Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:39:13.998Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authors' reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 December 2018

Scott Weich
Affiliation:
Head of the Mental Health Research Unit, Honorary Consultant Psychiatrist and Professor of Mental Health, School of Health and Related Research, University of Sheffield, UK Email: [email protected]
Craig Duncan
Affiliation:
Senior Research Fellow, Department of Geography, University of Portsmouth, UK
Sarah-Jane Fenton
Affiliation:
Lecturer in Mental Health Policy, Institute for Mental Health, Warwick University, UK
Graham Moon
Affiliation:
Professor of Spatial Analysis in Human Geography, School of Geography and Environment, University of Southampton, UK
Swaran Singh
Affiliation:
Head of Mental Health and Wellbeing, Warwick Medical School, Warwick University, UK
Kamaldeep Bhui
Affiliation:
Professor of Cultural Psychiatry and Epidemiology, Centre for Psychiatry, Barts and The London School of Medicine & Dentistry, Queen Mary University, UK
Liz Twigg
Affiliation:
Professor of Human Geography, Department of Geography, University of Portsmouth, UK
Jason Madan
Affiliation:
Professor in Health Economics, Warwick Clinical Trials Unit and Director of Postgraduate Research, Warwick Medical School, Warwick University, UK
David Crepaz-Keay
Affiliation:
Head of Empowerment and Social Inclusion, Mental Health Foundation, UK
Helen Parsons
Affiliation:
Senior Research Fellow, Warwick Medical School, Warwick University, UK.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2018 

We would like to thank Professor Burns for his thoughtful reply to our recent editorial and we are grateful for this opportunity to respond. To clarify: we would be happy to see more RCTs in psychiatry, but only as one form of evidence among others. Interestingly, the same work of Karl Popper referred to in the reply is drawn on by a leading proponent of realism to support such a position.Reference Pawson1

Professor Burns gives two examples of RCTs of complex interventions to demonstrate their value. Our view of the implications of these trials is, unsurprisingly, different. We find it hard to believe that assertive community treatment teams and community treatment orders are not effective for anyone, anywhere, or in any way. And although we agree with Professor Burns that the scarcity of trials evidence is problematic – in the case of community treatment orders, there have only been three RCTs with a total sample size of 749 patientsReference Kisely and Hall2 – we also believe that RCTs alone will never be the whole answer.

Rather than privileging a method designed to estimate singular ‘average treatment effects’ and whether a treatment does or does not ‘work’, we would argue that a more sensible way to proceed is to develop approaches intrinsically attuned to detecting variation and difference and, most importantly, understanding what gives rise to it.Reference Subramanian, Kim and Christakis3 Where RCTs design out the effects of context, realist approaches see this as key.

We agree that other medical and healthcare specialities rely on evidence for the effectiveness of complex interventions. But what distinguishes mental health is the preponderance of interventions that require human agency, and factors such as therapeutic alliance, empathic communication and motivation: the relationship between community treatment orders and readmission rates is of a different complexity than that between chemotherapy and cancer remission, or between digitalis and cardiac function.

We acknowledge, and celebrate, the contribution of RCTs to evidence-based healthcare. But there remains a need for a plurality of methods. However astute and research-literate the clinician, RCTs select participants in ways that can make generalisation to real-world settings difficult. Realist approaches that help bridge the gap between the ‘what’ and the ‘how’ of clinical outcomes can only be a good thing. And the more complex the intervention – and the more context dependent – the more important this is. For us, RCTs alone are unlikely to be sufficient.

Parity of esteem for psychiatry is undoubtedly worthwhile, but this does not mean we have to imitate other specialities; as so often in the past, we can lead the way instead. Primus inter pares.

References

1Pawson, R. The Science of Evaluation. Sage, 2013.Google Scholar
2Kisely, S, Hall, K. An updated meta-analysis of randomized controlled evidence for the effectiveness of community treatment orders. Can J Psychiatry 2014; 59: 561–4.Google Scholar
3Subramanian, SV, Kim, R, Christakis, NA. The “average” treatment effect: a construct ripe for retirement. A commentary on Deaton and Cartwright. Soc Sci Med 2018; 210: 7782.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.