar
Ducasse, D, Holden, RR, Boyer, L, Artéro, S, Calati, R, Guillaume, S, et al. Psychological pain in suicidality: a meta-analysis. J Clin Psychiatry 2018; 79: 16r10732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verrocchio, MC, Carrozzino, D, Marchetti, D, Andreasson, K, Fulcheri, M, Bech, P. Mental pain and suicide: a systematic review of the literature. Front Psychiatry 2016; 7: 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanarini, MC, Frankenburg, FR. Emotional hypochondriasis, hyperbole, and the borderline patient. J Psychother Pract Res 1994; 3: 2536.Google ScholarPubMed
Bohaterewicz, B, Sobczak, AM, Krześniak, A, Mętel, D, Adamczyk, P. On the relation of gyrification and cortical thickness alterations to the suicidal risk and mental pain in chronic schizophrenia outpatients. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag 2021; 316: 111343.Google Scholar
Demirkol, ME, Namlı, Z, Eriş Davul, Ö, Karaytuğ, MO, Tamam, L, Yılmaz, H. Psychache and suicidal history in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. NDT 2019; 15: 3531–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Fava, GA, Tomba, E, Brakemeier, EL, Carrozzino, D, Cosci, F, Eöry, A, et al. Mental pain as a transdiagnostic patient-reported outcome measure. Psychother Psychosom. 2019; 88: 341–49.Google Scholar
Ducasse, D, Courtet, P, Olié, E. Physical and social pains in borderline disorder and neuroanatomical correlates: a systematic review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2014; 16: 443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotge, JY, Lemogne, C, Hinfray, S, Huguet, P, Grynszpan, O, Tartour, E, et al. A meta-analysis of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 10: 1927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meerwijk, EL, Ford, JM, Weiss, SJ. Brain regions associated with psychological pain: implications for a neural network and its relationship to physical pain. Brain Imag Behav 2013; 7: 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wager, TD, Atlas, LY, Lindquist, MA, Roy, M, Woo, CW, Kross, E. An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N Engl J Med 2013; 368: 1388–97.Google ScholarPubMed
Gerber, JP, Chang, SH, Reimel, H. Construct validity of Williams’ ostracism needs threat scale. Pers Individ Diff 2017; 115: 50–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corns, J. The social pain posit. Aust J Philos 2015; 93: 561–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, F, Tricco, AC, Munn, Z, Pollock, D, Saran, A, Sutton, A, et al. Mapping reviews, scoping reviews, and evidence and gap maps (EGMs): the same but different – the ‘Big Picture’ review family. Syst Rev. 2023; 12: 45.Google ScholarPubMed
Chavalarias, D, Ioannidis, JPA. Science mapping analysis characterizes 235 biases in biomedical research. J Clin Epidemiol 2010; 63: 1205–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, VT, Rivière, P, Ripoll, P, Barnier, J, Vuillemot, R, Ferrand, G, et al. Research response to coronavirus disease 2019 needed better coordination and collaboration: a living mapping of registered trials. J Clin Epidemiol 2021; 130: 107–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Addiction Cue-Reactivity Initiative (ACRI) Network, Sangchooli, A, Zare-Bidoky, M, Fathi Jouzdani, A, Schacht, J, Bjork, JM, et al. Parameter space and potential for biomarker development in 25 years of fMRI drug cue reactivity: a systematic review. JAMA Psychiatry 2024; 81: 414–25.Google Scholar
Cragg, JJ, Kramer, JLK, Borisoff, JF, Patrick, DM, Ramer, MS. Ecological fallacy as a novel risk factor for poor translation in neuroscience research: a systematic review and simulation study. Eur J Clin Investig 2019; 49: e13045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mwilambwe-Tshilobo, L, Spreng, RN. Social exclusion reliably engages the default network: a meta-analysis of Cyberball. Neuroimage 2021; 227: 117666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, S, Frum, C, Asp, E, Weiss, RM, Lewis, JW, Cacioppo, JT. A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of social rejection. Sci Rep 2013; 3: 2027.Google ScholarPubMed
Vanhollebeke, G, Aers, F, Goethals, L, Raedt, RD, Baeken, C, Mierlo, PV, et al. Uncovering the underlying factors of ERP changes in the cyberball paradigm: a systematic review investigating the impact of ostracism and paradigm characteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2023; 155: 105464.Google Scholar
Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021; 372: n71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FDA-NIH Biomarker Working Group. BEST (Biomarkers, EndpointS, and other Tools) Resource. Food and Drug Administration (USA), 2016 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK338448/).Google Scholar
DeWall, CN, MacDonald, G, Webster, GD, Masten, CL, Baumeister, RF, Powell, C, et al. Acetaminophen reduces social pain: behavioral and neural evidence. Psychol Sci 2010; 21: 931–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Lefebre, C, Glanville, J, Briscoe, S, Featherstone, R, Littlewood, A, Marshall, C, et al. Searching for and selecting studies. In Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.3 (updated February 2022) (eds Higgins, JPT, Thomas, J, Chandler, J, Cumpston, M, Li, T, Page, MJ, et al): Ch. 4. Cochrane Collaboration, 2022 (https://training.cochrane.org/handbook/current/chapter-04).Google Scholar
Paynter, RA, Featherstone, R, Stoeger, E, Fiordalisi, C, Voisin, C, Adam, GP. A prospective comparison of evidence synthesis search strategies developed with and without text-mining tools. J Clin Epidemiol 2021; 139: 350–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperr, E. PubVenn. Sperr, 2015 (https://pubvenn.appspot.com/).Google Scholar
Affengruber, L, van der Maten, MM, Spiero, I, Nussbaumer-Streit, B, Mahmić-Kaknjo, M, Ellen, ME, et al. An exploration of available methods and tools to improve the efficiency of systematic review production: a scoping review. BMC Med Res Methodol 2024; 24: 210.Google ScholarPubMed
Olié, E, Doell, KC, Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C, Courtet, P, Perroud, N, Schwartz, S. Physical pain recruits the nucleus accumbens during social distress in borderline personality disorder. Social Cognit Affect Neurosci 2018; 13: 1071–80.Google ScholarPubMed
Tossani, E. The concept of mental pain. Psychother Psychosom 2013; 82: 6773.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunkley, C, Borthwick, A, Bartlett, R, Dunkley, L, Palmer, S, Gleeson, S, et al. Hearing the suicidal patient’s emotional pain: a typological model to improve communication. Crisis 2018; 39: 267–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahi, RS, Dieffenbach, MC, Gan, S, Lee, M, Hazlett, LI, Burns, SM, et al. The comfort in touch: immediate and lasting effects of handholding on emotional pain. PLoS One 2021; 16: e0246753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolger, EA. Grounded theory analysis of emotional pain. Psychother Res 1999; 9: 342–62.Google Scholar
Clark, D. From margins to centre: a review of the history of palliative care in cancer. The Lancet Oncology 2007; 8: 430–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. ‘Total pain’, disciplinary power and the body in the work of Cicely Saunders, 1958–1967. Soc Sci Med 1999; 49: 727–36.Google Scholar
Saunders, C. The symptomatic treatment of incurable malignant disease. Prescr J 1964; 4: 6873.Google Scholar
Mako, C, Galek, K, Poppito, SR. Spiritual pain among patients with advanced cancer in palliative care. J Palliat Med 2006; 9: 1106–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Delgado-Guay, MO, Hui, D, Parsons, HA, Govan, K, De la Cruz, M, Thorney, S, et al. Spirituality, religiosity, and spiritual pain in advanced cancer patients. J Pain Symp Manag 2011; 41: 986–94.Google ScholarPubMed
McGrath, P. Creating a language for ‘spiritual pain’ through research: a beginning. Support Care Cancer 2002; 10: 637–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Neill, MT, Mako, C. Addressing spiritual pain. Health Prog 2011; 92: 42–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Grames, EM, Stillman, AN, Tingley, MW, Elphick, CS. An automated approach to identifying search terms for systematic reviews using keyword co-occurrence networks. Methods Ecol Evol 2019; 10: 1645–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veritas Health Innovation. Covidence. Veritas Health Innovation, 2022 (www.covidence.org).Google Scholar
Hsieh, HF, Shannon, SE. Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res 2005; 15: 1277–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunson, J. ggalluvial: layered grammar for alluvial plots. J Open Source Softw 2020; 5: 2017.Google Scholar
Schneider, P, Pätz, M, Spanagel, R, Schneider, M. Adolescent social rejection alters pain processing in a CB1 receptor dependent manner. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2016; 26: 121201.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Salas, JC, De La Casa, LG, Torres, C, Papini, MR. Psychological pain and opioid receptors: Reward downshift is disrupted when tested in a context signaling morphine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2022; 216: 173386.Google Scholar
Papini, MR, Fuchs, PN, Torres, C. Behavioral neuroscience of psychological pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 48: 5369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzgibbon, BM, Kirkovski, M, Bailey, NW, Thomson, RH, Eisenberger, N, Enticott, PG, et al. Low-Frequency brain stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex increases the negative impact of social exclusion among those high in personal distress. Soc Neurosci 2017; 12(3): 41237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, DT, Sanford, BJ, Meyers, KK, Love, TM, Hazlett, KE, Walker, SJ, et al. It still hurts: altered endogenous opioid activity in the brain during social rejection and acceptance in major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20(2): 193200.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, MF, Wellisch, DK, Stanton, AL, Eisenberger, NI, Irwin, MR, Lieberman, MD. Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain’s reward center. Neuroimage 2008; 42(2): 72969.Google ScholarPubMed
Mo, L, Guo, T, Zhang, Y, Xu, F, Zhang, D. The role of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex on emotional regulation of social pain in depressed patients: a TMS study. Acta Psychologica Sinica 2021; 53(5): 494504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckel, A, Rothmayr, C, Rosengarth, K, Hajak, G, Greenlee, MW, Eichhammer, P. Aversive faces activate pain responsive regions in the brain. Neuroreport 2011; 22(11): 53548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priem, JS, McLaren, RM, Haunani Solomon, D. Relational messages, perceptions of hurt, and biological stress reactions to a disconfirming interaction. Commun Res, 2010; 37(1): 4872.Google Scholar
Inagaki, TK, Gianaros, PJ. Resting (tonic) blood pressure is associated with sensitivity to imagined and acute experiences of social pain: evidence from three studies. Psychol Sci 2022; 33(6): 98984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, KD, Cheung, CKT, Choi, W. Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored over the Internet. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000; 79: 748–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macdonald, G, Leary, MR. Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychol Bull 2005; 131: 202–23.Google ScholarPubMed
Reisch, T, Seifritz, E, Esposito, F, Wiest, R, Valach, L, Michel, K. An fMRI study on mental pain and suicidal behavior. J Affect Disord 2010; 126: 321–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orbach, I, Mikulincer, M, Sirota, P, Gilboa-Schechtman, E. Mental pain: a multidimensional operationalization and definition. Suicide & Life Threat Behav 2003; 33: 219–30.Google Scholar
Boss, L, Branson, S, Cron, S, Kang, DH. Spiritual pain in meals on wheels’ clients. Healthcare 2015; 3: 917–32.Google ScholarPubMed
Hopwood, CJ, Morey, LC, Markon, KE. What is a psychopathology dimension? Clin Psychol Rev 2023; 106: 102356.Google Scholar
Bollen, C. A reflective guide on the meaning of empathy in autism research. Methods Psychol 2023; 8: 100109.Google Scholar
Borelli, E, Bigi, S, Potenza, L, Artioli, F, Eliardo, S, Mucciarini, C, et al. Different semantic and affective meaning of the words associated to physical and social pain in cancer patients on early palliative/supportive care and in healthy, pain-free individuals. PLoS One 2021; 16: e0248755.Google ScholarPubMed
Kugelmann, R. Pain in the vernacular: psychological and physical. J Health Psychol 2000; 5: 305–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Elson, M, Hussey, I, Alsalti, T, Arslan, RC. Psychological measures aren’t toothbrushes. Commun Psychol 2023; 1: 25.Google ScholarPubMed
Perini, I, Gustafsson, PA, Hamilton, JP, Kämpe, R, Zetterqvist, M, Heilig, M. The salience of self, not social pain, is encoded by dorsal anterior cingulate and insula. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 6165.Google Scholar
Weschke, S, Niedeggen, M. Target and non-target processing during oddball and cyberball: a comparative event-related potential study. PLoS One 2016; 11: e0153941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedge, C, Powell, G, Sumner, P. The reliability paradox: why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behav Res 2018; 50: 1166–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nobile, B, Lutz, PE, Olie, E, Courtet, P. The role of opiates in social pain and suicidal behavior. In Behavioral Neurobiology of Suicide and Self Harm (ed Baca-Garcia, E): 197210. Springer International Publishing, 2020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cáceda, R, James, GA, Stowe, ZN, Delgado, PL, Kordsmeier, N, Kilts, CD. The neural correlates of low social integration as a risk factor for suicide. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2020; 270: 619–31.Google ScholarPubMed
Jollant, F, Perreira, F, Fiori, LM, Richard-Devantoy, S, Lutz, PE, Belzeaux, R, et al. Neural and molecular correlates of psychological pain during major depression, and its link with suicidal ideas. Progr Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biolog Psychiatry 2020; 100: 109909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Planchez, B, Surget, A, Belzung, C. Animal models of major depression: drawbacks and challenges. J Neural Transm 2019; 126: 1383–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abi-Dargham, A, Moeller, SJ, Ali, F, DeLorenzo, C, Domschke, K, Horga, G, et al. Candidate biomarkers in psychiatric disorders: state of the field. World Psychiatry 2023; 22: 236–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, AAM, Sutherland, BA, Landowski, LM, Macleod, M, Howells, DW. What has preclinical systematic review ever done for us? BMJ Open Sci 2022; 6: e100219.Google ScholarPubMed
Bigot, M, Vicq, E, Lledo, PM, Alonso, M, Henry, C. Assessing positive and negative valence systems to refine animal models of bipolar disorders: the example of GBR 12909-induced manic phenotype. Sci Rep 2022; 12: 7364.Google ScholarPubMed
Berk, M. Biomarkers in psychiatric disorders: status quo, impediments and facilitators. World Psychiatry 2023; 22: 174–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Hirst, TC, Klasen, MG, Rhodes, JK, Macleod, MR, Andrews, PJD. A systematic review and meta-analysis of hypothermia in experimental traumatic brain injury: why have promising animal studies not been replicated in pragmatic clinical trials? J Neurotrauma 2020; 37: 2057–68.Google Scholar
Munafò, MR, Nosek, BA, Bishop, DVM, Button, KS, Chambers, CD, Percie Du Sert, N, et al. A manifesto for reproducible science. Nat Hum Behav 2017; 1: 0021.Google ScholarPubMed
Munafò, MR, Chambers, C, Collins, A, Fortunato, L, Macleod, M. The reproducibility debate is an opportunity, not a crisis. BMC Res Notes 2022; 15: 43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooijmans, CR, Ritskes-Hoitinga, M. Progress in using systematic reviews of animal studies to improve translational research. PLoS Med 2013; 10: e1001482.Google ScholarPubMed
Hooijmans, CR, De Vries, RBM, Ritskes-Hoitinga, M, Rovers, MM, Leeflang, MM, IntHout, J, et al. Facilitating healthcare decisions by assessing the certainty in the evidence from preclinical animal studies. PLoS One 2018; 13: e0187271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, H, Duan, Q, Liu, Y, Qiao, X, Luo, S. Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain. Hum Brain Mapp 2022; 43: 3153–63.Google ScholarPubMed
Raja, SN, Carr, DB, Cohen, M, Finnerup, NB, Flor, H, Gibson, S, et al. The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain 2020; 161: 1976–82.Google Scholar
Open Science Collaboration. Estimating the Reproducibility of Psychological Science. Science, 2015.Google Scholar
Marek, S, Tervo-Clemmens, B, Calabro, FJ, Montez, DF, Kay, BP, Hatoum, AS, et al. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature 2022; 603: 654–60.Google ScholarPubMed
Chalmers, I, Bracken, MB, Djulbegovic, B, Garattini, S, Grant, J, Gülmezoglu, AM, et al. How to increase value and reduce waste when research priorities are set. The Lancet 2014; 383: 156–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Chevance, A, Ravaud, P, Cornelius, V, Mayo-Wilson, E, Furukawa, TA. Designing clinically useful psychopharmacological trials: challenges and ways forward. Lancet Psychiatry 2022; 9: 584–94.Google ScholarPubMed
Bale, TL, Abel, T, Akil, H, Carlezon, WA, Moghaddam, B, Nestler, EJ, et al. The critical importance of basic animal research for neuropsychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2019; 44: 1349–53.Google ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, JPA, Fanelli, D, Dunne, DD, Goodman, SN. Meta-research: evaluation and improvement of research methods and practices. PLoS Biol 2015; 13: e1002264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howells, DW, Sena, ES, Macleod, MR. Bringing rigour to translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurol 2014; 10: 3743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chevance, A, Tran, VT, Ravaud, P. Controversy and Debate Series on Core Outcome Sets. Paper 1: Improving the generalizability and credibility of core outcome sets (COS) by a large and international participation of diverse stakeholders. J Clin Epidemiol 2020; 125: 12.e1206.Google ScholarPubMed
Chevance, A, Ravaud, P, Tomlinson, A, Le Berre, C, Teufer, B, Touboul, S, et al. Identifying outcomes for depression that matter to patients, informal caregivers, and health-care professionals: qualitative content analysis of a large international online survey. Lancet Psychiatry 2020; 7: 692702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Percie Du Sert, N, Hurst, V, Ahluwalia, A, Alam, S, Avey, MT, Baker, M, et al. The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol 2020; 18: e3000410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Appelbaum, M, Cooper, H, Kline, RB, Mayo-Wilson, E, Nezu, AM, Rao, SM. Journal article reporting standards for quantitative research in psychology: the APA Publications and Communications Board task force report. Am Psychol 2018; 73: 325.Google ScholarPubMed
Hunniford, VT, Montroy, J, Fergusson, DA, Avey, MT, Wever, KE, McCann, SK, et al. Epidemiology and reporting characteristics of preclinical systematic reviews. PLoS Biol 2021; 19: e3001177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Study flowchart.a. References of systematic secondary research publications were screened in search of additional references of primary research publications that could have been overlooked by the search strategy within the 3 databases.b. A single report could discuss more than one study.

Figure 1

Table 1 Characteristics of the human studies (n = 92)

Figure 2

Table 2 The seven experimental paradigms used across the 68 human experimental studies and their external validity in clinical populationsa

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Methodological patterns of investigation in experimental studies of non-physical pain.The Sankey diagram displays the 416 methodological patterns identified across the 68 human experimental studies. Each line represents a methodological pattern, i.e. a combination of an experimental paradigm (first column), an investigation method (second column) and a biomarker (third column for the category and fourth column for the biomarker). Widths of the lines are proportional to the frequency of use of the methodological patterns across the studies. For instance, the use of the cyberball paradigm to investigate the prefrontal cortex using fMRI was the most frequently used methodological pattern (36/416, 8.7%). More precisely, the dlPFC was investigated by 8.33% (3/36) of the investigations of the prefrontal cortex using the cyberball as an experimental paradigm and fMRI as an investigation method.ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; BP, blood pressure; dACC, dorsal ACC; dlPFC, dorsolateral PFC; dmPFC, dorsomedial PFC; ECG, electrocardiogram; EEG, electroencephalogram; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; IL-6, interleukin 6; LPP, late positive potential; MRS, magnetic resonance spectroscopy; N2, P3 and P3b, event-related potential components; NIRS, near-infrared spectroscopy; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PAG, periacqueductal gray; PET/SPECT, positron emission tomography and/or single-photon emission computed tomography; PFC, prefrontal cortex; pgACC, pregenual ACC; rTMS/TMS, repetitive high frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation and/or transcranial magnetic stimulation; sgACC, subgenual ACC; SII, secondary somatosensory cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; SMA, supplementary motor area; vACC, ventral ACC; vlPFC, ventrolateral PFC; vmPFC, ventromedial PFC; vPFC, ventral PFC.

Figure 4

Table 3 Measures of non-physical pain in human studies (n = 92)

Figure 5

Table 4 Terms and definitions of non-physical pain retrieved in human studies (n = 86)

Figure 6

Table A1 Challenges and ways to improve research on the biological basis of non-physical pain

Supplementary material: File

Duranté et al. supplementary material

Duranté et al. supplementary material
Download Duranté et al. supplementary material(File)
File 82.1 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.