Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T08:58:11.179Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Aggressive incidents in first-episode psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

John Milton*
Affiliation:
East Midlands Centre for Forensic Mental Health, Leicester
Shazad Amin
Affiliation:
Moorside, Trafford General Hospital, Manchester
Swaran P. Singh
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham, Nottingham
Glynn Harrison
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Bristol, Bristol
Peter Jones
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
Tim Croudace
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
Ian Medley
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
John Brewin
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
*
Dr John Milton, East Midlands Centre for Forensic Mental Health, Arnold Lodge, Cordelia Close, Leicester LE5 0LE, UK. Tel: 0116 225 6060; Fax: 0116 225 6061; e-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Recent research has reported increased risk of aggressive incidents by individuals with psychotic illness.

Aims

To examine acts of aggression in first-episode psychosis.

Method

Subjects with a first-episode psychosis were ascertained from a defined catchment area (Nottingham, UK) and reassessed at 3 years (n=166) using clinical interview, informants, health care and forensic records.

Results

Of the subjects, 9.6% demonstrated at least one act of serious aggression (defined as weapon use, sexual assault or victim injury) during at least one psychotic episode and 23.5% demonstrated lesser acts of aggression (defined as all other acts of aggression). For all aggressive subjects (33.1%), unemployment (OR=3.6, 95%CI 1.6–8.0), comorbid substance misuse (OR=3.1, CI 1.1–8.8) and symptoms of overactivity at service contact (OR=6.9, CI 2.7–17.8) had independent effects on risk of aggression.

Conclusions

We confirmed some previously reported demographic and clinical associations with aggression in first-episode psychosis but no relationship with specific psychotic symptoms or diagnostic groups was observed.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2001 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

The renaissance in the psychiatric epidemiology of violence suggests that individuals with mental disorder may be two or three times more likely to behave violently than controls (Reference Brennan, Mednick and HodginsBrennan et al, 2000). Those diagnosed with psychotic illness can be at higher risk of harming others (Reference Swanson, Monahan and SteadmanSwanson, 1994) and acquiring a criminal conviction for violence (Reference Wessely, Castle and DouglasWessely et al, 1994). Schizophrenia (Reference Tiihonen, Isohanni and RasanenTiihonen et al, 1997) and substance misuse disorders (Reference Swanson, Holzer and GanjuSwanson et al, 1990) have been particularly implicated. Evidence regarding the relationship between specific symptoms and violence is conflicting; some reports link violence by individuals experiencing psychosis with persecutory delusions associated with passivity (so-called ‘threat-control-override (TCO) syndrome’; Reference Link, Stueve, Monahan and SteadmanLink & Stueve, 1994), whereas others find no increase in the overall risk of violence due to delusions (Reference Appelbaum, Robbins and MonahamAppelbaum et al, 2000). Several UK inquiries into homicides by people with mental illness, usually psychosis (Reference Ritchie, Dick and LinghamRitchie et al, 1994), have raised the awareness of risk management (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1996). Hence, there is considerable interest in identifying factors that are associated with and predict aggressive behaviour towards others by individuals with psychosis.

This study aimed to examine aggressive acts associated with an epidemiologically defined cohort of patients with a first-episode psychosis presenting to secondary psychiatric services and followed up 3 years later. Our objectives were:

  1. (a) to estimate the frequency and severity of aggression towards others during specified time periods;

  2. (b) to ascertain the characteristics at episode onset associated with subjects demonstrating aggression as compared with non-aggressive subjects, and to confirm previously reported demographic and clinical associations with violence by individuals with psychosis.

METHOD

Sample

The sample comprised 168 consecutive patients (aged 16-64 years) with a psychotic illness making first contact with psychiatric services in Nottingham (UK) between 1 June 1992 and 31 May 1994 (from a population of 397 148). All subjects met the criteria in the Screening Schedule for Psychosis (Reference Jablensky, Sartorius and ErnbergJablensky et al, 1992). Descriptions of case ascertainment and demographic characteristics of this cohort (including determination of ethnic origin) have been reported previously (Reference Brewin, Cantwell and DalkinBrewin et al, 1997; Reference Harrison, Glazebrook and BrewinHarrison et al, 1997).

Follow-up procedure

Follow-up assessments took place between June 1995 and May 1997, as close as possible to 3 years after first contact. One hundred and forty-three direct interviews (86%) were completed, with the remainder relying on other clinical material. We obtained informed consent to interview subjects, to examine their records and to interview their informal carers.

Assessment variables

Diagnosis

The diagnostic process is described by Brewin et al (Reference Brewin, Cantwell and Dalkin1997). Briefly, symptoms were derived from ratings of the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN) interview (World Health Organization, 1994), supplemented by other record sources and informant information. Where direct interview was impossible, other information sources, mainly hospital notes, were rated using the Item Group Checklist (IGC; World Health Organization, 1994), according to SCAN rules. When all data had been collected, meetings were held between fieldworkers and the authors G.H. and I.M. and consensus diagnoses were made using ICD—10 (World Health Organization, 1992).

Personality was evaluated using the Personality Assessment Schedule (PAS; Reference Tyrer, Alexander and TyrerTyrer & Alexander, 1988) and clinical consensus Axis II diagnoses for ICD—10.

Symptoms

We re-coded 17 of the items in the IGC into a binary format (presence of a symptom, rating>1). Items were chosen to reflect symptoms implicated in violent behaviour as best evidence from previous research. These items then were divided into three symptom subgroups: psychotic, behaviour and ‘threat-control-override’ (TCO) (see Appendix). Patients were placed in ‘high’ or ‘low’ categories, dichotomised by median scores in each subgroup.

Mode of illness onset (speed of evolution of symptoms) was estimated retrospectively using the Operational Criteria Checklist (OPCRIT; Reference McGuffin, Farmer and HarveyMcGuffin et al, 1991), broadly categorised as ‘acute’ if less than 1 month or ‘insidious’ if greater than 6 months. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Reference AndreasenAndreasen, 1982) was also administered.

Acts of aggression

Source of data. Offending behaviour (self-reported violence, previous arrests, convictions and imprisonment for violence) was recorded from the Psychiatric and Personal History Schedule (Reference Jablensky, Sartorius and ErnbergJablensky et al, 1992), follow-up assessments, psychiatric, community mental health team and general practitioner records and legal reports where available. Three-year follow-up assessment included questions about the nature and frequency of any aggression; the dates of such episodes were obtained exclusively from records. Not all patients consented to access to official offending data, such as Criminal Records Office information.

‘Untoward incident’ forms (completed by staff following acts of physical disturbance by in-patients, day or out-patients) were obtained for the study period. These forms also recorded when subjects were absent without leave from in-patient wards.

Definition and classification. Acts of aggression were classified according to severity and timing within an individual's illness episode.

  1. (a) Severity of aggression was classified by adapting the two-point scale from the MacArthur Community Violence Instrument (Reference Steadman, Mulvey and MonahanSteadman et al, 1998). Serious aggression (level 2) was defined as weapon use or threat, sexual assault or any other violence with injury to a victim. Lesser aggression (level 1) included all other acts. We considered that verbal threats, without an accompanying weapon, were likely to be recalled unreliably by subjects or informants and not recorded by staff in every case and therefore were not included. These definitions were adopted a priori because the severity of such behaviour was classified retrospectively.

  2. (b) Timing of aggression was categorised into three periods (see Fig. 1). Illness onset was defined as the date of first change of behaviour that was attributable to illness, and not necessarily the date of first psychotic symptoms. Contact with psychiatric services was defined as the date of first contact with secondary psychiatric services with a psychotic illness. Period B therefore represents the clinical ‘best estimate’ of the duration of untreated illness (DUI). The end of period C was defined as 3 years (to the nearest month for 95% of cases) after the date of contact with psychiatric services.

Fig. 1 Critical time periods.

Other variables

The Mental Illness Needs Index (MINI; Reference Glover, Robin and EmamiGlover et al, 1998) was determined from each subject's postcode and dichotomised into proxy measures of low (>110) or high (<110) deprivation scores.

Data analysis

Demographic and clinical characteristics were reported using proportions, means or medians according to their measurement level (discrete or continuous) and distribution (normal or skewed).

Stepwise logistic regression analyses were used to identify clinical and demographic characteristics associated with aggression. Separate analyses were conducted for all acts of aggression and for serious aggression. For each model, odds ratios with 95% confidence limits were reported for variables meeting the entry criteria. The 11 independent variables (chosen for their previously reported or hypothesised association with aggression and mental disorder) were age, gender, employment, history of violent convictions, MINI score, diagnosis at first contact, comorbid substance misuse diagnosis, total symptom score, psychotic symptom score, behaviour symptom score and TCO score.

RESULTS

Sample

Two of the 168 patients who entered the study were excluded: one had an organic psychosis and one withdrew consent for the study. Detailed analysis is therefore presented for 166 subjects. Socio-demographic characteristics and ICD diagnoses have been described for onset (Reference Brewin, Cantwell and DalkinBrewin et al, 1997; Reference Harrison, Glazebrook and BrewinHarrison et al, 1997) and follow-up (Reference Amin, Singh and BrewinAmin et al, 1999).

Details of aggressive acts

Details of timing and severity of all aggressive acts are presented in Fig. 2 and Table 1. A description of victims of all assaults committed ‘in episode’ is shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 2 Subjects exhibiting aggression per time period (number and % within groups).

Fig. 3 Victims of all assaults by subjects in episode.

Table 1 Number of subjects exhibiting aggressive behaviour by type, timing and severity (n=166)

Type of aggressive act1 Period A (pre-illness) Period B (onset to contact) Period C (post-contact) Total no. of subjects
No. of subjects No. of episodes No. of subjects No. of episodes No. of subjects No. of episodes
Total 16 20 28 42 35 104 61
Serious 10 14 8 15 8 10 21
Less serious 6 6 20 27 27 94 40

Fifty-five (33.1%) subjects were responsible for 166 aggressive acts towards others during periods B and C, some subjects being responsible for several incidents within one time period (e.g. two subjects were responsible for 44 of the aggressive incidents during period C). Of these, 16 subjects (9.6% of the total sample or 29.1% of the aggressive group) demonstrated at least one act of serious aggression in periods B and/or C and 4 (2.4%) exhibited aggressive behaviour in all time periods. All but one of the 35 subjects who were aggressive during period C were judged by investigators to be ill at the time of such incidents.

For 20 subjects (71.4% of the subjects exhibiting aggression in period B) aggression at presentation led directly to psychiatric contact, and in 18 cases the police were involved (10.8% of the total sample).

Demographic characteristics of subjects exhibiting any aggression towards others while in episode compared with non-aggressive subjects are shown in Table 2 and their clinical attributes in Table 3. Other features characteristic of subjects who exhibited aggression after service contact were high rates of absconding from in-patient care (34.3%, OR=16.6, 95% CI 4.9-55.9) and increased compulsory admissions over the following 3 years (mean number of sections=2.2 and s.d.=1.5 v. 0.11 and 0.32 for non-aggressive subjects; t=-2.4, P=0.017).

Table 2 Demographic and diagnostic characteristics of subjects aggressive during period B (pre-contact) and period C (post-contact) compared with non-aggressive subject group

Variable Non-aggressive group (n=111) Aggression during illness group (some subjects are included during both periods B and C)
% of 166 subjects Pre-contact (period B) (n=28) Post-contact (period C) (n=35) Total n=55 (aggression v. non-aggression) OR (95% CI)
% OR (95% CI) % (95% CI)
Gender
Male 53.2 78.6 3.2* (1.2-8.6) 68.6 1.9 (0.9-4.3) 2.1* (1.1-4.3)
Ethnicity
White 74.8 67.9 0.7 (0.3-1.8) 68.6 0.7 (0.3-1.7) 0.8 (0.4-1.5)
African—Caribbean 17.1 21.4 1.3 (0.5-3.7) 28.6 1.9 (0.8-4.7) 1.6 (0.8-3.6)
Other 7.2 10.7 1.5 (0.4-6.2) 2.9 0.4 (0.5-3.1) 0.7 (0.2-2.9)
Unemployed 46.4 75.0 3.5** (1.4-8.8) 80.0 4.6** (1.9-11.5) 3.7** (1.8-7.7)
Single 56.8 78.6 2.8* (1.1-7.4) 74.3 2.2 (0.95-5.1) 2.2* (1.1-4.6)
Living alone 28.9 21.4 0.6 (0.3-1.8) 31.4 1.1 (0.5-2.6) 0.8 (0.4-1.6)
Age (mean, s.d.)1 32.9 (12.3) 26.5 (9.1)** 29.0 (5.9)** 26.6 (7.9)**
Diagnosis
Schizophrenia 34.2 35.7 1.2 (0.4-2.5) 37.1 1.1 (0.5-2.5) 1.2 (0.6-2.3)
Other psychosis 65.8 64.3 0.9 (0.4-2.2) 62.9 0.8 (0.4-1.9) 0.8 (0.4-1.6)
Any compulsory admissions 32.4 67.9 4.4** (1.8-10.7) 85.7 12.5** (4.5-35.0) 6.7** (3.2-14.1)
Any convictions 9.0 32.1 4.8** (1.7-13.3) 30.3 4.3** (1.6-11.8) 4.4** (1.8-10.5)
Previous violent convictions 4.5 25.0 7.1** (2.0-24.4) 22.9 6.8** (2.0-23.5) 5.6** (1.8-16.9)
Any aggression 5.4 25.0 5.8** (1.8-19.1) 26.5 6.3** (2.1-19.3) 5.0** (1.8-14.2)
Prior imprisonment 5.4 21.4 4.8* (1.4-16.2) 22.9 5.2** (1.7-16.2) 4.4** (1.5-12.6)

Table 3 Clinical variables for subjects aggressive during period B (pre-contact) and period C (post-contact) individually compared with non-aggressive subject group

Variable Non-aggressive group (n=111) % Aggression during illness group (some subjects were aggressive during both periods B and C)
Pre-contact (period B) (n=28) Post-contact (period C) (n=35) Total (n=55) (non-aggressive v. aggressive) OR (95% CI)
% OR (95% CI) % OR (95% CI)
Illness variables
Mode of illness onset
Insidious (> 6 months) 27.0 42.9 2.0 (0.9-4.8) 22.9 0.8 (0.3-2.0) 1.2 (0.6-2.5)
Single illness episode 36.8 28.6 0.7 (0.3-1.7) 17.1 0.4* (0.1-0.9) 0.6 (0.3-1.2)
Duration of untreated illness (median, days)1 75 136 68 85
Substance misuse variables
ICD-10 drug-related psychosis at onset (Flx), n=13 9.0 7.4 0.8 (0.2-4.0) 6.1 0.7 (0.1-3.2) 0.8 (0.2-2.9)
Flx additional ICD-10 diagnosis (‘dual diagnosis’), n=15 4.0 11.1 3.0 (0.6-14.3) 27.3 9.0** (2.6-31.7) 6.4** (1.9-21.4)
Symptom variables (from the IGC)
High IG total symptom score 34.2 57.1 2.6* (1.1-6.0) 45.7 1.6 (0.8-3.5) 1.9 (0.96-3.6)
High IG behaviour score 54.1 85.7 5.1** (1.7-15.7) 82.9 4.1** (1.6-10.7) 3.6** (1.8-8.3)
High IG psychotic score 54.1 32.1 0.4 (0.2-1.0) 48.6 0.8 (0.4-1.7) 0.7 (0.3-1.3)
High IG TCO score 63.1 32.1 0.3** (0.1-0.7) 54.3 0.7 (0.3-1.5) 0.5* (0.3-0.9)

For the 29.1% of aggressive subjects who exhibited serious aggression while ill, a comparison is shown in Table 4 with subjects exhibiting less serious aggression (only positive statistical or relevant associations are shown).

Table 4 Comparison between serious and less serious aggression groups while ill

Variable Serious aggression to others (n=16) Less serious aggression to others (n=39) Odds ratio
Demographic and aggression variables
Age (mean, s.d.)1 28.9(11.1) 28.8(6.0)
Any pre-illness aggression, % (n=10) 31.3 12.8 3.1 (0.8-12.7)
Any serious pre-illness aggression, % (n=7) 25.0 7.7 4.0 (0.8-20.5)
Any pre-illness convictions, % (n=16/53) 60.0 18.4 6.6** (1.8-24.8)
Any pre-illness violent convictions, % (n=11/53) 46.7 10.5 7.4** (1.7-31.7)
Clinical variables
Mode of illness onset: insidious 56.3 20.5 5.0* (1.4-17.5)
(> 6 months), % (n=17)
Duration of untreated illness, days
Mean (s.d.)2 613(954) 227(620)
Median 226 76
High IG symptom score, % (n=27) 56.3 46.2 1.5 (0.5-4.8)
High IG behaviour score, % (n=45) 68.8 87.2 0.3 (0.1-1.3)
High IG psychotic score, % (n=24) 25.0 51.3 0.3 (0.1-1.2)
High IG TCO score, % (n=25) 25.0 53.8 0.3 (0.1-1.0)

Other than the characteristics shown in Table 4, demographic or clinical features failed to discriminate between lesser and serious aggression. Although more subjects committing serious aggression while ill but before contact had a slow mode of illness onset compared with those committing less serious aggression during the same period (6/8 v. 6/20, OR=7.0, 95% CI 1.1-45.2, P<0.05), their DUI was not significantly longer (median 554 days v. 128 days, Mann-Whitney U=48.0, P=0.10).

Symptoms at baseline

Psychotic symptoms such as delusions, hallucinations and passivity phenomena were common at service contact in both aggressive and non-aggressive groups with no statistically significant differences. For example, persecutory delusions were noted in 71.2% of non-aggressive subjects and in 83.3% of subjects who exhibited an aggressive act, with 19.8% of non-aggressive subjects describing delusions of passivity compared with 12.7% of aggressive subjects. A summary of IGC symptom group findings is shown in Table 3.

Comparisons of positive scores on individual item groups (IG) from the IGC, recorded at service contact, between subjects who were or were not aggressive after service contact (period C) showed that the aggressive group were significantly more likely to exhibit ‘overactivity’ (IG19: OR=2.2, 95% CI 1.1-4.5), ‘emotional turmoil’ (IG31: OR=2.9, 95% CI 1.4-5.6), ‘socially embarrassing behaviour’ (IG34: OR=4.0, 95% CI 1.7-9.7) and ‘problems in non-verbal communication’ (IG37: OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.1-6.0). Subjects exhibiting aggression during period B were also distinguished by symptoms relating to ‘overactivity’ (IG19: OR=2.8, 95% CI 1.2-6.5), ‘emotional turmoil’ (IG31: OR=4.1, 95% CI 1.7-9.5), ‘socially embarrassing behaviour’ (IG34: OR=3.2, 95% CI 1.2-8.0) and ‘problems in non-verbal communication’ (IG37: OR=3.1, 95% CI 1.3-7.6) as well as ‘self-neglect’ (IG38: OR=3.5, 95% CI 1.3-9.5), but were less likely to exhibit delusions of persecution (IG30: OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.1-5.0), delusions of reference (IG29: OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.25-5.0) or bizarre delusions (IG27: OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.1-5.0).

Seriously aggressive subjects were also characterised by ‘overactivity’ (IG19: OR=3.4, 95% CI 1.2-9.8) and ‘deficits in non-verbal communication’ (IG37: OR=5.5, 95% CI 1.9-16.2) and were less likely to exhibit bizarre delusions (IG27: OR=5.0 95% CI 1.4-10.0). There was no significant difference between total SANS scores for aggressive and non-aggressive subjects (t=-0.37, d.f.=147, P=0.71).

Substance misuse

No data on substance misuse were available for 14 (8.4%) cases. Thirteen (8.6% of 152) subjects had primary substance-related psychoses (ICD—10 F1x) and 15 (9.9%) other subjects had ICD—10 comorbid substance misuse and psychotic disorders, mostly illicit drugs (93.3%) rather than alcohol. Comparisons between substance misuse variables for aggressive and non-aggressive subjects are shown in Table 3.

Personality

The PAS data were unavailable for 74 (44%) subjects and therefore were not evaluated. Consensus F60 personality disorders were recorded for six subjects: one schizoid, one emotionally unstable and four dissocial personality disorders. No statistical associations were found between those with comorbid personality disorder and aggression.

Multivariate analyses

Two multiple logistic regression models were used to assess the risk of subjects exhibiting either any aggression or serious aggression while in episode. Of the 11 independent variables, age, gender, diagnosis at first contact, history of violent convictions, MINI score, total symptom severity score and psychotic symptom score did not meet the entry criteria. A high behaviour symptom group score (OR=6.9, 95% CI 2.7-17.8), comorbid substance misuse diagnosis (OR=3.1, 95% CI 1.1-8.8) and being unemployed (OR=3.6, 95% CI 1.6-8.0) all had an independent effect on the risk of exhibiting any aggression. A high TCO score had an independent effect on reduced risk of any aggression (OR=2.8, 95% CI 1.3-6.3).

For the model of serious aggression while in episode, a history of convictions for violence (OR=12.6, 95% CI 3.5-45.1) was the only independent variable to meet the entry criteria other than high TCO score, which was negatively associated with risk (OR=4.1, 95% CI 1.2-14.7).

DISCUSSION

This study utilised an epidemiologically defined cohort of patients diagnosed with first-episode psychosis presenting to secondary psychiatric services to examine the relationship between first-episode psychosis and aggression.

Methodological strengths and limitations

Criticisms of previous studies examining the relationship between violence and mental disorder have been described in the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment (MVRA) Study (Reference Steadman, Monahan, Appelbaum, Monahan and SteadmanSteadman et al, 1994).

Briefly, there are three types of problem: use of ‘static’ predictor variables (usually socio-demographic variables) rather than dynamic (symptom profile or timing of violent behaviour) variables; narrowly defined patient samples — in-patients (Reference Raja, Azzoni and LubichRaja et al, 1997), ‘high risk’ discharges from secure psychiatric or prison facilities (Reference Rice and HarrisRice & Harris, 1995) or single diagnostic groups (Reference Humphreys, Johnstone and MacMillanHumphreys et al, 1992) and data-sets restricted to individuals arrested or re-admitted to hospital (Reference Wessely, Castle and DouglasWessely et al, 1994; Reference Tiihonen, Isohanni and RasanenTiihonen et al, 1997). This study therefore sought to use a representative first-episode community-based sample, a broad diagnostic range of psychotic disorders and multiple data sources.

Our findings have several limitations. First, and of greatest hindrance to conclusions about prevalence, is the absence of a non-psychosis comparison group representative of the population from which our subjects were taken.

Second, the overall study was not designed primarily to obtain data on aggression or offending. Because difficulties with confidentiality and consent were encountered in accessing statutory criminological data, an incomplete criminological data-set probably reduced the prevalence of reported offending. Although the use of multiple data sources (e.g. informant interviews or psychiatric records) has been shown to increase significantly the reported prevalence of violence (Reference Steadman, Mulvey and MonahanSteadman et al, 1998), our findings still have limitations. For example, even in prospective studies such as the MVRA Study, subject attrition can be high, leading to restrictions in collection and analysis of violence-related data. Sources such as psychiatric case notes have been shown to underestimate convictions for violence among mentally disordered offenders by almost 50% in a UK sample of homicide perpetrators (Reference Shaw, Appleby and AmosShaw et al, 1999). Additionally, use of ‘untoward incident’ records may bias the reporting of aggressive incidents after service contact and in in-patient settings: either overreporting due to individuals' increased exposure to the scrutiny of health care staff or underreporting because aggression may be accepted by staff as symptomatic of mental disorder.

Third, modifications made to the IGC to allow comparison of symptoms across the whole cohort have yet to be validated as a means of assessing symptom clusters. However, the SCAN interview is widely used and good reliability between raters was demonstrated for this study (Reference Brewin, Cantwell and DalkinBrewin et al, 1997). We chose particular item groups and then adopted sub-scales based upon established empirical evidence for their association with violence. This approach has face validity although it could be criticised for focusing on particular target symptoms, thus biasing findings towards certain symptoms at the expense of other previously unreported but potentially important symptom areas.

Lastly, because the study was not conceived to ascertain clinical and contextual variables at the precise time of all aggressive acts, it was not always possible to elicit retrospectively (either from patients or informants) the precise motives after illness onset, although most attributed aggression after service contact to illness. Also, only those patients whose pre-contact aggression led to immediate admission received a SCAN interview temporally related to the incident. For the remaining subjects, we drew conclusions from SCAN interviews shortly after service contact about the relationship between psychopathology at service contact and at times of aggression. It could be argued also that the differentiation of illness episode into specific time periods is arbitrary, particularly when change in behaviour (rather than emergence of first psychotic symptom) was the criterion used. This could be relevant if different risk factors for aggression prevail at difference time periods of illness.

Type and frequency of aggression towards others

Approximately one-third of subjects exhibited aggressive behaviour during periods B and C and one-sixth after the onset of symptoms but prior to service contact. Comparisons with other UK samples are limited by diagnostic heterogeneity. However, the proportion of subjects with schizophrenia exhibiting aggression prior to contact (10/54 or 18.5%) is similar to another UK study (21%) (Reference Humphreys, Johnstone and MacMillanHumphreys et al, 1992) but less than that described for developed countries (31.5%) in the World Health Organization's study on the Determinants of Outcome of Severe Mental Disorders (DOSMD; Reference Volavka, Laska and BakerVolavka et al, 1997).

Aggressive acts were more common after service contact. Despite this, only four (2.4%) subjects demonstrated aggression in all the time periods studied (i.e. premorbidly and after illness). Serious aggression was more prevalent before onset of illness and diminished in frequency in proportion to less serious aggression after onset and contact with secondary services. However, the overall number of subjects committing lesser acts of aggression increased. Although this may relate in part to reporting bias after service contact, further analysis of the timing of violent events relative to symptoms and illness course is required.

Our findings regarding the victims of aggression by subjects with psychosis are in broad agreement with the MVRA Study (Reference Steadman, Mulvey and MonahanSteadman et al, 1998), where family and acquaintances form the majority of victims. Further study of the location, social context and social networks of subjects with psychosis in relation to aggression is needed.

Diagnosis, symptoms and violence

Excluding substance misuse (ICD—10 F1x) comorbidity, diagnostic group alone did not confer any special risk of aggression, regardless of aggression type or severity. This contrasts with other studies of serious offending, which report an increased risk for schizophrenia of both homicide (Reference Eronen, Tiihonen and HakolaEronen et al, 1996) and violent conviction (Reference Tiihonen, Isohanni and RasanenTiihonen et al, 1997).

In agreement with recent findings from the MVRA Study (Reference Appelbaum, Robbins and MonahamAppelbaum et al, 2000), we did not confirm previous reports linking specific psychotic symptoms and aggression. We found that although delusions, hallucinations and passivity phenomena were common, they did not specifically distinguish subjects who were aggressive, either in isolation or as the TCO syndrome. We also confirmed Appelbaum et al's (Reference Appelbaum, Robbins and Monaham2000) finding that higher TCO scores at baseline assessment were inversely related to the risk of further aggression, serious or otherwise. Such findings, relating to an inception rather than a discharge cohort, may add weight to Appelbaum et al's conclusions that “presumed risk of violence associated with delusions per se does not justify hospitalisation of a patient, in the absence of other indicators of violence risk or of other reasons for inpatient treatment”.

Symptoms that were associated with aggression and present at service contact were apparent in two areas. First, behavioural symptoms, such as overactivity and emotional turmoil, had large and independent effects on risk of aggression. This has some precedence in the symptoms of excitement found in acute-onset disorders during the DOSMD study (Reference Volavka, Laska and BakerVolavka et al, 1997). Second, symptoms related to aspects of negative symptoms and deficits in communication appeared to be characteristic of subjects, mainly younger men with insidious illness onset, who exhibited serious aggression towards others and specifically after illness onset and before service contact. However, higher global ratings of negative symptoms were not characteristic of these subjects.

In addition to insidious illness onset and past history of convictions for violence, subjects with psychosis who demonstrated serious aggression also had longer periods of untreated illness. Duration of untreated illness has attracted attention as an important factor associated with outcome across a number of domains (Reference McGorry, Krstev and HarriganMcGorry et al, 2000). Although the association with aggression could provide opportunities for reducing the DUI in order to diminish indirectly the risk of aggression, the relationship may be an artefact. Because the length of period B varied considerably (from a few days to over 10 years), it is possible that the connection between the DUI and aggression represents a longer period at risk.

Only a small number of subjects received any Axis II diagnosis. Our findings are likely to represent an underestimation of personality disorder, owing to the absence of interview- or informant-based personality data for all subjects.

Substance misuse and violence

Previous research has identified a relationship between substance misuse and violence, either alone or in combination with major mental disorder (Reference Swanson, Holzer and GanjuSwanson et al, 1990; Reference Tiihonen, Isohanni and RasanenTiihonen et al, 1997; Reference Steadman, Mulvey and MonahanSteadman et al, 1998). In a UK sample, Scott et al (Reference Scott, Johnson and Menezes1998) reported that individuals with comorbid psychosis and substance misuse are more likely to exhibit hostile behaviour and assaults than those with psychosis alone.

We confirmed that ICD—10 F1x diagnostic comorbidity, but not primary drug-related psychosis, at onset had an independent effect on the risk of subjects exhibiting aggression during a psychotic episode. Individuals with drug-related comorbidity were nine times more likely to exhibit aggression after service contact. Whether this represents reporting bias and how such individuals' substance misuse covaries with their psychotic disorder over time requires further study. Comorbid alcohol misuse was not a significant association for violence, as reported in the DOSMD study (Reference Volavka, Laska and BakerVolavka et al, 1997).

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  • Within a community-based first-episode psychosis cohort 33.1% of the subjects exhibited aggression, predominantly minor acts, towards family members.

  • Comorbid substance misuse and overexcited behaviour (but not psychotic symptoms) were independently associated with subjects perpetrating aggressive acts.

  • A history of convictions for violence was characteristic of the 9.6% of subjects committing acts of serious aggression.

LIMITATIONS

  • The absence of a comparison group of individuals without psychosis with similar socio-demographic characteristics limits the empirical utility of our findings.

  • Underreporting of aggressive or offending behaviour may have occurred, owing to incomplete statutory criminological data.

  • The use of symptom subgroups has yet to be validated as a means of assessing symptom clusters in relation to aggressive acts.

APPENDIX

Scores of Symptom Severity

The following 17 items (including all psychotic symptom items) are taken from the first 41 item groups of the Item Group Checklist (World Health Organization, 1994) as being the most relevant to violent behaviour (based upon previous reported research).

Total symptom severity rating

(maximum score=17, low=0-5, high=6+)

  • IG03 Autonomic anxiety and pani.

  • IG13 Depressive delusions and hallucination.

  • IG15 Heightened subjective functionin.

  • IG16 Rapid subjective temp.

  • IG18 Expansive delusions and hallucination.

  • IG19 Overactivit.

  • IG21 Non-specific auditory hallucination.

  • IG23 Non-specific psychoti.

  • IG24 Non-affective auditory hallucination.

  • IG25 Experience of disordered form of though.

  • IG26 Delusions of contro.

  • IG27 Bizarre delusions and interpretation.

  • IG28 Miscellaneous delusion.

  • IG29 Delusions of referenc.

  • IG30 Delusions of persecutio.

  • IG31 Emotional turmoil, etc.

  • IG34 Socially embarrassing behaviou.

Psychotic symptom subgroup

(maximum score=11, low=0-4, high=5+)

  • IG13 Depressive delusions and hallucination.

  • IG18 Expansive delusions and hallucination.

  • IG21 Non-specific auditory hallucination.

  • IG23 Non-specific psychoti.

  • IG24 Non-affective auditory hallucination.

  • IG25 Experience of disordered form of though.

  • IG26 Delusions of contro.

  • IG27 Bizarre delusions and interpretation.

  • IG28 Miscellaneous delusion.

  • IG29 Delusions of referenc.

  • IG30 Delusions of persecutio.

Behaviour symptom subgroup

(maximum score=6, low=0, high=1+)

  • IG03 Autonomic anxiety and pani.

  • IG15 Heightened subjective functionin.

  • IG16 Rapid subjective temp.

  • IG19 Overactivit.

  • IG31 Emotional turmoil, etc.

  • IG34 Socially embarrassing behaviou.

Threat-control-override (TCO) symptom subgroup

(maximum score=6, low=0-2, high=3+)

  • IG25 Experience of disordered form of though.

  • IG26 Delusions of contro.

  • IG27 Bizarre delusions and interpretation.

  • IG28 Miscellaneous delusion.

  • IG29 Delusions of referenc.

  • IG30 Delusions of persecutio.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank Mrs Rosemary Kwiecinski for her help with the assessments and administration of the database and Professor Conor Duggan for his comments on a draft of the paper.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

Support was received from the National Health Service Executive (Trent Research & Development).

References

Amin, S., Singh, S. P., Brewin, J., et al (1999) Diagnostic stability of first-episode psychosis. Comparison of ICD–10 and DSM–III–R systems. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 537543.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. (1982) The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Appelbaum, P., Robbins, P. & Monaham, J. (2000) Violence and delusions: data from the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 566572.Google Scholar
Brennan, P., Mednick, S. & Hodgins, S. (2000) Major mental disorders and criminal violence in a Danish birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 494500.Google Scholar
Brewin, J., Cantwell, R., Dalkin, T., et al (1997) Incidence of schizophrenia in Nottingham. A comparison of two cohorts, 1978–1980 and 1992–1994. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 140144.Google Scholar
Eronen, M., Tiihonen, J. & Hakola, P. (1996) Schizophrenia and homicidal behaviour. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 8389.Google Scholar
Glover, G., Robin, E., Emami, J., et al (1998) A needs index for mental health care. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 8996.Google Scholar
Harrison, G., Glazebrook, C., Brewin, J., et al (1997) Increased incidence of psychotic disorders in African-Caribbean migrants to the UK. Psychological Medicine, 27, 799806.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Johnstone, E. C., MacMillan, J. F., et al (1992) Dangerous behaviour preceding first admissions for schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 501505.Google Scholar
Jablensky, A., Sartorius, N., Ernberg, G., et al (1992) Schizophrenia: Manifestations, Incidence and Course in Different Cultures. A World Health Organization Ten Country Study. Psychological Medicine Monograph Supplement 20. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Link, B. & Stueve, A. (1994) Psychotic symptoms and the violent/illegal behaviour of mental patients compared to community controls. In Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment (eds Monahan, J. & Steadman, H.), pp. 135161. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McGorry, P. D., Krstev, H. & Harrigan, S. (2000) Early detection and treatment delay: implications for outcome in early psychosis. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 13, 3743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuffin, P., Farmer, A. & Harvey, I. (1991) A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 764770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raja, M., Azzoni, A. & Lubich, L. (1997) Aggressive and violent behaviour in a population of psychiatric inpatients. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 428434.Google Scholar
Rice, M. & Harris, G. (1995) Psychopathy, schizophrenia, alcohol abuse and violent recidivism. International Journal of Law Psychiatry, 18, 333342.Google Scholar
Ritchie, J., Dick, D. & Lingham, R. (1994) Report of the Inquiry into the Care and Treatment of Christopher Clunis. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Royal College of Psychiatrists (1996) Assessment and Clinical Management of Risk of Harm to Other People. (Council Report CR53). London: Royal College of Psychiatrists.Google Scholar
Scott, H., Johnson, S., Menezes, P., et al (1998) Substance misuse and risk of aggression and offending among the severely mentally ill. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 345350.Google Scholar
Shaw, J., Appleby, L., Amos, T., et al (1999) Mental disorder and clinical care in people convicted of homicide: national clinical survey. British Medical Journal, 318, 12401244.Google Scholar
Steadman, H., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P., et al (1994) Designing a new generation of risk assessment research. In Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk. Assessment (eds Monahan, J. & Steadman, H.), pp. 297318. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Steadman, H., Mulvey, E., Monahan, J., et al (1998) Violence by people discharged from acute psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 393401.Google Scholar
Swanson, J. (1994) Mental disorder, substance abuse and community violence: an epidemiological approach. In Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment (eds Monahan, J. & Steadman, H.), pp. 101136. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, J. Holzer, C., Ganju, V., et al (1990) Violence and psychiatric disorder in the community: evidence from the epidemiologic catchment area surveys. Hospital Community Psychiatry, 41, 761770.Google Scholar
Tiihonen, J., Isohanni, M., Rasanen, P., et al (1997) Specific major mental disorders and criminality: a 26 year prospective study of the 1966 Northern Finland birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 840845.Google Scholar
Tyrer, P. & Alexander, J. (1988) Personality Assessment Schedule. In Personality Disorders (ed. Tyrer, P.), pp. 4362. London: Wright.Google Scholar
Volavka, J., Laska, E., Baker, S., et al (1997) History of violent behaviour and schizophrenia in different cultures. Analyses based on the WHO study on Determinants of Outcome of Severe Mental Disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wessely, S., Castle, D., Douglas, A., et al (1994) The criminal careers of incident cases of schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 24, 483502.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992) Tenth Revision of the International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD–10). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1994) SCAN: Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (Version 2). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Critical time periods.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Subjects exhibiting aggression per time period (number and % within groups).

Figure 2

Fig. 3 Victims of all assaults by subjects in episode.

Figure 3

Table 1 Number of subjects exhibiting aggressive behaviour by type, timing and severity (n=166)

Figure 4

Table 2 Demographic and diagnostic characteristics of subjects aggressive during period B (pre-contact) and period C (post-contact) compared with non-aggressive subject group

Figure 5

Table 3 Clinical variables for subjects aggressive during period B (pre-contact) and period C (post-contact) individually compared with non-aggressive subject group

Figure 6

Table 4 Comparison between serious and less serious aggression groups while ill

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.