Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:55:44.597Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Delayed Auditory Feedback Some Effects on the Speech of Psychiatric Patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

F. G. Spear*
Affiliation:
United Sheffield Hospitals

Extract

Delayed Auditory Feedback (D.A.F.), sometimes known as delayed side-tone, is the name given to the technical process in which a subject's speech is recorded, delayed, amplified and returned to his ears through a headphone at such power as to prevent him hearing it through the normal channels of direct air and bone conduction. This process has been found to produce marked disturbance of speech, the main changes found being the production of an artificial stutter, marked slowing of speech, increase in loudness of speech and development of a flatness of intonation or decrease in vocal intensity variation (Lee, 1950/1951). The slowing and increase in intensity have been found by Black (1951) to be related to the variation in delay and amplification, but even when these factors are held constant there is considerable individual variation in the response to D.A.F.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1963 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beaumont, J. T., and Foss, B. M. (1957). “Individual differences in reacting to delayed auditory feedback”, Brit. J. Psychol., 48, 8589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellak, L. (1958). Schizophrenia: A Review of the Syndrome. New York: Logus Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, J. W. (1951). “The effects of delayed side-tone on vocal rate and intensity”, J. Speech. Hear. Dis., 16, 5563.Google Scholar
Brit. Med. J. (1958). “Delayed auditory feedback”. Leading Article, ii, 13981399.Google Scholar
Goldfarb, W., and Braunstein, P. (1958). “Responses to delayed auditory feedback in schizophrenic children”. In: Psychopathology of Communication, Ed. Hoch, P. H., and Zubin, J. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Kreitman, N., Sainsbury, P., Morrissey, J., Towers, J., and Scrivener, J. (1961). “The reliability of psychiatric assessment: an analysis”, J. Ment. Sci., 107, 887908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, B. S. (1950). “Effects of delayed speech feedback”, J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 22, 824826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, B. S. (1951). “Artificial stutter”, J. Speech Dis., 16, 5354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNemar, Q. (1957). “On Wilson's distribution-free test of analysis of variance hypotheses”, Psychol. Bull., 54, 361362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNemar, Q. (1958). “More on the Wilson test”, ibid., 55, 334335.Google Scholar
Sheffield, F. D. (1957). “Comment on a distribution-free factorial design analysis”, ibid., 54, 426428.Google Scholar
Siegal, S. (1956). Non-Parametric Statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Spilka, B. (1954). “Relationships between certain aspects of personality and some vocal effects of delayed auditory feedback”, J. Speech. Hear. Dis., 19, 491503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, K. V. (1956). “A distribution-free test of analysis of variance hypotheses”, Psychol. Bull., 53, 96101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yates, A. J. (1954). Ph.D. Thesis: quoted in: Shapiro, M. B., Campbell, D., Harris, A., and Dewsbury, J. B. (1958), “Effects of E.C.T. upon psychomotor speed and the ‘Distraction effect’ in depressed psychiatric patients”, J. Ment. Sci., 104, 681695.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.