Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T20:48:48.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Usefulness of the Semantic Differential with ‘Mild Grade’ Mental Defectives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

M. G. T. Dow
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychology Department, Gartnavel Royal Hospital, Glasgow, G12
F. Ledwith
Affiliation:
St. Andrews University, St. Andrews
W. I. Fraser
Affiliation:
Lynebank Hospital, Dunfermline, Fife and Department of Rehabilitation Studies, University of Edinburgh
M. Bhagat
Affiliation:
Airedale General Hospital, Keighley, Yorkshire

Summary

Although the semantic differential has been quite widely used with mental defectives, its usefulness (in its traditional form) with such a population has not been conclusively demonstrated.

The semantic differential was developed and shown to be valid as an index of representational mediation processes. However, there is also evidence of a mediational deficiency among retardates which, therefore, ought to be reflected in retardates' responses to the semantic differential. Thus, the present study was designed to test two hypotheses: (a) that mild grade retardates, in comparison with subjects of average intelligence, would show less discrimination in the use of the semantic differential, and (b) that such discriminative deficiencies would in turn predispose toward a polarized response bias among the retarded.

The results, which supported both hypotheses, are discussed in terms of their implications for the usefulness of an unmodified semantic differential with the mentally subnormal.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1975 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Annual Report of the Registrar General for Scotland (1970) No. 116. Edinburgh: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Bhagat, M. & Fraser, W. I. (1970a). ‘Young offenders’ images of self and surroundings: a semantic enquiry.’ British Journal of Psychiatry, 117, 381–7.Google Scholar
Bhagat, M. & Fraser, W. I. (1970b) ‘The meaning of concepts to the retarded offender.’ American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 75, 260–7.Google Scholar
Bhagat, M. & Fraser, W. I. (1971) The effect of low intelligence on the emotional and environmental concepts of retarded offenders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 119, 639–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialer, I. (1961) Primary and secondary stimulus generalization as related to intelligence level. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 395402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bopp, J. (1955) A quantitative semantic analysis of word association in schizophrenia. Unpublished Doctor's dissertation, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Brod, D., Kernoff, P. & Terwilliger, R. F. (1964) Anxiety and semantic differential responses. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 570–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donahoe, J. W. (1961) Changes in meaning as a function of age. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 99, 23–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, D. A. & Baumeister, A. A. (1971) The use of verbal mediation in the retarded as a function of developmental level and response availability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 12, 95105.Google Scholar
Haagen, Ch. (1949) Synonymity, vividness, familiarity and association value ratings of 400 pairs of common adjectives. Journal of Psychology, 27, 453–63.Google ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A. R. (1971) The role of verbal mediation in mental development. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 118, 3970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A. R. & Rohwer, W. D. Jr., (1963a) Verbal mediation in paired associate serial learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 1, 346–52.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. & Rohwer, W. D. Jr., (1963b) The effect of verbal mediation on the learning and retention of paired-associates by retarded adults. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 68, 80–4.Google ScholarPubMed
Kerrick, J. (1956) The effects of manifest anxiety and IQ, on discrimination. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 136–8.Google Scholar
Kumata, H. & Schramm, W. (1956) A pilot study of cross-cultural meaning. Public Opinion Quarterly, 20, 229–38.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1960) The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behaviour. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare.Google Scholar
Macmillan, D. L. (1970) The facultative effect of verbal mediation on the learning of paired-associates by cultural familial retardates. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 74, 762–4.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M. & Sartorius, N. H. (1968) A contribution to the measurement of sexual attitudes: the semantic differential as a measure of sexual attitude in sexual deviations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 145, 441–51.Google Scholar
Milgram, N. A. (1967) Retention of mediation set in paired-associate learning of normal children and retardates. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 5, 341–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milgram, N. A. (1968) The effect of verbal mediation in paired-associate learning in trainable retardates. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 72, 518–24.Google ScholarPubMed
Osgood, C. E. & Luria, Z. (1954) A blind analysis of a case of multiple personality using the semantic differential. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49. 579–91.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E. Suci, G. J. & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957) The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana, Illinois.Google Scholar
Penney, R. K., Seim, R. & Peters, R. Dev. (1968) The mediatorial deficiency of mentally retarded children: I. The establishment of retardates’ mediational deficiency. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 72, 626–30.Google Scholar
Penney, R. K., Seim, R. & Peters, R. Dev. & Willows, D. M. (1970) Mediational deficiency of mentally retarded children: III. Effect of length of institutionalization. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 74, 780–3.Google Scholar
Rybolt, G. A. (1968) A factorial analysis of the semantic structures of retarded adolescents. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 72, 512–17.Google ScholarPubMed
Rybolt, G. A. (1969) Stability characteristics of the semantic differential responses of the mentally retarded. Psychological Reports, 24. 103–8.Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E. & Weaver, W. (1949) The Mathematical Theory of Communication. Urbana, Illinois.Google Scholar
Zeaman, D. & House, B.J. (1963) The role of attention in retardate discrimination learning. In Handbook of Mental Deficiency (ed. N. R. Ellis). New York.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.