Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:53:02.789Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Failed Leucotomy with Misplaced Cuts: A Clinico-anatomical Study of Two Cases

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Philip Evans*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, The National Hospital for Nervous Diseases, Queen Square, London, W.C.1

Extract

Failure of treatment does not necessarily mean inappropriate treatment; it must first be ascertained that the treatment has been carried out. Increasing attention is being paid to this question in regard to drug therapy with estimations of blood and urine levels. Patients who fail to get better after leucotomy may have been unsuitably chosen; they may also fail to get better because the aim of the operation, namely to sever the thalamo-frontal radiations, has not been achieved. Neuro-anatomical studies have long suggested that not only do many patients remain with these tracts intact following a blind operative approach but that there is also great variability in the size and position of the lesions produced, a consequence of unreliable surface markings, brain fibre elasticity, complicating haemorrhages and progressive gliosis. The blind approach is no ideal surgical method, since it results in a largely fortuitous lesion (Meyer and McLardy, 1948, 1949; Beck, McLardy, and Meyer, 1950; Eie, 1954; Meyer and Beck, 1954). There is even a gross discrepancy between the actual and desired point of insertion of the leucotome (Dax, 1943). Whilst the effects of misplaced cuts have been studied by these workers, one can only guess what proportion of clinical failures can be attributed to misplaced cuts and what proportion of successes follow misplaced cuts (Meyer and Beck, 1954). Appeals for further studies with post-mortem correlates have been made by these authors.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1971 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beck, E., McLardy, T., and Meyer, A. (1950). ‘Anatomical comments on psychosurgical procedures.’ J. ment. Sci., 96, 157–67.Google Scholar
Dax, E. C., and Radley Smith, E. J. (1943). ‘Early effects of prefrontal leucotomy on disturbed patients with mental illness of long duration.’ J. ment. Sci., 89, 182–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eie, N. (1954). ‘Macroscopical investigation of 29 brains submitted to frontal leucotomy.’ Acta psychiat. et neurol. Scand., Suppl. 90.Google Scholar
Gardiner, Q. (1968). ‘The need for drug monitoring in psychiatric practice.’ Brit. J. Psychiat., 114, 877–81.Google Scholar
McLardy, T., and Meyer, A. (1949). ‘Anatomical correlates of improvement after leucotomy.’ J. ment. Sci., 95, 182–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., and Beck, E. (1945). ‘Neuropathological problems arising from prefrontal leucotomy.’ J. ment. Sci., 91, 411–25.Google Scholar
Meyer, A., and McLardy, T. (1948). ‘Posterior cuts in prefrontal leucotomies.’ J. ment. Sci., 94, 555–64.Google Scholar
Meyer, A., and McLardy, T. (1949). ‘Clinico-anatomical studies of frontal lobe function based upon leucotomy material.’ J. ment. Sci., 95, 403–17.Google Scholar
Meyer, A., and Beck, E. (1954). Prefrontal leucotomy and Related Operations. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar
Partridge, M. (1950). Prefrontal Leucotomy. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Smith, M. (1970). Personal communication.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.