Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T05:16:34.376Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

PHONOLOGICAL MEMORY PREDICTS SECOND LANGUAGE ORAL FLUENCY GAINS IN ADULTS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 October 2007

Irena O'Brien
Affiliation:
Université du Québec à Montréal
Norman Segalowitz
Affiliation:
Concordia University
Barbara Freed
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University
Joe Collentine
Affiliation:
Northern Arizona University

Abstract

This study investigated the relationship between phonological memory and second language (L2) fluency gains in native English-speaking adults learning Spanish in two learning contexts: at their home university or abroad in an immersion context. Phonological memory (operationalized as serial nonword recognition) and Spanish oral fluency (temporal/hesitation phenomena) were assessed at two times, 13 weeks apart. Hierarchical regressions showed that, after the variance attributable to learning context was partialed out, initial serial nonword recognition performance was significantly associated with L2 oral fluency development, explaining 4.5–9.7% of unique variance. These results indicate that phonological memory makes an important contribution to L2 learning in terms of oral fluency development. Furthermore, these results from an adult population extend conclusions from previous studies that have claimed a role for phonological memory primarily in vocabulary development in younger populations.This study was conducted by the first author as part of her doctoral dissertation research, under the supervision of Norman Segalowitz. The research was funded by a grant to Barbara Freed from the Council for International Educational Exchange (New York), in part by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to Norman Segalowitz, in part from the Dean's Office, Faculty of Arts and Science at Concordia University to Norman Segalowitz, and in part from the Fonds de recherche en santé du Québec to Irena O'Brien. The authors wish to thank Manuel Díaz-Campos, Barbara Lafford, and Nicole Lazar, who were members of the research team involved in the larger project of which this study is one part. The authors would also like to thank Peter Scherzer, Leif French, and two anonymous SSLA reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. Finally, the authors would like to thank Conchita Bueno, Hazel Casas, Elizabeth Gatbonton, Randall Halter, Guy Lacroix, Anne-Marie Linnen, Magnolia Negrete, Laura Renteria-Díaz, Marlene Taube-Schiff, and Naomi Yamasaki, who helped during various phases of this project.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, A.-M., Bourke, L., & Willis, C. (1999). Working memory and spoken language comprehension in young children. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 364373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, A.-M. & Gathercole, S.E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 403414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, A.-M. & Gathercole, S.E. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 216233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, A.-M. & Gathercole, S.E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 95116.Google Scholar
Atkins, P.W.B. & Baddeley, A.D. (1998). Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 537552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417423.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D., Gathercole, S.E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 8 (pp. 4790). San Diego: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A.D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. & Wilson, A.W. (2002). Prose recall and amnesia: Implications for the structure of working memory. Neuropsychologia, 40, 17371743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, J., Austin, W., Cannon, M., Lisus, A., & Vaughan, A. (1994). The relationship between memory span and measures of imitative and spontaneous language complexity in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 91107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breiner-Sanders, K., Lowe, P., Miles, J., & Swender, E. (2000). ACTFL proficiency guidelines: Speaking, revised 1999. Foreign Language Annals, 33, 1318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, G.D.A. & Hulme, C. (1992). Cognitive psychology and second language processing: The role of short-term memory. In R. J. Harris (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 105121). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Brown, G.D.A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107, 127181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, N. & Hitch, G.J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J.B. (1990). Cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude: Then and now. In T. Parry & C. Stansfield (Eds.), Language aptitude reconsidered (pp. 1129). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christoffels, I.K., de Groot, A.M.B., & Kroll, J.F. (2006). Memory and language skills in simultaneous interpreters: The role of expertise and language proficiency. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 324345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collentine, J. (2004). The effects of learning contexts on morphosyntactic and lexical development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 227248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collentine, J. & Freed, B.F. (2004). Learning context and its effects on second language acquisition: Introduction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 153171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cucchiarini, C., Strik, H., & Boves, L. (2002). Quantitative assessment of second language learners' fluency: Comparisons between read and spontaneous speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111, 28622873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derwing, T., Rossiter, M., Munro, M., & Thomson, R. (2004). Second language fluency: Judgments on different tasks. Language Learning, 54, 655769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Campos, M. (2004). Context of learning in the acquisition of Spanish second language phonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 249273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dufva, M. & Voeten, M.J.M. (1999). Native language literacy and phonological memory as prerequisites for learning English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 329348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., Szmalec, A., Kemps, E., & Vandierendonck, A. (2003). Verbal working memory is involved in associative word learning unless visual codes are available. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 527541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 91126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C. (2001). Memory for language. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 3368). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ellis, N.C. & Sinclair, S.G. (1996). Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: Putting language in good order. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 234250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freed, B.F. (1995). What makes us think that students who study abroad become fluent? In B. F. Freed (Ed.), Second language acquisition in a study abroad context (pp. 123148). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
French, L.M. (2006). Phonological working memory and L2 acquisition: A developmental study of Quebec francophone children learning English. New York: Edwin Mellen Press.
Frisch, S.A., Large, N.R., Zawaydeh, B., & Pisoni, D.B. (2001). Emergent phonotactic generalizations in English and Arabic. In J. Bybee & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 159179). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Gathercole, S.E. & Adams, A.-M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E. & Adams, A.-M. (1994). Children's phonological working memory: Contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 672688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E. & Baddeley, A.D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Hitch, G.J., Service, E., & Martin, A.J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S.M. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Service, E., Hitch, G.J., Adams, A.-M., & Martin, A.J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 6577.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A.D. (1991). Differentiating phonological working memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 387406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Willis, C.S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A.D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 12131236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jefferies, E., Frankish, C., & Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2006). Lexical and semantic influences on item and order memory in immediate serial recognition: Evidence from a novel task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 949964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klapp, S.T., Greim, D.M., & Marshburn, E.A. (1981). Buffer storage of programmed articulation and the articulatory loop: Two names for the same mechanism or two distinct components of short-term memory? In J. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 459472). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lafford, B.A. (2004). The effect of the context of learning on the use of communication strategies by learners of Spanish as a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 201225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazar, N.A. (2004). A short survey on causal inference, with implications for context of learning studies of second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 329347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E.V. & Gathercole, S.E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 383388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E.V. & Gathercole, S.E. (2005). Contrasting contributions of phonological short-term memory and long-term knowledge to vocabulary learning in a foreign language. Memory, 13, 422429.Google Scholar
Michas, I.C. & Henry, L.A. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 147163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical, narrative, and grammatical skills in second-language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 377402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A.D. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C. & Vallar, G. (1992). Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effect of phonological similarity and item length. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 4767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C. & Vallar, G. (1995). Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 98107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. (2003). Individual differences and instructed language learning. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 4673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romani, C., McAlpine, S., Olson, A., Tsouknida, E., & Martin, R. (2005). Length, lexicality, and articulatory suppression in immediate recall: Evidence against the articulatory loop. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 398415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N. (1997). Individual differences in second language acquisition. In A. M. B. de Groot & J. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism (pp. 85112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Segalowitz, N. & Freed, B.F. (2004). Context, contact and cognition in oral fluency acquisition: Learning Spanish in at home and study abroad contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 173199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 2150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E. & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155172.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. & Butterworth, B. (1977). Short-term memory impairment and spontaneous speech. Neuropsychologia, 15, 729735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sleve, R.L. & Miyake, A. (2006). Individual differences in second-language proficiency. Psychological Science, 17, 675681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, S. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, & Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speciale, G., Ellis, N.C., & Bywater, T. (2004). Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 293321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speidel, G.E. (1989). Imitation: A bootstrap for learning to speak? In G. E. Speidel & K. E. Nelson (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 151179). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Speidel, G.E. & Herreshoff, M.J. (1989). Imitation and the construction of long utterances. In G. E. Speidel & K. E. Nelson (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 181197). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Thorn, A.S.C. & Gathercole, S.E. (2001). Language differences in verbal short-term memory do not exclusively originate in the process of subvocal rehearsal. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 357364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorn, A.S.C., Gathercole, S.E., & Frankish, C.R. (2002). Language familiarity effects in short-term memory: The role of output delay and long-term knowledge. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 13631383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Towell, R. (2002). Relative degrees of fluency: A comparative case study of advanced learners of French. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 40, 117150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Towell, R., Hawkins, R., & Bazergui, N. (1996). The development of fluency in advanced learners of French. Applied Linguistics, 17, 84119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallar, G. & Baddeley, A.D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.N. & Lovatt, P. (2003). Phonological memory and rule learning. Language Learning, 53, 67121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willis, C.S. & Gathercole, S.E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory contributions to sentence processing in young children. Memory, 9, 349363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, D. (2001). In search of fluency: What is it and how can we teach it? Canadian Modern Language Review, 57, 573589.Google Scholar